page_content
stringlengths
39
150k
I went to a social gathering with a friend of mine recently. We talked with old friends, met some new people, and mingled, sampling conversations, mu-sic, food, and drink. As is our custom, we immediately shared our perceptions after leaving the party. “Did you notice how casually some people were dressed? ”my friend asked. Actually, I hadn ’t. I asked him what he thought of a man we had both met. “Wasn ’t he the most arrogant person? ”I asked. My friend hadn ’t seen anything to indicate so. As we continued to exchange im- pressions, I began to wonder if my friend had been at the same party interact- ing with the same people I had. I couldn’ t believe he hadn ’t noticed how weird the music was or realized how ill at ease the hostess seemed. My friend didn’ t understand how I had failed to recognize the architecture of the house or eventhe furniture I sat on. “I guess we learned one lesson, ”I said. “Never go to a party at their house again. ”My friend stared at me in disbelief. “Are you kid- ding?”he said. “I had a great time! ” How can two people participate in the same situation yet leave with very different impressions of what happened? The answer from the cognitive ap- proach to personality is that my friend and I have very different ways of pro-cessing information. Whereas I was attending to and processing information about the weirdness of the music and the arrogance of the guests, my friend entered the party prepared to notice clothing styles and furniture. Because we attended to different features of the party, we had very different perceptions of it and very different experiences. These different perceptions no doubt af- fected how we acted that night and how we will respond to future invitations. The cognitive approach explains differences in personality as differences in the way people process information. Because I have developed relativelystable ways of processing information in social settings, I probably respond to parties and other social gatherings in a similar way most of the time. Other people respond differently than I do because they consistently see something different from what I see. Cognitive models of personality have become popular in recent years, but they are not entirely new. An early predecessor can be found in Kurt Lewin ’s (1938) field theory of behavior. Lewin described the mental representationswe form of the important elements in our lives and how we organize those cognitive elements within our “life space. ”A more recent and, for the pur- poses of this book, more important cognitive personality theory was devel- oped by George Kelly. Since the publication of his book The Psychology of Personal Constructs in 1955, Kelly ’s work has evolved into a rich source of ideas for personality researchers and psychotherapists (Fransella, 2003,2005). It is interesting that Kelly did not think of himself as a cognitive psy- chologist. “I have been so puzzled over the early labeling of [my] theory as cognitive, ”he wrote, “that several years ago I set out to write another short book to make it clear that I wanted no part of cognitive theory ”(1969, p. 216). Despite his protests, Kelly ’s writings have become the starting point for many of the approaches to personality we now identify as “cognitive.”The Cognitive Approach 411
PERSONAL CONSTRUCT THEORY George Kelly ’s approach to personality begins with a unique conception of hu- mankind. He called it a man-the-scientist perspective. Like scientists, people constantly generate and test hypotheses about their world. Just as scientists try to predict and control the things they study, we all want to predict and control as many events in our lives as possible. Not knowing why things happen or how the people around us might act can be unsettling. So to satisfy our need for predictability, we engage in a process Kelly compared to template matching. That is, our ideas about the world are similar to transparent templates. Weplace these templates over the events we encounter. If they match, we retain the templates. If not, we modify them for a better prediction next time. For ex- ample, based on past observations, you may have generated a few hypothesesabout one of your instructors. One hypothesis is that this man is stuffy and arrogant. Whenever you see this instructor, you collect more information and compare the new data with your hypothesis. If it is verified (the instructor actsthe way stuffy people act), you continue using it. If not (outside of the class- room he is warm and charming), you discard the hypothesis and replace it with a new one. The process resembles the one used by scientists who retainand reject hypotheses based on empirical findings. Kelly called the cognitive structures we use to interpret and predict events personal constructs . No two people use identical personal constructs, and no two people organize their constructs in an identical manner. What do these constructs look like? Kelly described them as bipolar. That is, we classify rele- vant objects in an either or fashion within our constructs. When I meet someone for the first time, I might apply the personal constructs friendly – unfriendly, tall –short, intelligent –unintelligent, and masculine –feminine in constructing an image of this person. I might decide that this person isfriendly, tall, intelligent, and feminine. But this does not mean that we see the world as black and white with no shades of gray. After applying our first construct, we often use other bipolar constructs to determine the extent ofthe blackness or whiteness. For example, after determining that this new acquaintance is intelligent, I might then apply an academically intelligent – commonsense intelligent construct to get an even clearer picture of what this person is like. How can personal constructs be used to explain personality? Kelly main- tained that differences in personality result largely from differences in the way people “construe the world. ”If you and I interact with Jacob, I might use friendly –unfriendly, fun –boring, and outgoing –shyconstructs in forming my impression. But you might interpret Jacob in terms of refined –gross, sensitive – insensitive, and intelligent –unintelligent constructs. After we both talk to Jacob for a while, I might act as if I ’m interacting with a friendly, fun, and outgoing person. You might respond to Jacob as if dealing with a gross, insensitive, andunintelligent person. We ’re both in the same situation, but because we interpret that situation very differently, we respond in very different ways. In addition,because I tend to use these same constructs when meeting other individuals, Iprobably have a characteristic way I interact with people that is different from“Id on o tr e g a r dm y career in psychology as a ‘calling. ’ Everything aroundus‘calls, ’if we choose to heed. Itwas I who got myselfinto it and I who have pursued it. ” George Kelly412 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
yours. In other words, the relatively stable patterns in our behavior are the re- sult of the relatively stable way we construe the world. Personal Construct Systems To get a rough idea of your own personal constructs, ask yourself what youtend to notice about people when you first meet them. The first few thoughts that come to mind are probably some of the constructs you typically use to make sense of other people and their behavior. It is also possible that two people use the same constructs but construe the world differently. That is, I might think someone intelligent, and you might see the same person as unin- telligent. Further, two people ’s constructs might be similar on one pole but not the other. I might use an outgoing –reserved construct, whereas you use anoutgoing –melancholy construct. If that were the case, what I see as re- served behavior you might see as sadness. One reason you and I act differently from each other is that we use differ- ent constructs. Another reason is that we organize our constructs differently.After I determine that a new acquaintance appears friendly, I might want toGeorge Kelly 1905 –1967 George Alexander Kelly was born in a farming commu-nity near Wichita, Kansas, in1905. He attended FriendsUniversity in Wichita for 3 years before graduatingfrom Park College inMissouri in 1926. He was an active member of the intercollegiate debate teamduring these years and developed a keen ability tochallenge arguments and conventional positions.Although these skills would eventually become anasset, they may have kept him away from the field ofpsychology for many years. Kelly described his first psychology course as boring and unconvincing. Theinstructor spent considerable time discussing learningtheories, but Kelly was unimpressed. “The most I could make of it was that the S was what you had to have in order to account for the R, and the R was put there so the S would have something to account for, ”he wrote. “I never did find out what that arrow stood for ”(1969, pp. 46 –47). He was also skeptical when he first read Freud. “I don’t remember which one of Freud’ s books I was trying to read, ”he recalled, “but I do rememberthe mounting feeling of incredulity that anyone could write such nonsense, much less publish it ”(p. 47). After graduating with a degree in physics and mathematics, Kelly went to the University of Kansasto study educational sociology. After a series of odd jobs, including teaching speech and working as anaeronautical engineer, he went to the University ofEdinburgh to study education in 1929. While there,he developed a growing interest in psychology andreceived his PhD in psychology from the University ofIowa a few years later. Kelly spent the next 10 years at Fort Hays Kansas State College. During this time he set up a network ofclinics to provide psychological services to the poor anddestitute Dustbowl victims of the 1930s. “I listened to people in trouble, ”he wrote, “and tried to help them figure out what they could do about it” (p. 50). He soon came to see that what these people needed most was an explanation for what had happened to them and the ability to predict what would happen to them in the future. Personal construct theory evolved fromthis insight. After serving in the Navy in World War II,Kelly spent a year at the University of Maryland andthen 20 years at Ohio State University. He moved toBrandeis University in 1965 and died soon after. National Library of MedicinePersonal Construct Theory 413
know if the person is outgoing or quiet. We could diagram the relation be- tween my constructs this way: Friendly –Unfriendly Outgoing– Quiet Note that within this construct system, I could not see an unfriendly personas either outgoing or quiet, just unfriendly. On the other hand, you might use the same constructs but organize them this way: Friendly –Unfriendly Outgoing –Quiet Outgoing –Quiet In this case, whether you judge someone as friendly or unfriendly, you canstill judge that person as either outgoing or quiet. Of course, it is also possible to organize these same two constructs this way: Outgoing –Quiet Friendly –Unfriendly Friendly –Unfriendly In this case, after deciding someone is a quiet person, you might want to know if she is a quiet– friendly person or a quiet –unfriendly one. In short, not only do we use a limitless number of constructs to make sense of ourworld, but the ways we organize and use these constructs also are practically endless. Psychological Problems Like many personality theorists, Kelly was a practicing psychotherapist who applied his ideas about personality to treating psychological problems.However, unlike many theorists, Kelly rejected the notion that psychological disorders are caused by past traumatic experiences. Rather, he argued, people suffer from psychological problems because of defects in their construct sys- tems. Past experiences with an unloving parent or a tragic incident may ex- plain why people construe the world as they do, but they are not the cause of the person’ s problems. Kelly placed anxiety at the heart of most psychological problems. We be- come anxious when our personal constructs fail to make sense of the events in our lives. We have all had this experience on occasion. An upcoming inter-view will cause more anxiety if you have no idea who you will meet or what kind of questions you will be asked. Similarly, when you can ’t understand414 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
why certain people treat you the way they do or you don ’t know how to be- have in certain situations, you probably feel confused, disoriented, and anxi- ous. Relationship problems are particularly unsettling when you don’ t know why things are going poorly and have no idea how to put the relationship back on track. The problem is that construct systems are never perfect. For a variety of reasons, our constructs occasionally fail us. Most of the time, we simply gener- ate a new construct to replace the inadequate one. If you anticipate that a con-versation with Anna is going to be boring but then find it interesting, you will probably alter your expectations for future encounters with Anna. But failure to consider this new information lessens your ability to predict what will hap-pen the next time you interact with Anna. You may have experienced this frus- tration when you said to someone, “Ij u s td o n ’t understand you anymore. ” COGNITIVE PERSONALITY VARIABLES In the early days of behaviorism (Chapter 13), psychologists sometimes used a“black box ”metaphor to describe the relationship between stimuli and re- sponses. In this model, features in the environment (e.g., a loud noise) causebehaviors (e.g., running away). But what happens inside the organism be- tween the stimulus and response is unknown and unknowable, i.e., the black box. In contrast, it is exactly the elements between stimulus and response that are of greatest interest to cognitive personality psychologists. In recent years,these psychologists have introduced a large number of cognitive variables to account for individual differences (Mischel & Shoda, 1995, 2008; Shoda, Tiernan, & Mischel, 2002). Some of these cognitive variables, sometimescalled cognitive-affective units, are shown in Table 15.1. TABLE 15.1 Cognitive-Affective Units Encodings Categories (constructs) for encoding informa- tion about one ’s self, other people, events, and situations Expectations and Beliefs Expectations for what will happen in certain situations, for outcomes for certain behaviors, and for one ’s personal efficacy Affects Feelings, emotions, and emotional responses Goals and Values Individual goals and values, and life projects Competencies and Self-RegulatoryPlansPerceived abilities, plans, and strategies forchanging and maintaining one ’s behavior and internal states Source: From Kelly, George, A. A Theory of Personality: The Psychology of Personal Constructs . Copyright © 1955, 1963 by George A. Kelly, renewed 1983, 1991 by Gladys Kelly. Used by permission of W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.Cognitive Personality Variables 415
These cognitive variables are part of a complex system that links the situations we encounter with our behavior. An oversimplified illustration of this process is shown in Figure 15.1. How we react to features in the environ- ment, and even whether we notice these features, depends on our cognitive structures. Once perceived, various mental representations, such as expecta- tions, values and goals, interact with one another to determine how we respond to the situation. Notice also that, as in some of the social learning models, ourbehavior can then affect the situation. How do we explain individual differences within this cognitive frame- work? The answer is that each of us possesses a different set of mental repre-sentations. In addition, how easily we access certain kinds of information stored in memory varies from individual to individual. As a result, two people often react to the same situation differently. What one person hears as aclever retort someone else might take as an insult. A Christmas tree will re- mind one person of religious values, another of family and seasonal joy, and a third of sad memories from childhood. Schemas Let’s now return to the scene at the beginning of this chapter —the one in which my friend and I came away from the party with completely different impressions. Although we were exposed to essentially the same people andi jhgfedcbaFeatures of SituationsCognitive- Affective System Behaviors FIGURE 15.1 Cognitive Model of Personality Source: From “A Cognitive-Affective System Theory of Personality: Reconceptualizing Situations, Disposition, Dynamics, and Invariance in Personality Structure, ”by W. Mischel and Y. Shoda, Psychological Review , 1995, 102, 246 –148. Copyright © 1995 American Psychological Association.416 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
events, our experiences were quite different. One explanation for our different reactions is that my friend and I were using different schemas. Schemas are hypothetical cognitive structures that help us perceive, organize, process, and use information. Because there are so many stimuli to attend to in most situations, we need some way to make sense of the mass confusion around us. Imagine what the world must look like to a baby — what psychologist William James once referred to as a “buzzing, blooming confusion. ”The baby does not know what in all this confusion to pay atten- tion to and what to ignore. Of course, the mass of stimuli doesn ’t go away. Think about all the sounds and sights bombarding your senses at this verymoment. Fortunately, each of us has developed systems for identifying and at-tending to what is important and ignoring the rest. One of the main functions of schemas is to help us perceive features in our environment. Naturally, when something extremely important happensor someone possesses an attention-grabbing feature, everyone notices. If a 7- foot-tall man attends a party, everyone makes note of his height. But less con- spicuous features of an environment will probably not be noticed unless weenter the situation with a readiness to process that information. Actually, I seldom notice how tall people are. However, a friend of mine is very aware of other people’ s height (she is short). In schema terms, the reason she pays attention to height is that she has a well-developed schema for processing this information. Because I use different schemas to process information about others, she and I often have different impressions of people. Beyond helping to perceive certain features in our environment, schemas provide us with a structure within which to organize and process informa-tion. For example, I can incorporate a new piece of information about my mother into my existing knowledge of her because I have a well-defined mother schema. I can give you a well-organized description of her because the information is organized into one well-formed cognitive structure rather than scattered about as bits of information in various unrelated schemas. I also should be able to process information about my mother more readily than information about a woman I have never met. When asked if my motheris sociable, I should be able to answer more quickly than if asked whether the queen of England is sociable. Without a strong schema for the queen, it will take me longer to process whatever information I might have about her.Moreover, because my mother schema provides me with a framework within which to process and organize information, it is easier for me to use this in-formation. I should be able to recall information about my mother morereadily than information stored loosely in my memory. COGNITIVE REPRESENTATIONS OF THE SELF Of all the cognitive structures that organize and store memories, the most impor- tant mental representation is probably the one most unique to you. Beginning at a very early age, each of us develops a cognitive representation of ourselves. Psychologists sometimes refer to this representation as our self-concept. As with other personality constructs, researchers f ind that our self-concepts are relativelyCognitive Representations of the Self 417
stable over time (Markus & Kunda, 1986). Moreover, research indicates that cognitive representations of the self p lay a central role in the way we process in- formation and thus in how we interact with the world around us. Self-Schemas Surveys tell us that most Americans believe exercise is good for their physical and mental health. The majority of adults periodically take up jogging, swim- ming, aerobic dancing, or some other type of exercise program. However, alarge number of people rarely, if ever, exercise. And about half of those who begin an exercise program quit within the first year. Why do some people succeed in making exercise a part of their lives, whereas others fail? One ex-planation has to do with whether the would-be exerciser incorporates exercise into his or her self-schema. Self-schemas are cognitive representations of ourselves that we use to or- ganize and process self-relevant information (Markus, 1977, 1983). Your self- schema consists of the behaviors and attributes that are most important to you. Because each part of your life is not equally important, not everythingyou do becomes part of your self-schema. If both you and I occasionally play baseball and write poetry, we can ’t assume that these two activities play an equally important role in our self-schemas. Baseball might be an important part of how I think of myself, but not poetry, whereas the opposite might be the case for you. If you could see your self-schema, what would it look like? An example is shown in Figure 15.2. Basic information about you makes up the core of yourself-schema. This includes your name, information about your physical ap- pearance, and information about your relationships with significant people, such as with a spouse or parents. Although different for each of us, these SelfMemory MenParentsWine Clothing SchoolBasketballGeographyReligion FIGURE 15.2 Example of a Self-Schema Diagram418 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
basic elements are found in nearly everyone ’s self-schema. More interesting to personality psychologists are the unique features within your self-schema (Markus & Sentis, 1982; Markus & Smith, 1981). Returning to the exercise question, some people include athlete orphysically fit in their self-schemas. Another way of saying this is that these i ndividuals consider their athletic activities a part of who they are. Researchers find that people who incorpo- rate such identities into their self-schemas are more likely to stick with regu- lar exercise programs than those who do not (Kendzierski, 1988, 1990).When exercising becomes a part of who you are, you are much less likely to give it up when the weather turns bad or you experience a few aches and pains. Trait concepts, such as independence or friendliness, can also be part of your self-schema. That is, you might think of yourself as a friendly person. Ifthat is the case, you frequently evaluate your behavior by asking yourself,“Was that a friendly thing to do? ”However, it might never occur to me to evaluate my actions in terms of friendliness. In this example, friendliness is a feature of your self-schema, but not mine. Because the elements that consti-tute self-schemas vary from person to person, we process information about ourselves differently. And because of these individual differences in self- schemas, we behave differently. In one study, elementary school children with prosocial as a part of their self-schemas were more likely to give valu- able tokens to others than children who did not include prosocial as part of their self-concepts (Froming, Nasby, & McManus, 1998). In another investi-gation, men and women whose self-schemas included sexuality reported higher levels of sexual desire and stronger romantic attachments than those whose self-schemas did not include sexuality (Andersen, Cyranowski, & Espindle, 1999; Cyranowski & Andersen, 2000). One team of researchers found that Latino Americans were more likely than White Americans to in- clude simpatico (an interpersonal style emphasizing hospitality and gracious- ness) in their self-schemas (Holloway, Waldrip, & Ickes, 2009). Participants with simpatico as part of their self-schema were found to interact with others in a warmer, more engaged style. At this point, you may be asking how psychologists determine what a person ’s self-schema looks like. Although examining something as abstract as self-schemas presents a challenge, cognitive personality researchers have de- veloped some creative procedures to test their hypotheses. Essentially, these psychologists look at how people perceive and use information presented to them. For example, answer the following question yes or no: Are you a com-petitive person? When faced with this question on a personality inventory, some people answer immediately and decisively, whereas others have to pause to think about what it means to be competitive and whether they possessthose qualities. In taking the various personality tests in this book, you prob- ably found some items were easy to answer and some for which you simply couldn ’t make up your mind. According to a self-schema analysis, the items that were easy to answer are those for which you have a well-defined schema. People who say yes immediately when asked if they are competitive have a strong competitive schema that is part of their self-schema. The schemaCognitive Representations of the Self 419
enables them to understand the question and respond immediately. People without a strong competitive schema are unable to process the information as quickly. Much of the early research on self-schemas was based on this reasoning. Participants in one study were classified as possessing either a strong indepen- dence schema or a strong dependence schema or as aschematic (Markus, 1977). Later these participants were presented with a series of adjectives ona computer screen. Their task was to press either a ME or a NOT ME buttonto indicate whether the adjective described them. Fifteen of the adjectives were related to independence (for example, individualistic, outspoken ) and 15 to dependence (for example, conforming, submissive). As Figure 15.3 shows, people with strong independence schemas pressed the ME button quickly on the independence-related adjectives but took longer to respond on the dependence-related adjectives. Participants with strong dependence sche- mas responded in the opposite pattern. Aschematics showed no difference in making these judgments for any of the words. Researchers find similar results when they divide participants along other personality dimensions (Shah &Higgins, 2001). 2.02.5Latency in SecondsDependent Adjectives Independent AdjectivesWords Judged Self-Descriptive 2.02.5Latency in SecondsDependent Adjectives Independent AdjectivesWords Judged Not Self-DescriptiveDependentsParticipants: Aschematics Independents FIGURE 15.3 Mean Response Latencies for Adjectives Source: From “Self-schemata and processing information about the self, ”by H. Markus, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1977, 35, 63 –78. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association.420 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
In addition to allowing for rapid processing of schema-relevant information, self-schemas provide a framework for organizing and storing this information.Consequently, we would expect people to retrieve information from memory more readily when they have a strong schema for a topic than when the information is stored in a less organized manner. To test this hypothesis, researchers presented college students with a series of 40 questions on a computer screen (Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977). Participants answered each question by pressing a YES or a NO button as quickly as possible. Thirty of thequestions were written so that people could answer easily without using their self-schemas to process the information. For these questions, participants simply answered whether a word was printed in big letters, whether it rhymedwith another word, or whether it meant the same thing as another word. However, for 10 questions participants had to decide whether the word described them. That is, they had to process the information through theirself-schemas. What the participants were not told was that afterward they would be asked to recall as many of the 40 words as possible. As shown in Figure 15.4, when participants answered questions about themselves, they were more likely to remember the information than when the question was processed in other ways. The researchers point to this finding as evidence for a self- schema. When asked whether a word describes them, participants processed the question through their self-schemas. Because information in our self- schemas is easy to access, the self-referent words were easier to remember 123Number of Words Recalled Big letters?Rhymes with?Means same as?Describes you? FIGURE 15.4 Mean Number of Words Recalled as a Function of Cue Question Source: From Rogers, et al. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 35, 677 –688. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association.Cognitive Representations of the Self 421
than those not processed through self-schemas. But might this finding be ex- plained in other ways? Could it be that the self-referent question was simplyharder than the other questions, thus causing participants to think about it more? Apparently not. When people are asked if a word describes a celebrity, they don’ t recall the words as well as when they are asked about themselves (Lord, 1980). Possible Selves Suppose two college students, Denise and Carlos, receive an identical poor grade in a course on deductive logic and argumentation. Neither of them is pleased with the grade, but Denise quickly dismisses it as a bad semester,whereas Carlos frets about the grade for weeks. Denise turns her attention to the next term, but Carlos looks over his final exam several times and thinks about taking another course in this area. Although many explanations canbe suggested to account for the two students ’different reactions, a key piece of information may be that Carlos is thinking about going to law school andbecoming a trial attorney someday, but Denise is not. A negative evaluationof his deductive logic and argumentation skills means something quite differ- ent to Carlos than it does to Denise. Our behavior is directed not only by cognitive representations of the way we think of ourselves at the moment, but also by representations of what wemight become. You might think about a future self with a lot of friends, with a medical degree, or with a physically fit body. Psychologists refer to these images as our possible selves (Markus & Nurius, 1986; Ruvolo & Markus, 1992; Vignoles, Manzi, Regalia, Jemmolo, & Scabini, 2008). Possible selves are cognitive representations of the kind of person we might become some- day. These include roles and occupations we aspire to, such as police officer or community leader, as well as the roles we fear we might fall into, such as alcoholic or divorced parent. Possible selves also include the attributes we think we might possess in the future, such as being a warm and loving per- son, an overworked and underappreciated employee, or a contributor to soci- ety. In a sense, possible selves represent our dreams and aspirations as well as our fears and anxieties. Like other personality constructs, possible selves are fairly stable over time (Frazier, Hooker, Johnson, & Kaus, 2000; Morfei, Hooker, Fiese, & Cordeiro, 2001). Possible selves serve two important functions (Markus & Nurius, 1986). First, they provide incentives for future behavior. When making decisions, we ask ourselves whether a choice will take us closer to or further awayfrom one of our future selves. A woman might enter an MBA program be- cause this decision moves her closer to becoming her powerful business exe- cutive possible self. A man might stop seeing old friends if he thinks the association could lead him to the criminal self he fears he might become. The second function of possible selves is to help us interpret the meaning of our behavior and the events in our lives. A man with a professional base- ball pitcher possible self will attach a very different meaning to an arm injury than someone who does not think of himself this way. A woman with a pos- sible self of cancer patient will react differently to small changes in her health422 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
than someone without this cognitive representation. In other words, we pay more attention to and have a stronger emotional reaction to events that arerelevant to our possible selves. Because possible selves guide many of our choices and reactions, they can be useful in predicting future behavior. Researchers have used measures ofpossible selves to look at such varied behaviors as binge drinking (Quinlan, Jaccard, & Blanton, 2006), academic performance (Oyserman et al., 2006), weight loss (Granberg, 2006), and adherence to an exercise program(Ouellette, Hessling, Gibbons, Reis-Bergan, & Gerrard, 2005). Other research has tied possible selves to problem behaviors. One team of investigators ex- amined possible selves in juvenile delinquents (Oyserman & Markus, 1990;Oyserman & Saltz, 1993). Significantly, more than one-third of the juvenile delinquents had developed a criminal possible self. In addition, very few of these adolescents possessed possible selves for more conventional goals, such as having a job. Because possible selves are indicative of our goals, fears, and aspirations, we should not be surprised if many of these youthful offenders were to become adult criminals. Researchers also find gender differences in the possible selves of young men and young women. In particular, female high school and college students are less likely than males to see themselves in traditionally masculine roles in the future. University women are less likely than men to have a possible self that includes a career in a math, science or business (Lips, 2004). Male stu- dents are less likely than women to see themselves in careers related to arts, culture, and communication. However, research also points to solutions for these gender discrepancies. High school girls who have female friends inter- ested in science and who receive encouragement from their peers are more likely to develop a scientist possible self (Stake & Nickens, 2005). Same- gender role models also help. High school girls who see women scientistsand business leaders are more likely to incorporate these occupations intotheir possible selves. People are more optimistic about reaching their hoped- for occupation when they know someone already in that position (Robinson, Davis, & Meara, 2003). Self Discrepancies While reading the previous section, you may have been struck by the simi- larities between possible selves and Carl Rogers’ description of the real-self and ideal-self (Chapter 11). Rogers described the difficulties people face when the person they are is very different from the person they would like to be. Cognitive personality psychologists have also explored the relation between different self-concepts. One approach, called self-discrepancy the- ory, proposes three different cognitive representations of self (Higgins, 1987, 1989). First, each of us possesses an actual self. The actual self con- tains all the information you have about the kind of person you are (or be- lieve you are), similar to the notion of self-concept used by other personalitypsychologists. Second, you also possess an ideal self, w h i c hi sy o u rm e n t a l image of the kind of person you would like to be. The ideal self includesyour dreams and aspirations and the goals you have set for yourself in life.Cognitive Representations of the Self 423
Third, there is the ought self. This is the self you believe you should be, the kind of person who fulfills all the duties and obligations various sources (parents, religion) have defined for you. Your ought self might be a devoted parent, a patriotic citizen, or someone who gets involved in community activities. According to the theory, we often compare the way we act (our actual self) with the way we want to be (ideal self) or the way we should be (ought self). Not surprisingly, we often fall short in these comparisons.Discrepancies between our actual self and ideal self result in disappointment, dejection, and sometimes sadness. This is the reaction of a would-be honors student who becomes lax in his or her study habits and receives low grades.On the other hand, discrepancies between the actual self and the ought self lead to agitation, anxiety, and guilt. These are the emotions we might expect when we act selfishly or take advant age of someone, in contrast with the generous and kind person we think we ought to be. As with other cognitive processes, this comparison is said to take place outside of conscious aware- ness. Thus you can experience sadness or guilt without being aware of whyyou feel that way. Researchers find support for many of the predictions generated from self-discrepancy theory. In particu lar, individuals made aware of a gap be- tween their actual and ideal self often experience sadness, whereas those with discrepant actual and ought selves suffer from anxiety (Cornette, Strauman, Abramson, & Busch, 2008; Hardin & Lakin, 2009; Petrocelli & Smith, 2005; Phillips & Silva, 2005; Renaud & McConnell, 2007). Researchers also find that, like other p ersonality variables, measures of an adult’s ideal-self and ought-self are fairly consistent over time (Strauman, 1996). Although little is known about how self-discrepancies affect emotions in other cultures, one study found Japanese undergraduates had larger discrepancies between their actual and ideal selves than did Canadianstudents (Heine & Lehman, 1999). Interestingly, this increase in self- criticism by the Japanese students did not translate into higher levels of depression. APPLICATION: COGNITIVE ( BEHAVIOR) PSYCHOTHERAPY The increased attention given to cognitive structures by personality research- ers in recent years has been paralleled by the growing popularity of cognitive approaches to psychotherapy. As described in Chapter 13, today many thera-pists combine cognitive approaches to therapy with procedures from traditional behavioral therapies. But whether they call themselves cognitive therapists or cognitive-behavior therapists, each identifies inappropriate thoughts as a causeof mood disorders and self-defeating behavior. People become anxious and depressed because they harbor anxiety-provoking and depressing thoughts. Consequently, the goal of most cognitive therapies is to help clients recognizeinappropriate thoughts and replace them with more appropriate ones. A cog- nitive therapist ’s role usually falls somewhere between that of the intrusive Freudian therapist and the Rogerian therapist who relies on the client for“The best scientist is one who approaches his subject [as] inti- mately as a clinician…and the best clini- cian is one who in-vites his client to joinhim in a controlled investigation of life. ” George Kelly424 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
clinical progress. Although clients must come to see how their cognitions affect their emotions and behaviors, the therapist plays an active role in theprocess. In addition to addressing current issues, cognitive psychologists often teach clients how to deal with future and recurring problems (Meichenbaum &Deffenbacher, 1988). One cause of recurring problems is self-defeating think- ing(Meichenbaum & Cameron, 1983). A man who suffers from shyness probably approaches a party telling himself something like this: “I don ’t know why I ’m going to this dumb party. No one ever wants to talk with me. And when they do, I usually sound awkward and stupid. ”This man has set himself up to fail. At the first awkward moment, he will conclude that thingsare going as poorly as anticipated. All the nervousness and embarrassment he dreaded are likely to follow. What can be done for this man? A cognitive therapist might try to re- place these self-defeating thoughts with more appropriate, positive ones. This is not to say the man should unrealistically expect that everything will go well. Rather, he should be prepared for some disappointments and failuresand learn to interpret these in appropriate ways. Some psychologists compare this process to inoculation. Like a medical vaccine that prevents a patient from becoming ill, the treatment is designed to keep negative thoughts from creating undue psychological distress. Like any approach to treatment, cognitive psychotherapy does not work for everyone and may be limited to psychological problems thatare based in irrational and self-defeat ing thinking. Nonetheless, the suc- cess many therapists have had with t his approach has been encouraging (Butler, Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006; Gaudiano, 2005). Cognitive-behavioral therapies have been found t o be especially effective for treat- ing emotional disorders like depressi on and anxiety (Hollon, Stewart, & Strunk, 2006; Vittengl, Clark, Dunn, & Jarrett, 2007). Not only do thesetreatments relieve the symptoms of e motional disorders, but people who have gone through cognitive-behavioral therapies are less likely to experi-ence a relapse in the future. Presumably these individuals have learnedhow to identify unhealthy thoughts and how to replace them with posi- tive ones. Rational Emotive Therapy One of the earliest advocates of cogni tive therapy was Albert Ellis, who de- veloped rational emotive therapy . Consistent with the tendency to blend cognitive and behavioral treatments, r ecently Ellis referr ed to his approach asrational emotive behavior therapy (Ellis, 2003). According to Ellis, peo- ple become depressed, anxious, upse t, and the like because of faulty rea- soning and a reliance on irrational be liefs. Ellis described this as an A-B-C process. For example, suppose your boyfriend/girlfriend calls tonight and tells you the relationship is over . This is the A, which Ellis calls the Activating experience . However, when clients seek out psychotherapy, they usually identify the reason as the C, the emotional Consequence .I n this case, you are probably depressed, guilty, or angry. But how did youApplication: Cognitive (Behavior) Psychotherapy 425
logically get from A to C? Why should a personal setback or loss cause such strong negative emotions? The answer is that you have used a middlestep in this sequence, B —the irrational Belief. The only way you could logically conclude from breaking up with your partner that you should be depressed is that you are also saying to yourself something like “It is nec- essary for me to be loved and approved by virtually every person in my life,”or“I can never be happy without this person.” Of course, when iso- lated like this, the belief is obviously irrational. But these irrational beliefs are so entrenched in our thoughts that it often takes professional help tosee the flaws in our thinking. Ellis maintained that each of us harbors and relies on a large number of these irrational beliefs. Imagine that you fail an important class (A). If youthen fall back on the irrational belief “I need to do well at everything to be considered worthwhile ”(B), you’ ll lead yourself to the conclusion that this is a catastrophe and therefore become excessively anxious (C). A rational emo- tive therapist would point out that, whereas failing is certainly an unfortunate event—and something you ’d prefer didn ’t happen —it does not warrant ex- treme anxiety. Expecting everything to work out well all the time will only lead to disappointment and frustration. Some of the more commonly used ir- rational beliefs are listed in Table 15.2. Ellis (1987) maintained that some of these beliefs are blatantly irrational and therefore easily identified and cor- rected during therapy. However, other beliefs are more subtle or trickier and thus are more resistant to change. The goal of rational emotive therapy is twofold. First, clients must see how they rely on irrational beliefs and thereby identify the fault in their rea-soning. Second, the therapist works with the client to replace irrational beliefs with rational ones. For example, instead of deciding that your romantic breakup is a reason to be depressed, you might tell yourself that, although you enjoy a stable romantic relationship and wish this one could have con-tinued, you know that not all relationships work out. You also know that this doesn ’t mean no one else can love you or that you are never going to have a good relationship again. Thus, whereas the A statement is the same—“I broke up with my partner ”—the B statement is different. Because the situation is identified as unpleasant but not catastrophic, there is no needto become overly depressed, the old C. In the following sample, taken from one of Ellis ’therapy sessions with a young woman (Ellis, 1971), you can see how a rational emotive thera-pist tries to change faulty thoughts: Client: Well, this is all a part of something that’ s bothered me for a long time. I ’m always afraid of making a mistake. Ellis: Why? What ’s the horror? C: I don’t know. E: You’re saying that you ’re a bitch, you ’re a louse when you make a mistake.426 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
C: But this is the way I ’ve always been. Every time I make a mistake, I die a thousand deaths over it. E: You blame yourself. But why? What ’s the horror? Is it going to make you better next time? Is it going to make you make fewer mistakes? C: No. E: Then why blame yourself? Why are you a louse for making a mistake? Who said so? C: I guess it ’s one of those feelings I have. E: One of those beliefs. The belief is: “I am a louse! ”And then you get the feeling: “Oh, how awful! How shameful! ”But the feeling follows the belief. And again, you ’re say- ing,“I should be different; I shouldn’ t make mistakes! ”instead of, “Oh, look: I made a mistake. It ’s undesirable to make mistakes. Now, how am I going to stop making one next time? ”… C: It might all go back to, as you said, the need for approval. If I don ’t make mistakes, then people will look up to me. If I do it all perfectly —TABLE 15.2 Some Common Irrational Beliefs Obvious Irrational Beliefs Because I strongly desire to perform important tasks competently and successfully, I absolutely must perform them well at all times. Because I strongly desire to be approved by people I find significant, I absolutely must always have their approval. Because I strongly desire people to treat me considerately and fairly, they absolutely must at all times and under all conditions do so. Because I strongly desire to have a safe, comfortable, and satisfying life, the condi- tions under which I live absolutely must at all times be easy, convenient, andgratifying. Subtle and Tricky Irrational Beliefs Because I strongly desire to perform important tasks competently and successfully, and because I want to succeed at them only some of the time, I absolutely mustperform these tasks well. Because I strongly desire to be approved by people I find significant, and because I only want a little approval from them, I absolutely must have it. Because I strongly desire people to treat me considerately and fairly, and because I am almost always considerate and fair to others, they absolutely must treat me well. Because I strongly desire to have a safe, comfortable, and satisfying life, and be- cause I am a nice person who tries to help others lead this kind of life, the condi-tions under which I live absolutely must be easy, convenient, and gratifying. Source: From “The impossibility of achieving consistently good mental health, ”by A. Ellis, American Psychologist , 1987, 42, 364 –375. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association.Application: Cognitive (Behavior) Psychotherapy 427
E: Yes, that ’s part of it. That is the erroneous belief: that if you never make mistakes ev- erybody will love you and that it is necessary that they do. …But is it true? Suppose you never did make mistakes —would people love you? They ’d sometimes hate your guts, wouldn ’t they? Rational emotive therapists challenge clients to identify their irrational beliefs and see how these beliefs lead them to their faulty conclusions. Of course, thisis not easy. Most of us can readily identify what ’s wrong with our friends ’ thinking, but it ’s quite another matter when we ’re the ones with an emotional problem. Nonetheless, the success of rational emotive therapy with a largenumber of clients has contributed to the increased popularity of cognitive approaches to psychotherapy in recent years. ASSESSMENT: THE REPERTORY GRID TECHNIQUE George Kelly made personal constructs the key concept in his theory of personality as well as the focus of his approach to psychotherapy. But this emphasis created a bit of a problem. Specifically, how does one go about measuring a person’ s personal constructs? Of course, a therapist might obtain some idea of a client ’s construct system during the course of therapy sessions. But Kelly and his colleagues needed a more efficient way to examine con-struct systems that could then be communicated fairly easily to the client.Kelly’s answer was the Repertory Grid Technique. Kelly and his followers de- veloped several variations of this technique (Fransella, Bell, & Bannister,2003), but the essential procedure consists of two steps (Bell, 1990). First,the test taker creates a list of elements. The items on this list can be anything the individual encounters in life, but most often the list consists of specificpeople the test taker knows. Second, the test taker’ s personal constructs are elicited by comparing and contrasting various elements on the list. The most common version of the grid technique is the Role Construct Repertory Test, or more commonly, the Rep Test. A shortened version of the basic Rep Test procedure is presented on pages 433 –434. Therapists begin by asking clients to provide a list of 24 people from various personal experiences —for example, a teacher they liked, the most interesting person they know, and so on. The therapist then presents clients with three of the names from this list and asks, “In what important way are two of these peo- ple alike but different from the third?” A client might say that two of them arewarm people and that the third person is cold. In Kelly ’s terms, this client has used a warm– cold construct to categorize the three people. The process is repeated with three different names from the list. Perhaps this time the client will divide the people along an outgoing –shyor a generous –miserly construct. Kelly suggested that about 20 trials, or “sorts,”provide the therapist with a useful sample of the client ’s principal constructs. In one variation of the Rep Test, the therapist takes away one of the three names and replaces it with a new one. This procedure can be useful in identi-fying clients ’difficulties in applying new constructs to new situations. To ex- amine self-concepts, therapists sometimes present the client ’s name along with428 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
two names from the list. Again, clients are asked how two of the three are alike and one is different. Many therapists take the list of constructs gener- ated from the client ’s initial Rep Test and ask the client to evaluate each per- son on the list according to the construct. This step creates a grid similar to the one shown in Table 15.3 and allows the therapist and client to look for patterns across a broad set of information. The Repertory Grid Technique has been widely used by therapists and clinical psychologists to obtain a visual map of how clients and thosesuffering from various psychological disorders construe the world (Feixas,Erazo-Caicedo, Harter, & Bach, 2008; Winter, 2003). But the grid technique has also been used by researchers when studying such diverse topics as com- munication within a large organization (Coopman, 1997), teaching effective-ness (Chitsabesan, Corbett, Walker, Spencer, & Barton, 2006), profiles of specific criminal types (Horley, 1996), and career counseling (Savickas, 1997). One reviewer counted more than 3,000 studies using variations of the Repertory Grid Technique (Neimeyer, 2001). Like other assessment procedures, the grid technique also has its limita- tions. One concern is that, unlike other personality measures, the RepertoryGrid Technique does not generate a simple test score (Horley, 1996). Although various number-generating systems have been developed, the proce- dure still allows for a large degree of interpretation on the part of the thera- pist. Another limitation concerns the many assumptions underlying the test. One assumption when using the Rep Test is that the constructs clients provide are not limited to the people on the list but also would apply to new people in new situations. Another assumption is that the constructs elicited during the test have some degree of permanence. That is, we assume clients are not using these constructs for the first time in the testing session and never again. A re- lated assumption is that the people on the list are representative of the kind of people clients are likely to deal with in their daily lives. Constructs used only for unique people that clients rarely encounter are of little use in understand-ing how clients deal with the majority of people with whom they interact.TABLE 15.3 Sample Grid Mom Dad Sister Brother Boss Neighbor Friend Coworker Pleasant P U U U U U P U Unpleasant Trustworthy U U U U U U ? U Untrustworthy Competitive N N C C ? N C C Not CompetitiveWarm W C C C C C ? C Cold Intelligent N I I I I ? N I Not IntelligentFun D D D D D D F D DullAssessment: The Repertory Grid Technique 429
ASSESSING YOUR OWN PERSONALITY Personal Constructs To begin, write down the names of the following 12 people. Although a person may fit more than one category, you need to compile a list of 12 different people. If there is no one who fits a category, name someone whois similar to the category description. For example, if you do not have a brother, select someone who is like a brother to you. 1. A teacher you liked 2. A teacher you disliked 3. Your wife (husband) or boyfriend (girlfriend) 4. An employer, supervisor, or officer you found hard to get along with 5. An employer, supervisor, or officer you liked 6. Your mother 7. Your father 8. Brother nearest your age 9. Sister nearest your age 10. A person with whom you have worked who was easy to get along with 11. A person with whom you have worked who was hard to understand 12. A neighbor with whom you get along well Next, take three of these people at a time, as indicated by the numbers inthe following list. Then describe in what important way two of them are alike but different from the third. Put a word or phrase describing the two alike people in the Construct list and a description of the remaining person in the Contrast list. Names Construct Contrast 3, 6, 7 1, 4, 10 4, 7, 8 1, 6, 9 4, 5, 8 2, 11, 12 8, 9, 10 (continues)430 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
But the most precarious assumption made by test givers, according to Kelly, is that people are ableto describe the constructs they use. Unfortunately, the grid technique is subject to the inherent limits of our language. Although clients may supply words that come close to what they mean, these words may be inadequate. Kelly did not assume that words necessarily exist for describing all constructs. In fact, he described “preverbal ”constructs, those developed before we learn to speak. And even when clients do use appropriatewords, therapists may interpret those words differently. For example, a client ’s definition of aggressive may be quite different from a therapist ’s. In this case the therapist may still end up with a false impression of how the client viewsthe world. STRENGTHS AND CRITICISMS OF THE COGNITIVE APPROACH Strengths One strength of the cognitive approach to personality is that many of the ideasevolved out of and were developed through empirical research findings. Mostof the cognitive structures used to account for individual differences have been subjected to extensive investigation in controlled laboratory experiments. In many cases personality psychologists have borrowed ideas and research proce- dures from social and cognitive psychologists investigating similar phenomena. Moreover, cognitive models of personality have been modified as investigatorsNames Construct Contrast 2, 3, 5 5, 7, 11 1, 10, 12 This is an abbreviated version of Kelly’ s Rep Test (the Minimum Context Form). The test provides a quick idea of the constructs you use to organize information about the people you know and meet. You may want to com- pare your responses with those of other test takers. No doubt you will find a few overlapping constructs, but also many you hadn ’t thought of. Of course, these differences in personal constructs represent differences in personality that should translate into individual differences in your behavior. Scale: The Role Construct Repertory (Rep) Test Source: The Role Construct Repertory (REP) Test from A Theory of Personality: The Psychology of Personal Constructs by George A. Kelly. Copyright © 1955, 1963 by George A. Kelly, renewed 1983, 1991 by Gladys Kelly. Used by permission of W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.Strengths and Criticisms of the Cognitive Approach 431
learn more about cognitive structures and processes through their ongoing research. Another strength of the cognitive approach is that it fits well with the current mood, or Zeitgeist, of psychology. The number of journal articles and doctoral dissertations examining cognitive concepts has risen dramati- cally over the past few decades. Researchers in other areas of psychology, such as developmental and social psychologists, are working on related lines of research that often complement and extend what is known from the cogni-tive personality perspective. Related to the preceding point, cognitive approaches to psychotherapy have become particularly popular in recent years. Even therapists who identify withother approaches to personality sometimes incorporate aspects of cognitive therapy in their practice. A survey of practitioners in the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, a group originally composed of behaviortherapists, found that 67% described their therapy orientation as “cognitive be- havioral ”(Elliott et al., 1996). Nearly half said they occasionally use rational emotive therapy with their clients. Criticisms A frequent criticism of the cognitive approach is that the concepts are some-times too abstract for empirical research. What exactly is a “personal con- struct”or a“possible self ”? How do we know if a schema is being used? How many schemas are there, and how are they related? More important, how can we study their influence on behavior if we can ’t agree on clear oper- ational definitions? Some of the answers may come with more research, but the nature of cognitions probably renders them more nebulous than manyconstructs used by personality theorists. A related question is whether we need to introduce these concepts to account for individual differences in be havior. For example, strict behavior- ists might argue that they can explain the same phenomena with fewer con- structs. Introducing schemas or possible selves may be unnecessary and perhaps even an obstacle to understanding personality. Applying the law ofparsimony, it is incumbent upon cognitive theorists to demonstrate how their approach can explain personality better than other, less complicated approaches. Another source of concern about the cognitive approach to personality is that there is no single model to organize and guide theory and research.Basic questions about how various cognitive structures relate to one another and to other aspects of information processing, such as memory, remain unanswered. A related problem concer ns the relationship between the vari- ous cognitive structures different theorists have introduced. Is a personal construct different from a schema? A comprehensive model would help re- searchers understand precisely what these terms mean and how they are related.432 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
SUMMARY 1. The cognitive approach to personality describes consistent behavior pat- terns in terms of the way people process information. George Kelly was an early pioneer in this approach with his personal construct theory. Kelly maintained that we are motivated to make sense out of our world. He compared people to scientists, always striving for better predictions about what will happen to them. Kelly described the cognitive structures we use in this regard as personal constructs. He maintained that psycho- logical problems stem from anxiety, which results from a person ’s inabil- ity to predict events. 2. Psychologists have described a number of cognitive structures to help explain individual differences and intrapersonal processes. Schemas arecognitive structures that help us perceive, organize, and store information. 3. Perhaps the most important cognitive structures for personality psy- chologists are the cognitive representations we have for our selves. Much research in this area is concerned with self-schemas. Studies demonstrate that we perceive information more readily and recall itbetter when it is relevant to our self-schemas. Researchers also find that cognitive representations of future selves guide our behavior, but that discrepancies between different self-concepts can result in negative emotions. 4. Cognitive approaches to psychotherapy have become increasingly popu- lar in the last few decades. These therapies focus on changing the clients ’ thoughts. Albert Ellis, an early advocate of this approach, argued that people have emotional problems when they use irrational beliefs. Rational emotive therapy helps clients see how they use these beliefs and how to replace them with more rational ones. 5. Kelly introduced the Repertory Grid Technique to measure individual differences in personal constructs. In one example, test takers typicallydevelop a list of people in their lives and then divide these people into various categories. This procedure helps therapists see the constructs clients use to make sense of the world. Kelly acknowledged severalassumptions behind this approach, including that people can adequately communicate the constructs they use. 6. Among the strengths of the cognitive approach is its strong empirical background. The cognitive approach also fits nicely with the current trend in psychology toward cogniti ve explanations of behavior. Some critics of the cognitive approach have complained that many of the concepts used by cognitive theor ists are too abstract. Others have questioned whether it is always neces sary to introduce cognitions to explain behavior. The cognitive approach also suffers from the lack ofa general model to organize all of the work that falls under this approach.Summary 433
KEY TERMS personal constructs (p. 412) possible selves (p. 422) rational emotive therapy (p. 425) schema (p. 417) self-schema (p. 418) MEDIA RESOURCES Visit our website. Go to www.cengage.com/ psychology/burger to find online resources directly linked to your book, including quizzing, glossary, flashcards, and more.434 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach
CHAPTER16 The Cognitive Approach Relevant Research Cognitions and Aggression Gender, Memory, and Self-ConstrualCognitions and Depression Summary 435
If you think back to the first chapter, you may recall the story about the blind men trying to describe an elephant. The point was that obtaining a completeunderstanding of human personality requires that we examine personality from several different perspectives. Although each perspective offers useful in- formation, each also provides only a limited view of this complex topic. That lesson is clearly illustrated in this chapter. Each program of research examines from a cognitive perspective a topic covered elsewhere in the book. It ’sn o t that the research covered earlier is wrong or needs updating. Rather, the point is that we need to examine important topics from more than one perspective if we want to obtain a complete picture. We ’ll begin with aggression, a topic we’ve looked at in depth from a psychoanalytic and behavioral/social learning perspective. Next, we ’ll return to the topic of gender. In addition to the many gender differences we ’ve touched on throughout the book, researchers find that men and women also differ in the way they remember information. Finally, we ’ll examine cognitive explanations for depression, another topic that has surfaced often in this book. Investigators find that the way we process in- formation plays a crucial role in this psychological disorder. Psychologists alsolook at cognitive styles to identify those who might be vulnerable to future bouts of depression. COGNITIONS AND AGGRESSION Imagine you are strolling alone through a park. Two teenage boys walking about 30 feet behind you suddenly quicken their pace and draw closer. What is your reaction? Perhaps the boys are in a hurry to get somewhere. Perhaps they are simply more energetic and walk faster than you do. Maybe they are interested in catching up to you to ask for the time or directions. Or maybe they want to harm you. This situation, like many we encounter, contains a fair degree of ambiguity, and people react to it differently. How you respond to this situation depends on how you interpret it. Whether you see the circumstances as threatening, annoying, or benign willcause you to run away, prepare to fight, or move out of the way. This ex-ample illustrates a key concept that cognitive researchers rely on when try- ing to predict aggressive behavior. It ’s not enough to know that a person is high in aggressiveness, has just witnessed someone model aggression, or has a history of being rewarded for violence. Although all of these variables play a role, a full understanding of aggressive behavior requires that we also ex- amine the cognitions that come into play when people encounter a poten- tially threatening or dangerous situation (Anderson & Huesmann, 2003; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Fontaine & Dod ge, 2006; Wilkowski & Robinson, 2008). General Aggression Model Whether a potentially violent encounter passes uneventfully or leads to ag- gression depends on a large number of factors. To explain this process, one team of psychologists have combined decades of research findings into a436 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
General Aggression Model (Anderson & Bushman, 2002a). As shown in Figure 16.1, the model begins with a social encounter with the potential for triggering aggression. This might be an insult, a threat, a shove, or any action that could be interpreted along these lines. How we respond to the event de- pends first on the kind of person we are and the kind of situation we are in. Because of differences in traits, attitudes, past experiences, genetic predisposi- tions, etc., some people are more prone to aggression than others. Moreover, some situations lend themselves to violence more than others. Aggression ismore likely when the situation includes provocation, frustration and elements that we associate with violence (visual cues, words, noises). But psychologists looking at the process from a cognitive perspective argue that these personal and situational factors are important only to theextent that they are tied to and activate aggression-relation thoughts and emotions. Some people are more prone to making these cognitive associa-tions than others. For a highly aggressi ve person, even a mild insult can gen- erate hostile thoughts and anger. Cog nitive psychologists would say that these individuals have a well-developed and easily accessible network of Social Encounters Person Hostile Thoughts and Emotions Aggression Scripts Appraisal and DecisionSituation Thoughtful ActionImpulsive Action FIGURE 16.1 General Aggression Model Source: Adapted from Anderson and Bushman (2002a).Cognitions and Aggression 437
hostile cognitions. Aspects of the situation that the person associates with aggression activate these hostile cogniti ons. These situational variables in- clude visual aggressive cues, such as weapons, fists, and blood, but also in- clude any sounds, smells, or sights that the person cognitively links to hostile thoughts. People recently exposed to violent images, such as scenes from violent mo- vies or violent video games, also have highly accessible hostile cognitions. Similarly, frustrated people are likely to have highly accessible hostile thoughts.Individuals who frequently expose themselves to violent images may have chronically accessible hostile cognitions. In a sense, these people move through their worlds ready to think aggressively about whatever they might encounter. Among the hostile cognitions potentially activated in this process are ag- gressive behavior scripts . These are potential patterns of behavior that have been learned and sometimes practiced. In many cases, the aggressive scripthas been learned by watching aggressive models. Even though you may never have punched someone in the face, you have seen the behavior modeled often enough in your life that you probably could act out this script if you needed to.Aggressive scripts that have been practiced are even more likely to be acted out. People can practice aggressive scripts simply by imagining themselves taking an aggressive action or by reliving in their minds a violent sequence they may have experienced or seen. People also practice aggressive scripts when they physically rehearse the behavior, such as during karate drills or target practice. The more accessible the behavior script, the more likely the individual will act aggressively. This cognitive analysis helps us understand why violent movies and tele- vision programs sometimes lead to aggression. Recall from Chapter 14 thatpeople often imitate acts of aggression. However, researchers find that partici- pants frequently act in violent ways not modeled in the movie or program. Psychologists explain this effect in terms of hostile thoughts and emotionsthat are primed by the violent images (Anderson & Huesmann, 2003). When violent memories and thoughts are primed and therefore highly accessible, the likelihood that the viewer will act aggressively increases. Consistent with thisanalysis, a large number of studies find exposure to violent movies or violent video games increases aggressive thoughts (Carlson, Marcus-Newhall, & Miller, 1990; Todorov & Bargh, 2002). Even exposure to songs with violentlyrics has been found to increase aggressive thoughts and feelings (Anderson, Carnagey, & Eubanks, 2003; Fischer & Greitemeyer, 2006). Aggressive cognitions not only trigger aggressive behavior scripts, they also affect the way we interpret situations. Returning to the example of the teenageboys in the park, if hostile thoughts and emotions were easily accessible (per- haps you just came from a violent movie), you might very well interpret thesituation as a threat, perhaps even one that called for an aggressive reaction on your part. However, psychologists are quick to point out that a great deal of aggression takes place in a more-or-less automatic or impulsive manner(Berkowitz, 2008; Fontaine & Dodge, 2006). Sometimes when someone steps on our toes or bumps us from behind we respond without thinking. In these in- stances, highly accessible hostile cognitions can be particularly dangerous.438 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
Reactive Aggression in Boys Common observation confirms what researchers know about aggression in adolescents and preadolescents. Boys are much more likely to engage in phys- ical acts of aggression than girls (Card, Stucky, Sawalani, & Little, 2008). Needless to say, this aggression creates numerous problems for the aggressive boys as well as for those around them. Researchers are particularly interested in boys who exhibit reactive aggression , i.e., angry, hostile aggression in re- sponse to frustration or provocation. These are the boys who react to a little teasing by threatening to beat up the teaser and who turn an accidental bump in the hallway into a fist fight. To better understand these reactions, some psychologists examine the way these boys interpret potentially aggression-provoking situations. Oneteam of researchers presented a series of hypothetical situations to boys with a history of reactive aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1996). For example, in onescenario another student breaks the boy’ s radio while the boy is out of the room. For each situation, the boys were asked why the other student did what he did and whether the act was intentional. As shown in Figure 16.2, the boys with a history of reactive aggression were more likely to see the act as intentional and hostile than were nonaggressive boys. Another way to say this is that the aggressive boys had chronically accessible hostile thoughts that led them to interpret harmless acts as threatening. Not surprisingly, researchers find that these types of interpretations often lead to aggression (Dodge, 2006; Dodge et al., 2003). If the attributions some boys make for unintentional acts are responsible for reactive aggression, then programs designed to reduce physical aggression Attri butionMore Intentional Less Intentional Grade Level3/4Aggressive BoysNonaggressive Boys 5/69 87 6 5 FIGURE 16.2 Mean Attribution Score Source: Adapted from Crick and Dodge (1996).Cognitions and Aggression 439
in schools could benefit from focusing on those attributions. This was the ap- proach taken by one team of investigators (Metropolitan Area Child StudyResearch Group, 2007). The researchers first identified elementary school children who were at risk for acting violently. Beginning in Grades 2 and 3, some of these children attended 40 one-hour sessions over a 2-year period in which they learned, among other things, how to respond in effective, non- violent ways to conflict and threats. Other children were randomly assigned to a control group that did not attend the sessions. At the beginning and atthe end of the 2-year study, the researchers presented the children with sce- narios and asked the children how they would probably react to the situa- tions. Among the children who came from moderate-income neighborhoods,there was a tendency for those in the control group to become more aggres- sive as they became older. However, the children in the intervention condi- tion showed a decrease in aggressive tendencies over the same periodof time. GENDER, MEMORY, AND SELF-CONSTRUAL The next time you want to stir things up at a dull social gathering, raise thisquestion: Do men or women have better memories? Inevitably, I find peoplecome to the defense of their own gender. Men complain about times their spouses forgot to pay bills or fill the gas tank, and women point out the way their husbands overlook anniversaries and the names of in-laws. Although these responses reflect more than an ounce of stereotype, they also highlight observations psychologists make about gender and memory. Research sug- gests men and women do not differ in their general ability to memorize andrecall information. However, investigators often find differences in what men and women remember. Consider a study in which men and women were asked to recall several different kinds of information (Seidlitz & Diener, 1998). Participants first were given 3 minutes to list as many positive and negative events as they could recall from the previous 3 years of their lives. Later the participantswere asked to recall, among other things, emotional events from the previous year and from a randomly selected 1-hour interval from the previous week. Participants were also given a limited amount of time to recall events fromAmerican history. Who had the better recall? As shown in Figure 16.3, the answer depends on what kind of information the participants were asked to remember. Thewomen recalled significantly more personal events than the men. This was true for both negative events and positive events. On the other hand, men did better recalling the impersonal information about American history. In short, women were better able to remember happy occasions with friends and times they embarrassed themselves, whereas men recalled better the facts they had learned in school or read about. Psychologists explain these differences in memory by pointing to the way people process self-relevant information. Specifically, investigators identify two differences in the way men and women organize information in memory.440 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
First, the genders differ in the extent to which self-relevant information is as- sociated with emotions. Second, men and women differ in the extent to whichinformation about themselves is connected with information about personal relationships. Emotional Memories From an early age, females learn to pay attention to their emotions and the emotions of others. Consequently, women are more likely than men to en-code information about themselves in terms of emotions (Bloise & Johnson, 2007; Feldman Barrett, Lane, Sechrest, & Schwartz, 2000; Kuebli, Butler, & Fivush, 1995). And if women organize their memories around emotions, we should not be surprised to find that they are better able to recall both positive and negative emotional experiences (Fujita, Diener, & Sandvik, 1991). Memories for both happy and sad experiences should be more accessible for women.Moreover, the cognitive link between one emotional memory and another should be stronger for women than men. Thus recalling one sad experience is likely to trigger another sad memory for women, but perhaps not for men. Number of Events Recalled Past 3 Y earsPast Ye arPast WeekMenWomen16 1514 13 121110 Personal Experiences 9 87 6 Historical Facts FIGURE 16.3 Number of Events Recalled Source: Adapted from “Sex differences in the recall of affective experience, ”by L. Seidlitz and E. Diener, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1998, p. 262 –271. Copyright © 1998 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission of the author.Gender, Memory, and Self-Construal 441
These gender differences were demonstrated in a study in which adult men and women were asked to recall childhood experiences (Davis, 1999). Participants were cued with a series of emotional words and phrases, such as “feeling rejected” or“getting something you really wanted. ”As shown in Figure 16.4, women recalled more emotional memories from childhood than did men. Moreover, this was true for each emotion examined, whether positive or negative. When the investigator compared similar recall in male and femalestudents in grades 3, 5, 8, and 11, she found a similar pattern. That is, regard- less of age, females are better able to recall emotional memories than males. Interestingly, the researcher found no gender differences when men and womenwere asked to recall nonemotional memories. This tendency for women to recall more emotional memories could help explain why women suffer from depression more often than men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001). Not only do women remember sad experiences more often than men, but recalling one unhappy incident is also likely to activate memo- ries about other sad events. Memories About Relationships Another line of research looks at the extent to which men and women consider relationships when they organize self-relevant information. Drawing from the work on individualist and collectivist cultures (Chapter 1), some psychologists argue that the way men and women are raised in our society causes them to form different cognitive representations of themselves (Cross & Madson, 1997). Men are said to develop independent self-construals .T h a ti s ,m e n ’ss e l f - concepts are relatively unrelated to the cognitive representations they have for510152025 Guilty Self-Conscious Fear Angry Sad HappyNumber of Memories Recalled Recall ConditionsWomen Men FIGURE 16.4 Recall of Emotional Childhood Memories Source: From “Gender differences in autobiographical memory of childhood emotional experience, ”by P. J. Davis, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1999, 76, 498 –510. Copyright © 1999 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission of the American Psychological Association.442 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
other people. On the other hand, women in our society tend to develop inter- dependent self-construals . Their self-concepts are highly related to the cognitive representations they have of others and their relationships with those people. In particular, these cognitive representations are tied to those with whom women feel close and personal relations (Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). Put another way, relationships with friends and loved ones are an impor- tant part of how women think of themselves. It ’s not just that they enjoy their relationships more than men, but rather that women are more likely to define themselves in terms of the relationships they share with others. Returning to the memory data, perhaps one reason women recall certain kinds of experi- ences more readily than men is that these events may have involved otherpeople. Because of their interdependent self-construal, information involving relationships is more accessible for women than for men. Consistent with this analysis, several studies find women are more likely than men to define themselves in terms of their relationships (Guimond, Chatard, ASSESSING YOUR OWN PERSONALITY Self-Construal Indicate the extent to which you agree with the following statements. Use a 7-point scale to indicate your response, with 1 ¼Strongly disagree and 7¼Strongly agree. 1. My close relationships are an important reflection of who I am. 2. When I feel very close to someone, it often feels to me like that person is an important part of who I am. 3. I usually feel a strong sense of pride when someone close to me has an important accomplishment. 4. I think one of the most important parts of who I am can be captured by looking at my close friends and understandingwho they are. 5. When I think of myself, I often think of my close friends or family also. 6. If a person hurts someone close to me, I feel personally hurt as well. 7. In general, my close relationships are an important part of my self-image. 8. Overall, my close relationships have very little to do with how I feel about myself. 9. My close relationships are unimportant to my sense of what kind of person I am. 10. My sense of pride comes from knowing who I have as close friends. 11. When I establish a close friendship with someone, I usually develop a strong sense of identification with that person. (continues)Gender, Memory, and Self-Construal 443
Martinot, Crisp, & Redersdorff, 2006). Participants in one investigation were asked simply to list as many statements as they could in response to the question “Who am I? ”(Mackie, 1983). Compared to male participants, the women in the study included more statements about their roles as parents and family members. Similar findings were uncovered when elementary and high school students were asked to “tell us about yourself ”(McGuire & McGuire, 1982). Another group of researchers gave participants a camera with 12-exposure film and asked them to take (or have someone else take) photographs that “describe who you are as you see yourself ”(Clancy & Dollinger, 1993). In other words, the photographs provided a rough indication of the cognitive representationsthe men and women held of themselves. As shown in Figure 16.5, the women ’s photographs were more likely to include other people. When the women por-trayed the way they thought of themselves, they chose to include pictures withbest friends and loved ones. In contrast, the men more often portrayed their self-concept with images of themselves alone. Other research finds gender differences in the way men and women per- ceive and recall information about significant people in their lives. Participants in one study were asked if certain words described them, their best friend, a group they belong to, or the president of the United States (Josephs, Markus, & Tafarodi, 1992). As described in the previous chapter, researchers assume information processed through a strong schema will be more accessible than information processed through weaker schemas. When participants were later asked to recall as many of the words as possible, the women remembered words processed through their best friend and group schemas better than the men. Married couples in another study were asked to talk about their first date together, their last vacation together, and a re- cent argument between the two of them (Ross & Holmberg, 1992). TheTo obtain your score, reverse the answer values for items 8 and 9 (that is, 1¼7, 2¼6, etc.). Then add all 11 answer values together. High scores indicate a tendency to think of oneself in terms of your relationships withclose others. That is, those scoring high on the scale have self-concepts closely tied to the cognitive representations they have of the people they feel emotionally closest to (Cross, Morris, & Gore, 2002). You can compare your score with those obtained from a sample of American undergraduates(Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000): Men Women Total Mean 52.89 55.11 54.10 Standard Deviation 8.07 10.03 9.29 Scale: The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale Source: Copyright © 1998 by the American Psychological Association. Reproduced with permission. Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 78, 791 –808. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.791. No further reproduction or distribution is permitted without written permission from the American Psychological Association.444 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
stories told by the wives were more vivid and contained more details than those described by the husbands. In short, evidence from many sources makes a strong case that men and women differ in the way they store and recall in- formation about relationships and thus about themselves. COGNITIONS AND DEPRESSION For a moment, try to think of a time when you felt a little depressed. One ofthe first things you may notice is that this is relatively easy if you already feel a little down today and relatively difficult if you feel pretty good. Depressed people not only remember sad experiences more easily but may also have dif- ficulty keeping themselves from generating one depressing thought after an- other. Sad people easily recall times when they felt lonely and unloved. They tend to dwell on their problems and worry about all the things that might gowrong. They recall embarrassing mishaps, things they wish they had never said, and experiences they wish they could erase. Even when good things hap- pen, depressed people look for the gray cloud to go with the silver lining. Justgot accepted into a good school? Think of all that pressure and what happens if you fail. Been invited to a party? What if you don ’t know anyone or you embarrass yourself there? In short, when you ’re depressed, your mind fills with depressing thoughts. These observations make it clear that depressing thoughts are tied to de- pressing feelings. This is why psychologists increasingly are turning to cogni- tive approaches to understand depression. Although negative thoughts are often considered a symptom of depression, the cognitive perspective argues that these thoughts can also cause people to become depressed (Clark, Beck, & Alford, 1999). Psychologists sometimes describe the thoughts of depressed012345Females Males Self Alone Self With OthersNumber of Photographs FIGURE 16.5 Number of Photographs Used to Portray Self Source: With kind permission from Springer Science+Business Media: “Photographic depictions of the self: Gender and age differences in social connectedness, ”by S. M. Clancy and S. J. Dollinger, Sex Roles , 29, 477–495 (1993). Copyright © 1993 by Plenum Publishing Corporation. Reprinted by permission of Springer.Cognitions and Depression 445
people as a depressive cognitive triad (Beck, 1972). That is, depressed people typically have negative thoughts about themselves, are pessimistic about the future, and tend to interpret ongoing experiences in a negative manner. Many psychologists look for clues about the causes and treatment of de- pression by examining the way people perceive, organize, and recall emotion-ally laden information (Rusting, 1998). Among other questions, these investigators want to know if some people are prone to depression because of the way they process information. We ’ll look at two concepts investigators use in this research —depressive schemas and negative cognitive style. Depressive Schemas Each day we encounter some good events, a few bad events, and an occa-sional incident with ambiguous emotional meaning. Which ones will you think about today and which will you ignore? From a cognitive perspective, the happiest people are those who pay attention to the positive information, dismiss the negative information, and interpret the ambiguous information as positively as possible. In fact, most of us have an unrealistically positive out- look on life (Taylor, 1989). We believe we are better than most at almosteverything we do, certain that good things will happen to us, and convinced unfortunate events happen to other people. Because most of us look at life through rose-colored glasses, we remain content and in good psychologicalhealth (Alloy & Abramson, 1988). Unfortunately, many people look at life through glasses that are tinted blue. Psychologists from a cognitive perspective say that depressed peopleprocess information through an active depressive schema (Clark et al., 1999; Kuiper & Derry, 1981; Kuiper, MacDonald, & Derry, 1983). A depressive schema is a cognitive structure containing memories about and associations with depressing events and thoughts. People processing information through this schema attend to negative information, ignore positive information, and interpret ambiguous information in a depressing way. They also recall de- pressing memories easily and often associate current sad experiences with sad incidents from their past. In short, depressed people are set to process informa- tion in a way that keeps negative thoughts prominent and positive thoughts away. Little wonder, then, that these people remain depressed. Researchers have developed a number of procedures to study depressive schemas. Along with clinical observations about how depressed people thinkand act, these experiments provide an impressive body of evidence pointing to the role of cognitive structures in the development and maintenance of de- pression. Much of the evidence for depressive schemas comes from studies employing the self-schema research techniques described in the previous chap- ter. Researchers sometimes ask depressed and nondepressed individuals to an- swer questions about a series of words. In one study, depressed patientsresponded to a list of adjectives by pressing a YES or a NO button to indicate if the word described them (Derry & Kuiper, 1981). Half the words were re- lated to depression (for example, bleak, dismal, helpless ), and half were not. The researchers then surprised the participants by giving them 3 minutes to recall as many of the words as they could.446 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
The results of the study are shown in Figure 16.6. As predicted, depressed patients remembered the depression-associated words better, whereas two groups of nondepressed participants recalled the other words better. Thisfinding has been replicated with clinically depressed patients (Lim & Kim, 2005) and mildly depressed college students (Moilanen, 1993). Depressed people recall words like dismal and helpless better because they process these words through a depressive schema. They are more likely to attend to the depression-related words, associate them with aspects of themselves, and readily recall them later on. If depressed people process information through a depressive schema, we would also expect them to recall sad memories more readily than people whoare not depressed. If I ask you to quickly think of something that happened toyou in high school, most likely you will think of a pleasant time. You might recall a star performance in a play or perhaps just the fun you had hanging out with friends. But if you are a little depressed today, you might instead re- call a test you failed or a time you were rejected by friends. This is because people processing information through a depressive schema have greater ac- cess to the depressing memories stored there. When you are depressed, it should not take long to recall times when you were sad, lonely, or embar- rassed because using a depressive schema makes these memories readily accessible. This easy access to sad memories was demonstrated in an experiment with depressed clients (Clark & Teasdale, 1982). Clients were given a series10203040Percentage of Words Recalled Depressed PatientsNondepressed PatientsNondepressed NormalsDepression- Associated Words Nondepression- Associated Words FIGURE 16.6 Proportion of Self-Descriptive Words Recalled with Self-Referent Processing Source: From “Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression, ”by P. A. Derry and N. A. Kuiper, Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 1981, 90, 286 –297.Cognitions and Depression 447
of words (such as train, ice ) and asked to recall a real-life experience each word brought to mind. For example, a client might describe a train ride to visit her favorite aunt or a time she missed a train. Clients were tested twice, once when they were feeling particularly depressed and once when they were less depressed. As shown in Figure 16.7, most of the memories recalled during the depressed period were unhappy ones. However, when clients were less de- pressed, they recalled happier experiences. Presumably the depressive schemas were activated more when the clients ’depression levels were higher. A similar pattern is found when depressed people are asked to think about the future. Depressed individuals in one study could more easily come up with reasons something bad is likely to happen to them someday than could nondepressedparticipants (Vaughn & Weary, 2002). Because depressed people filter information through a depressive schema, they also tend to interpret ambiguous information in the most negative lightpossible. When depressed people consider their performances, they tend to dwell on what they did wrong and fail to give themselves enough credit for what they did right (Crowson & Cromwell, 1995; Moretti et al., 1996).Participants in one study were given the choice of looking at either the favor- able or unfavorable scores from a battery of tests they had taken (Giesler, Josephs, & Swann, 1996). Eighty-two percent of the depressed participants chose the unfavorable feedback, significantly more than the nondepressed participants. Thus, if an instructor tells a depressed student he did well on five essay answers but was a little weak on one, the student will most likely 30405060Percentage of Recalled Experiences When More DepressedWhen Less DepressedHappy Experiences Unhappy Experiences FIGURE 16.7 Percentages of Happy and Unhappy Experiences Recalled Source: From “Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences, ”by D. M. Clark and J. D. Teasdale, Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 1982, 91, 87 –95. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association.448 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
focus his or her attention on the one weak answer and conclude that the per- formance was poor. Not surprisingly, depressive thoughts go hand-in-hand with other depres- sion symptoms, such as sad mood and decreased activity. Cognitive theorists see the causal arrow between depressive cognitions and these other symptoms running both ways (Clark et al., 1999). That is, depressing thoughts can cause depression, and depression can lead to an increase in depressing thoughts. However, several studies suggest that although negative thoughts decline aspeople recover from an episode of depression, the underlying cognitive network often remains in place (Dozois & Dobson, 2001; Hedlund & Rude, 1995; Ilardi & Craighead, 1999; Ingram & Ritter, 2000; Segal, Gemar, & Williams, 1999).If a strong depressive schema stays intact, the individual may be vulnerable to future bouts of depression (Havermans, Nicolson, & deVries, 2007; Lewinsohn, Joiner, & Rohde, 2001). In fact, people with strong depressiveschemas may face a daily battle to fend off depression. Formerly depressed patients in one study showed an increase in negative thoughts simply after lis- tening to a sad piece of music (Gemar, Segal, Sagrati, & Kennedy, 2001). Negative Cognitive Style In Chapter 14 we looked at research on learned helplessness. As you recall, psychologists initially demonstrated this effect in dogs that failed to escape from electric shocks after first experiencing inescapable shocks. The dogs learned they were helpless in one situation and inappropriately generalized that perception to the new situation. Not long after the demonstrations with animals, researchers found that people also sometimes generalize helpless feel- ings to controllable situations. Similarities between learned helplessness parti-cipants and depressed patients led some psychologists to propose learned helplessness as a model for understanding depression. However, investigators soon found the simple model used to explain ani- mal behavior was insufficient for understanding learned helplessness in peo-ple. Human research participants reacted to some uncontrollable situations with helplessness, but not others. Feelings of helplessness generalized to sometasks, but not every task. People exposed to inescapable noise sometimes be- came less motivated, but occasionally motivation increased (Costello, 1978; Depue & Monroe, 1978; Roth, 1980). The limitations of the original model led some investigators to argue that negative life experiences alone are not sufficient to produce depression.Rather, how we interpret these events is the key (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Miller & Norman, 1979). If you attribute the loss of a job to a general lack of skills and aptitude that will keep you from getting a good job anywhere else, you may be headed for depression. However, if you fail an algebra class and conclude it ’s because this particular instructor used a strange and unfair grading system, it is unlikely you’ ll generalize feelings of helplessness to other math classes or other subjects. Psychologists soon observed individual differences in the way people ex- plain the events they encounter. In particular, researchers identified whatCognitions and Depression 449
they called a negative cognitive style. Individuals who possess a negative cognitive style tend to attribute their problems to stable (enduring) and global (widespread) causes. They also tend to anticipate the most dreadful conse- quences and often believe the problem is the result of or reflects their own per- sonal shortcomings. Investigators have developed procedures to measure the extent to which people rely on this type of thinking (Alloy et al., 2000; Beevers, Strong, Meyer, Pilkonis, & Miller, 2007; Peterson et al., 1982; Peterson & Villanova, 1988). Like other personality variables, negative cognitive styletends to be fairly stable over time (Burns & Seligman, 1989; Hankin, 2008). Not surprisingly, researchers find that negative cognitive style is related to depression (Goldberg, Gerstein, Wenze, Beck & Welker, 2008; Haeffel et al.,2003; Hankin, Fraley, & Abela, 2005; Lau & Eley, 2008; Otto et al., 2007; Riso et al., 2003). People who interpret negative events as the result of personal shortcomings that are enduring and widespread are vulnerable to depressionwhen one of life ’s unfortunate experiences inevitably comes their way. One pair of investigators looked at the psychological effects of physical and emo- tional abuse in a group of battered women (Palker-Corell & Marcus, 2004).The researchers contacted the women within 2 weeks after their arrival at a battered women ’s shelter. They found the women with a negative cognitive style suffered from depression and other symptoms of trauma more often than the women who did not rely on this style of thinking. Another study looked at emotional reactions following a strong earthquake (Greening, Stoppelbein, & Docter, 2002). Participants with a negative cognitive style were more likely to be depressed after the earthquake than those without this cognitive style. Investigators also use negative cognitive style to predict who might be vul- nerable to future episodes of depression (Evans, Heron, Lewis, Araya, &Wolke, 2005; Fresco, Alloy, & Reilly-Harrington, 2006; Lewinsohn et al., 2001; Robinson & Alloy, 2003). One team of researchers measured negative cognitive style in incoming college freshmen (Alloy, Abramson, Whitehouse,Hogan, Panzarella, & Rose, 2006). They divided the students into those who had previously suffered from an episode of depression and those who had not. The researchers contacted the students again every 6 weeks for the next2 1/2 years. As shown in Figure 16.8, the students who were identified as high risk for depression based on their negative cognitive style were much more likely to experience at least one episode of major depression during this timethan the other students. This was not only the case for those who had bouts of depression prior to college, but also for those who had never before suffered from depression. In fact, the high-risk students in the latter group were 6 timesmore likely to experience depression than their low-risk classmates. We should also note that the link between cognitive style and depression may be affected by culture. Recall from earlier chapters that people in collec-tivist cultures tend to emphasize their role in the community, whereas people in individualistic cultures focus on their individual aspirations and accomplish- ments. One team of researchers found that college students in China (a collec-tivist culture) had a more pessimistic cognitive style than American students (Lee & Seligman, 1997). Consistent with their individualistic emphasis, the Americans were more likely to attribute successes to their own efforts and450 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
failures to other people or unfortunate circumstances. However, another inves- tigation comparing American and Chinese students found that the kinds of ex-planations that predict depression in the United States are also associated with depression in China (Anderson, 1999). Thus, although the way people in two cultures typically explain events may be different, the cognitive style that leads to depression is the same. SUMMARY 1. Psychologists have looked at the role of hostile cognitions in aggressive behavior. They find that aggressive responses are more likely when asituation activates hostile thoughts and emotions. Hostile cognitionsinclude aggressive behavior scripts. When hostile cognitions are highly accessible, the likelihood of acting aggressively increases. Boys with a history of reacting to minor events aggressively tend to interpret uninten-tional acts as deliberate and hostile. 2. Some psychologists have looked at differences in men ’s and women ’s abilities to recall certain kinds of information. These researchers findevidence that women are more likely than men to organize self-relevant information around emotions. Women also have been found to have strong cognitive connections between mental representations of themselvesand the mental representations they have of close friends and loved ones. 3. The cognitive approach assumes that depressing thoughts are an impor- tant cause of depression. Depressed people are said to process informa- tion through a depressive schema. Depressed people recall depressing information and remember depressing events more readily than non- depressed people. Other researchers look at negative cognitive style. People who tend to interpret negative events in terms of enduring and widespread causes are more prone to depression than those who do not rely on this cognitive style.25 20 15 10 No Prior DepressionLow Risk High Risk Prior Depression30 5Percent Experiencing Major Depression FIGURE 16.8 Students Experiencing Depression Source: Alloy et al. (2006).Summary 451
KEY TERMS depressive cognitive triad (p. 446) depressive schema (p. 446)negative cognitive style (p. 450) MEDIA RESOURCES Visit our website. Go to www.cengage. com/psychology/burger to find online resources directly linked to your book, including quizzing, glossary, flashcards, and more.452 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach
GLOSSARY absorption The ability to become highly involved in sensory and imaginative experiences. achievement goals Targets people aspire to in achievement situations. affect intensity The strength or degree to which people typically experience their emotions. anal stage The psychosexual stage of development in which the anal region is the primary erogenous zone. androgyny A personality trait consisting of masculine as well as feminine characteristics. anima/animus The archetype that is the feminine side of the male (anima) or the masculine side of the female (animus). archetypes Primordial images that predispose us to comprehend the world in a particular manner. avoidance coping strategies Coping strategies designed to distract us from thinking about the source of anxiety. behavior modification Therapy procedures based on operant conditioning and classical conditioning principles. behavioral approach system A hypothetical biological system that is focused on seeking out and achievingpleasurable goals.behavioral inhibition system A hypothetical biological system that is focused on avoiding dangerous andunpleasant experiences. behavioral validation A method for establishing a test ’s validity by predicting behavior from test scores. Big Five The five basic dimensions of personality found in many factor analytic studies. case study method An in-depth examination of one person or one group. catharsis A release of tension or anxiety. central traits The 5 to 10 traits that best describe a person’ s personality. cerebral asymmetry Higher levels of brain activity in one cerebral hemisphere than the other. classical conditioning Learning resulting from pairing a conditioned stimulus with a new, unconditioned stimulus. collective unconscious The part of the unconscious mind containing thoughts, images, and psychic characteristics common to all members of a culture. collectivist culture Culture that emphasizes the importance of belonging to a larger group, such as a family, tribe, or nation. conditional/unconditional positive regard Acceptance and respect forpeople either only when they act as we desire (conditional) or regardless of their behavior (unconditional). congruent validity A method for establishing a test ’s validity by correlating the test scores with other measures of the same construct. conscious In Freud ’s topographic model, the part of personality that contains the thoughts we are currently aware of. construct validity The extent to which a test measures the hypothetical construct it is designed to measure. contingencies of self-worth The domains of self-concept an individual uses to evaluate his or her self. coping strategies Conscious efforts to reduce anxiety in the face of a perceived threat. correlation coefficient A statistic that indicates the strength and direction of a relationship between two variables. defense mechanisms Devices the ego uses to keep threatening material out of awareness and thereby reduce or avoidanxiety. defensive pessimism The tendency to attend to and worry about failure on upcoming tasks in a strategic effort tomotivate oneself to do well. deficiency motive A need that is reduced when the object of the need is attained. 453
denial A defense mechanism in which a person denies the existence of a fact. dependent variable The experimental variable measured by the experimenter and used to compare groups. depressive cognitive triad Three elements that describe a depressed person’ s cognitions: negative views of the self, pessimism, and interpretingevents in a negative manner. depressive schema A cognitive structure that allows people to readily make negative associations. discriminant validity A method for establishing a test ’s validity by demonstrating that its scores do not correlate with the scores of theoreticallyunrelated measures. discrimination A learned tendency to respond only to stimuli that result in reinforcement and not to similar, but unrewarded, stimuli. displacement A defense mechanism in which a response is directed at a nonthreatening target instead of the unconsciously preferred one. dispositional optimism The extent to which a person typically adopts an optimistic or pessimistic approach to dealing with life ’s challenges. egoIn Freud ’s structural model, the part of personality that considers externalreality while mediating between the demands of the id and the superego. emotional affectivity The extent to which people typically experience positive and negative emotions. emotional expressiveness The extent to which people outwardly express their emotions. emotion-focused strategies Coping strategies designed to reduce emotional distress. evaluation apprehension A strong concern about receiving negative evaluations from others. face validity A method for establishing a test ’s validity in which test items appear to measure what the test was designed to measure. factor analysis A statistical procedure used to determine the number of dimensions in a data set. fixation Tying up psychic energy at one psychosexual stage, which results in adult behaviors characteristic of that stage.free association A procedure used in psychoanalysis in which patients say whatever comes into their mind. Freudian slip A seemingly innocent misstatement that reveals unconscious associations. fully functioning person A psycho- logically healthy individual who is able to enjoy life as completely as possible. generalization The tendency to respond to stimuli similar to the one used in the initial conditioning. goodness of fit model A model proposing that a child performs best when the demands of the environmentmatch with his or her temperament. growth need A need that leads to personal growth and that persists after the need object is attained. hierarchy of needs In Maslow ’s theory, the order in which human needs demand attention. Human Figure Drawing test A projective test in which test takers are asked simply to draw a person. hypothesis A formal prediction about the relationship between two or more variables that is logically derived from a theory. idIn Freud ’s structural model, the part of personality concerned with immediate gratification of needs. idiographic approach A method of studying personality through in-depth analysis of one individual and the dimensions relevant to that person’ s personality. independent variable The experimental variable used to divide participants into groups. individualistic culture Culture that places great emphasis on individual needs and accomplishments. inhibited/uninhibited children Inhibited children show strong anxiety about novel and unfamiliar situations; uninhibited children show very little ofthis anxiety. intellectualization A defense mechanism in which the emotional content of threatening material is removed before itis brought into awareness. internal consistency The extent to which test items are interrelated and thus appear to measure the same construct. learned helplessness The cognitive, motivational, and emotional deficits thatfollow a perceived lack of control over important aversive events. libido The limited amount of psychic energy that powers mental activity. locus of control A personality trait that divides people along a continuum according to the extent to which theybelieve what happens to them and others is controllable. manipulated independent variable An independent variable for which participants have been randomly assigned to an experimental group. masculinity –femininity A personality trait indicating the extent to which a person possesses sex-typed character- istics, with masculine characteristics atone end of the trait continuum and feminine characteristics at the other end. need for Achievement The motive to engage in and succeed at entrepreneurial achievement behavior. negative cognitive style A style of processing information in which people attribute their problems to stable and global causes, anticipate dreadful consequences, and believe the problemsreflect their own personal shortcomings. neodissociation theory Hilgard ’s theory, which maintains that consciousness is divided into aware and unaware parts during hypnosis. nomothetic approach A method of understanding personality that compares many people along the same personality dimensions. nonmanipulated independent variable An independent variable for which condition assignment is determined by a characteristic of the participant. observational learning Learning that results from watching or hearing about a person modeling the behavior. operant conditioning Learning resulting from the response an organism receives following a behavior. optimal experience A state of happiness and satisfaction characterized by absorption in a challenging and personally rewarding task. oral stage The psychosexual stage of development in which the mouth, lips, and tongue are the primary erogenous zones. personal constructs In Kelly ’s theory, the bipolar cognitive structures through which people process information.454 GLOSSARY
personal narratives Assessment procedure that asks individuals to provide autobiographical descriptions of important events in their lives. personality Consistent behavior patterns and intrapersonal processes originating within the individual. person-by-situation approach An approach to understanding behavior that maintains behavior is a function of the person as well as the situation. phallic stage The psychosexual stage of development in which the genital region is the primary erogenous zone and in which the Oedipus complexdevelops. possible selves Cognitive representations of the kind of people we think we might become some day. preconscious In Freud ’s topographic model, the part of personality that contains thoughts that can be broughtinto awareness with little difficulty. primordial images The images that make up the collective unconscious.problem-focused strategies Coping strategies directed at taking care of the problem causing the anxiety. projection A defense mechanism in which one ’s own unconscious thoughts and impulses are attributed to other people. projective tests Tests designed to assess unconscious material by asking test takers to respond to ambiguous stimuli. psychoanalysis The system of psychotherapy developed by Freud that focuses on uncovering the unconscious material responsible for a patient ’s disorder. psychogenic need In Murray ’s theory, a relatively stable predisposition toward a type of action. psychosexual stages of development The innate sequence of development made up of stages characterized by primaryerogenous zones and sexual desires. Q-Sort An assessment procedure in which test takers distribute personal descriptions along a continuum. rational emotive therapy A psychotherapy procedure introduced byEllis that examines the irrational reasoning causing emotional problems. reaction formation A defense mechanism in which people act in a manner opposite to their unconscious desires. reciprocal determinism The notion that external determinants of behavior, internal determinants of behavior, and behavior all influence one another. reliability The extent to which a test measures consistently. repression A defense mechanism in which the ego pushes threatening material out of awareness and into the unconscious. Rorschach inkblot test A projective test in which test takers are asked to describe what they see in a series of inkblots. schema A hypothetical cognitive structure used to process information. self-actualization A state of personal growth in which people fulfill their true potential. self-disclosure The act of revealing intimate information about oneself to another person. self-efficacy A person’ s expectancy that he or she can successfully perform a given behavior. self-esteem Evaluation of one ’s self- concept, usually measured in terms of a relatively stable and global assessment of how a person feels about him- or herself. self-regulation The ability to develop and apply rewards and punishments for internal standards of behavior. self-schema A schema consisting of aspects of a person ’s life most important to him or her. shadow The archetype that contains the evil side of humanity. social anxiety A trait dimension indicating the extent to which people experience anxiety during socialencounters or when anticipating socialencounters. social desirability The extent to which test takers tend to respond to items in a manner that presents them in a positivelight.statistical significance The likelihood that a research finding represents a genuine effect rather than a chance fluctuation of measurement. striving for superiority The primary motivational force in Adler ’s theory, which is the person’ s effort to overcome feelings of inferiority.structural model Freud ’s model of personality that divides personality into the id, the ego, and the superego. subception The perception of information at a less-than-conscious level. sublimation A defense mechanism in which threatening unconscious impulses are channeled into socially acceptable behaviors. superego In Freud ’s structural model, the part of personality that represents society ’s values. temperaments General behavioral predispositions present in infancy andassumed to be inherited. Thanatos The self-destructive (death) instinct, which is often turned outward in the form of aggression. Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) A projective test in which test takers are asked to tell stories about a series ofambiguous pictures. theory A general statement about the relationship between constructs or events. topographic model Freud ’s original model of personality structure, in which personality is divided into three differentlevels of awareness. trait A dimension of personality used to categorize people according to the degree to which they manifest aparticular characteristic. twin-study method A procedure for examining the role of genetics on personality in which pairs of monozygotic and dizygotic twins are compared. unconscious In Freud ’s topographic model, the part of personality that contains material that cannot easily be brought into awareness. validity The extent to which a test measures what it is designed to measure.GLOSSARY 455
REFERENCES Abramson , L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,87,4 9 –74. Accortt , E. E., & Allen, J. J. B. (2006). Frontal EEG asymmetry andpremenstrual dysphoric symptom- atology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,115, 179 –184. Ainsworth , M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist ,44, 709–716. Ainsworth , M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Aldwin , C. M., & Revenson, T. A. (1987). Does coping help? A reex-amination of the relation between coping and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,53, 337 –348. Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223 –265). New York: Guilford. Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Hogan, M. E., Whitehouse, W. G., Rose,D. T., Robinson, M. S., et al. (2000). The Temple-Wisconsin cog-nitive vulnerability to depression project: Lifetime history of Axis Ipsychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk fordepression. Journal of Abnormal Ps ychology ,10 9 ,4 0 3 –418. Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Panzarella, C., & Rose, D. T. (2006). Prospective incidence of first onsets and recurrences ofdepression in individuals at highand low cognitive risk for depres- sion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,115, 145 –156. Allport , G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality . New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Allport , G. W. (1965). Letters from Jenny . New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. Allport , G. W. (1967). Gordon W. Allport. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 5, pp. 3 –25). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Allport , G. W. (1968). The person in psychology: Selected essays . Boston: Beacon. Almagor , M., Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The Big Seven model:A cross-cultural replication and further exploration of the basic dimensions of natural languagetrait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 69, 300–307.Altman , I. (1975). The environment and social behavior . Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Altman , I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The develop-ment of interpersonal relationships . New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Amir , N., Beard, C., & Bower, E. (2005). Interpretation bias andsocial anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,29, 433 –443. Amirkhan, J. H., Risinger, R. T., & Swickert, R. J. (1995). Extraversion: A “hidden ”person- ality factor in coping? Journal of Personality ,63, 189 –212. Andersen, B. L., Cyranowski, J. M., & Espindle, D. (1999). Men ’s sexual self-schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,76, 645–661. Anderson , C. A. (1999). Attributional style, depression, and loneliness:A cross-cultural comparison of American and Chinese students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,15, 482 –499. Anderson , C. A. (2004). An update on the effects of playing violent video games. Journal of Adolescence ,27, 113–122. Anderson , C. A., & Anderson, K. B. (1998). Temperature and aggres- sion: Paradox, controversy, and a (fairly) clear picture. In R. Geen & 456
E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression: Theories, research, and implications for public policy (pp. 247 –298). New York: Academic Press. Anderson , C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive af-fect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science ,12, 353–359. Anderson , C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002a). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology ,53, 27–51. Anderson , C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002b). The effects of media violence on society. Science ,295, 2377 –2378. Anderson , C. A., Carnagey, N. L., & Eubanks, J. (2003). Exposure toviolent media: The effects of songswith violent lyrics on aggressive thoughts and feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 84, 960–971. Anderson , C. A., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C. (2004). Violent video games:Specific effects of violent content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology ,36, 199 –249. Anderson , C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior inthe laboratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 772 –790. Anderson , C. A., & Harvey, R. J. (1988). Discriminating between problems in living: An examination of measures of depression, loneli- ness, shyness, and social anxiety. Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology ,6, 482 –491. Anderson , C. A., & Huesmann, L. R. (2003). Human aggression: Asocial-cognitive view. In M.A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 296 –323). London: Sage. Ansbacher , H. L., & Ansbacher, R. R. (Eds.). (1956). The individual psychology of Alfred Adler . New York: Basic Books. Antill , J. K. (1983). Sex role comple- mentarity versus similarity in mar- ried couples. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology ,45, 145–155. Antonen, M. (1993, June 25). Sparky thrives on high anxiety. USA Today , p. 3C. Archer , J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social role and evolutionary explanationscompatible? American Psychologist ,51, 909 –917. Archibald , F. S., Bartholomew, K., & Marx, R. (1995). Loneliness inearly adolescence: A test of thecognitive discrepancy model of loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,21, 296 –301. Arkin , A. M., Antrobus, J. S., & Ellman, S. J. (1978). The mind in sleep: Psychology and psychophysiology . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Aron , A., Aron, E. N., & Allen, J. (1998). Motivations for unrecipro-cated love. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,24, 787 –796. Arthur , W., & Graziano, W. G. (1996). The five-factor model, conscien-tiousness, and driving accident in- volvement. Journal of Personality , 64, 593–618. Asendorpf , J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects on social rela-tionships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,74, 1531 –1544. Aserinsky , E., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of eye motility and concomitant phenom-ena during sleep. Science ,118, 273–274. Ashton , M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practicaladvantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,11, 150–166. Ashton , M. C., Lee, K., Goldberg, L. R., & de Vries, R. E. (2009). Higher order factors of personality: Do they exist? Personality and Soci al Psychology Review ,13, 79–91 . Aspinwall , L. G., & Brunhart, S. M. (1996). Distinguishing optimismfrom denial: Optimistic beliefs pre- dict attention to health threats. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,22, 993 –1003. Aspinwall , L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1992). Modeling cognitive adaptation: Alongitudinal investigation of theimpact of individual differences andcoping on college adjustment and performance. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology ,63, 989–1003. Aube , J. (2008). Balancing concern for other with concern for self: Links between unmitigated communion,communion, and psychological well-being. Journal of Personality , 76, 101–133. Aube , J., Norcliffe, H., Craig, J., & Koestner, R. (1995). Gender char-acteristics and adjustment-related outcomes: Questioning themasculinity model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,21, 284–295. Austenfeld , J. L., & Stanton, A. L. (2004). Coping through emotionalapproach: A new look at emotion, coping, and health-related out- comes. Journal of Personality ,72, 1335 –1363. Bagby , R. M., Rogers, R., Nicholson, R. A., Buis, T., Seeman, M. V., & Rector, N. A. (1997). Effectiveness of the MMPI-2 validity indicatorsin the detection of defensive re- sponding in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Assessment , 9, 406–413. Baker , E. L., & Nash, M. R. (2008). Psychoanalytic approaches to clini- cal hypnosis. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 439 – 456). New York: Oxford University Press. Baker , L. A., & Daniels, D. (1990). Nonshared environmental influ-ences and personality differencesin adult twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 58, 103–110. Baker , S. R. (2007). Dispositional opti- mism and health status, symptomsand behaviours: Assessing idio- thetic relationships using a pro- spective daily diary approach. Psychology and Health ,22, 431–455. Baldwin , M. W., & Main, K. J. (2001). Social anxiety and the cued activa-tion of relational knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 1637 –1647. Bandura , A. (1965). Influences of mod- els’reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative re-sponses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,1, 589 –595. Bandura , A. (1973). Aggression: A so- cial learning analysis . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.REFERENCES 457
Bandura , A. (1977a). Social learning theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura , A. (1977b). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of be- havioral change. Psychological Review ,84, 191 –215. Bandura , A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura , A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control . New York: Freeman. Bandura , A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology ,52, 1–26. Bandura , A. (2006). Toward a psychol- ogy of human agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science ,1, 164–180. Bandura , A., Adams, N. E., & Beyer, J. (1977). Cognitive processes medi-ating behavioral change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 35, 125–139. Bandura , A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996).Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy beliefs on academic functioning. Child Development ,67, 1206 –1222. Bandura , A., & Bussey, K. (2004). On broadening the cognitive, motiva- tional, and sociostructural scope of theorizing about gender develop-ment and functioning: Comment on Martin, Ruble, and Szkrybalo (2002). Psychological Bulletin ,130, 691–701. Bandura , A., Pastorelli, C., Barbaranelli, C., & Caprara, G. V. (1999). Self- efficacy pathways to childhood de- pression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,76, 258 –269. Barber , T. S., & Calverley, D. S. (1964). Toward a theory of hypnotic be-havior: Effects on suggestibility ofdefining the situation as hypnosis and defining responses to sugges- tion as easy. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology ,68, 585–592. Barber , T. X. (1969). Hypnosis: A scientific approach . New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Barber , T. X. (1999). A comprehensive three-dimensional theory of hypno- sis. In I. Kirsch, A. Capafons, E. Cardena-Buelna, & S. Amigo(Eds.), Clinical hypnosis and self- regulation: Cognitive-behavioralperspectives (pp. 21 –48). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bardos, A. N., & Powell, S. (2001). Human figure drawings and theDraw-A-Person: Screening proce- dures for emotional disturbances. In W. I. Dorfman & M. Hersen(Eds.), Understanding psychologi- cal assessment (pp. 275 –294). New York: Plenum. Bar-Haim , Y., Fox, N. A., Benson, B., Guyer, A. E., Williams, A., Nelson,E. E., et al. (2009). Neural corre- lates of reward processing in ado- lescents with a history of inhibitedtemperament. Psychological Science ,20, 1009 –1018. Barlett , C., Branch, O., Rodeheffer, C., & Harris, R. (2009). How long dothe short-term violent video gameeffects last? Aggressive Behavior , 35, 225–236. Barlett , C. P., & Rodeheffer, C. (2009). Effects of realism on extended vio-lent and nonviolent video game play on aggressive thoughts, feel- ings, and physiological arousal. Aggressive Behavior ,35, 213 –224. Barlow , D. H. (1988). Anxiety and its disorders . New York: Guilford. Baron , R. A. (1978a). The influence of hostile and nonhostile humor upon physical aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,4, 77–80. Baron , R. A. (1978b). Aggression- inhibiting influence of sexual be- havior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,36, 189 –197. Barrett , M. S., & Berman, J. S. (2001). Is psychotherapy more effective whentherapists disclose information about themselves? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,69, 597 –603. Barrett , P., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1984). The assessment of personality factors across 25 countries. Personality and Individual Differences ,5, 615 –632. Barrick , M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta- analysis. Personnel Psychology ,44, 1–26. Barrick , M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and per-formance at the beginning of the new millennium: What do we knowand where to we go next? International Journal of Selection and Assessment ,91,9–30.Barrick , M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1993). Conscientiousness and performance of sales representatives: Test of themediating effects of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology ,78, 715–722. Barrick , M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1994). Antecedents of involuntary turnover due to a reduction in force. Personnel Psychology ,47, 515 –535. Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals andoptimal motivation: Testing multi- ple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 80, 706–722. Bartholomew , K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspec- tive. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships ,7, 147 –178. Bartholomew , K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles amongyoung adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 61, 226–244. Bartholomew , K., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Methods of assessing adult attachment. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 25 –45). New York: Guilford. Bartholow , B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic vio-lent video game exposure and de-sensitization to violence: Behavioral and event-related brain potential data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology ,42, 532 –539. Basgall , J. A., & Snyder, C. R. (1988). Excuses in waiting: External locusof control and reactions to success-failure feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 656–662. Bates, J. E., Wachs, T. D., & Emde, R. N. (1994). Toward practical usesfor biological concepts of tempera- ment. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the interface of biol- ogy and behavior (pp. 275 –306). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bauder , D. (2005, November 29). Violence surging on networks. San Jose Mercury News , p. 2E. Bauer , J. J., & Bonanno, G. A. (2001). I can, I do, I am: The narrative dif- ferentiation of self-efficacy and other self-evaluations while458 REFERENCES
adapting to bereavement. Journal of Research in Personality ,35, 424–448. Baumeister , R. F. (1991). On the stabil- ity of variability: Retest reliability of metatraits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,17, 633–639. Baumeister , R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin ,117, 497–529. Baumeister , R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1988). Metatraits. Journal of Personality , 56, 571–598. Baumeister , R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1990). Anxiety and social exclusion. Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology ,9, 165 –195. Bauminger , N., Finzi-Dottan, R., Chason, S., & Har-Even, D. (2008).Intimacy in adolescent friendship:The roles of attachment, coherence,and self-disclosure. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships , 25, 409–428. Bechtle , R. (1984). C. G. Jung and the religion of the unconscious. In J. Heaney (Ed.), Psyche and spirit (pp. 138 –163). New York: Paulist. Beck , A. T. (1972). Depression: Causes and treatments . Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Beck , A. T., & Bhar, S. S. (2009). ‘Effectiveness of long-term psycho- dynamic psychotherapy: A meta-analysis ’: Comment. Journal of the American Medical Association , 301, 931. Beevers , C. G., Strong, D. R., Meyer, B., Pilkonis, P. A., & Miller, I. W. (2007). Efficiently assessing nega- tive cognition in depression: Anitem response theory analysis of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Psychological Assessment ,19, 199–209. Bell, R. C. (1990). Analytic issues in the use of Repertory Grid Technique. In G. J. Neimeyer & R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.), Advances in per- sonal construct psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 25–48). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross- situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review ,81,5 0 6 –520. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journalof Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,42, 155 –162. Bem, S. L. (1977). On the utility of alternative procedures for assessingpsychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,45, 196 –205. Bem, S. L., & Lenney, E. (1976). Sex- typing and the avoidance of cross- sex behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,33,4 8 –54. Bem, S. L., Martyna, W., & Watson, C. (1976). Sex typing and androgyny:Further explorations of the expres- sive domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,34, 1016 –1023. Benassi , V. A., Sweeney, P. D., & Dufour, C. L. (1988). Is there a re- lationship between locus of control orientation and depression? Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,97, 357–367. Benet-Martinez , V., & Oishi, S. (2008). Personality and culture. In O.P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of per- sonality (3rd ed., pp. 542 –567). New York: Guilford. Benet-Martinez , V., & Waller, N. G. (1997). Further evidence for the cross-cultural generality of the Big Seven factor model: Indigenous andimported Spanish personality con- structs. Journal of Personality ,65, 567–598. Bengston , P. L., & Grotevant, H. D. (1999). The individuality and con-nectedness Q-Sort: A measure for assessing individuality and con-nectedness in dyadic relationships. Personal Relationships ,6, 213–225. Benham, G., Woody, E. Z., Wilson, K. S., & Nash, M. R. (2006).Expect the unexpected: Ability, at- titude, and responsiveness to hyp- nosis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,91, 342 –350. Ben Hamida , S., Mineka, S., & Bailey, J. M. (1998). Sex differences inperceived controllability of matevalue: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,75, 953 –966. Bennett , P., Moore, L., Smith, A., Murphy, S., & Smith, C. (1994). Health locus of control and value for health as predictors of dietary behaviour. Psychology and Health , 10,4 1–54. Berg, J. H., & Peplau, L. A. (1982). Loneliness: The relationship ofself-disclosure and androgyny. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,8, 624 –630. Berkman , L. F. (1995). The role of social relations in health promotion. Psychosomatic Medicine ,57, 245–254. Berkow , I. (2002, March 27). True heroism outside the lines. New York Times , p. C15. Berkowitz , L. (1970). Aggressive humor as a stimulus to aggressive re- sponses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,16, 710 –717. Berkowitz , L. (1989). The frustration- aggression hypothesis: Anexamination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin ,106,5 9 –73. Berkowitz , L. (1994). Is something missing? Some observationsprompted by the cognitive-neoassociationist view of anger and emotional aggression. In L. R. Huesmann (Ed.), Aggressive behavior: Current perspectives (pp. 35 –57). New York: Plenum. Berkowitz , L. (1998). Affective aggres- sion: The role of stress, pain, andnegative affect. In R. G. Geen &E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human ag- gression: Theories, research, and implications for social policy (pp. 49–72). San Diego: Academic Press. Berkowitz , L. (2008). On the consider- ation of automatic as well as con- trolled psychological processes in aggression. Aggressive Behavior , 34, 117–129. Berman , M. E., McCloskey, M. S., Fanning, J. R., Schumacher, J. A.,& Coccaro, E. F. (2009). Serotoninaugmentation reduces response to attack in aggressive individuals. Psychological Science ,20, 714–720. Berman , M. I., & Frazier, P. A. (2005). Relationship power and betrayal experience as predictors of reac- tions to infidelity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,31, 1617 –1627. Berry , D. S., & Hansen, J. S. (1996). Positive affect, negative affect, andsocial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 71, 796–809. Berry , D. S., & Willingham, J. K. (1997). Affective traits, responses to conflict, and satisfaction in romantic relationships. Journal of Research in Pe rsonality ,31,5 6 4 –576. Berry , D. S., Willingham, J. K., & Thayer, C. A. (2000). Affect andREFERENCES 459
personality as predictors of conflict and closeness in young adults ’ friendships. Journal of Research in Personality ,34,8 4 –107. Bettencourt , B. A., Talley, A. E., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analyticreview. Psychological Bulletin ,132, 751–777. Bettencourt , B. A., Talley, A. E., Molix, L., Schlegel, R., & Westgate, S. J.(2008). Rural and urban breast cancer patients: Health locus of control and psychological adjust-ment. Psycho-Oncology ,17, 932–939. Bianchi , E. C. (1988). Jungian psychol- ogy and religious experience. InR. L. Moore (Ed.), Carl Jung and Christian spirituality (pp. 16 –37). New York: Paulist. Biederman , J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Herot, C.,Friedman, D., Snidman, N., et al. (2001). Further evidence of associ- ation between behavioral inhibitionand social anxiety in children. American Journal of Psychiatry , 158, 1673 –1679. Blankenship , V., Vega, C. M., Ramos, E., Romero, K., Warren, K.,Keenan, K., et al. (2006). Using the multifaceted Rasch model to im- prove the TAT/PSE measure of needfor Achievement. Journal of Personality Assessment ,86, 100–114. Bleidorn , W., Kandler, C., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M.(2009). Patterns and sources of adult personality development: Growth curve analysis of the NEOPI-R Scales in a longitudinal twin study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,97, 142 –155. Block , J. (1978). The Q-Sort method in personality assessment and psychi- atric research . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Block , J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to person- ality description. Psychological Bulletin ,117, 187 –215. Block , J. (2008). The Q-Sort in charac- ter appraisal . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Block , J. H., Gjerde, P. F., & Block, J. H. (1991). Personality antece-dents of depressive tendencies in 18-year-olds: A prospective study.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,60, 726 –738. Bloise , S. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2007). Memory for emotional and neutral information: Gender and individualdifferences in emotional sensitivity. Memory ,15, 192 –204. Bodner , E., & Mikulincer, M. (1998). Learned helplessness and the oc- currence of depressive-like and paranoid-like responses: The role of attentional focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1010 –1023. Boldizar , J. P., Perry, D. G., & Perry, L. C. (1989). Outcome values andaggression. Child Development ,60, 571–579. Bond , M. (1992). An empirical study of defensive style: The Defense StyleQuestionnaire. In G. Vaillant (Ed.), Ego mechanisms of defense: A guide for clinicians and researchers (pp. 127 –158). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Booth-Kewley , S., & Vickers, R. R. (1994). Associations between major domains of personality and health behavior. Journal of Personality , 62, 281–298. Borkenau , P., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M. (2001).Genetic and environmental influ-ences on observed personality: Evidence from the German obser- vational study of adult twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,80, 655 –668. Bowers , S. P. (1999). Gender role iden- tity and the caregiving experience of widowed men. Sex Roles ,41, 645–655. Bowlby , J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment . New York: Basic Books. Bowlby , J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anger and anxiety . New York: Basic Books. Bowlby , J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness, and depres- sion . New York: Basic Books. Bradbury , T. N., Campbell, S. M., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Longitudinal and behavioral analy- sis of masculinity and femininity in marriage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,68, 328–341. Braffman , W., & Kirsch, I. (1999). Imaginative suggestibility and hyp-notizability: An empirical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,77, 578 –587.Brebner , J., & Cooper, C. (1978). Stimulus- or response-induced ex- citation: A comparison of behavior in introverts and extraverts. Journal of Research in Personality ,12, 306–311. Brennan , K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure- ment of adult attachment. InJ. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46 –76). New York: Guilford. Brennan , K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1993). Attachment styles and parental divorce. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage ,21, 161 –175. Brennan , K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult attachment, affect regulation, and romantic re-lationship functioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,21, 267–283. Bridges , N. A. (2001). Therapist ’s self- disclosure: Expanding the comfortzone. Psychotherapy ,38,2 1 –30. Briggs , S. R. (1989). The optimal level of measurement of personality con-structs. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor(Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging direc- tions (pp. 246 –260). New York: Springer-Verlag. Brissette , I., & Cohen, S. (2002). The contribution of individual differ- ences in hostility to the associations between daily interpersonal con-flict, affect, and sleep. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,28, 1265 –1274. Brissette , I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2002). The role of optimism in social network development, coping, and psychological adjust-ment during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,82, 102 –111. Britt, T. W. (1993). Metatraits: Evidence relevant to the validity of the con- struct and its implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,65, 544 –562. Britt, T. W., & Shepperd, J. A. (1999). Trait relevance and trait assess- ment. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,3, 108 –122. Brockner , J. (1979). The effects of self- esteem, success-failure, and self-consciousness on task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,37, 1732 –1741. Brockner , J., Derr, W. R., & Laing, W. N. (1987). Self-esteem and460 REFERENCES
reactions to negative feedback: Toward greater generalizability. Journal of Research in Personality , 21, 318–333. Brody , J. E. (2000, April 25). Memories of things that never were. New York Times , p. D8. Brody , L. R., Rozek, M. K., & Muten, E. O. (1985). Age, sex, and indi- vidual differences in children ’s de- fensive styles. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology ,14, 132 –138. Brooks-Gunn , J., & Fisch, M. (1980). Psychological androgyny and col- lege students ’judgments of mental health. Sex Roles ,6, 575 –580. Brown , J., & Inouye, D. K. (1978). Learned helplessness throughmodeling: The role of perceived similarity in competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 36, 900–908. Brown , J. D., & Dutton, K. A. (1995). The thrill of victory, the complexity of defeat: Self-esteem and people ’s emotional reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,68, 712 –722. Brown , J. D., & Gallagher, F. M. (1992). Coming to terms with fail-ure: Private self-enhancement and public self-effacement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 28,3–22. Brown , J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001). Great expectations: Optimism and pessimism in achievement settings.In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pe ssimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 239 –25 5). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Brown , J. D., & Smart, S. A. (1991). The self and social conduct: Linking self-representations to prosocial be-havior. Journal of Personality and So cial Psychology ,60,3 6 8 –375. Brown , R. J., & Donderi, D. C. (1986). Dream content and self-reported well-being among recurrent drea- mers, past-recurrent dreamers, and nonrecurrent dreamers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 50, 612–623. Brown , S. (1998, April 20). Television violence stays constant. Broadcasting and Cable ,128, 20. Bruch , M. A., Hamer, R. J., & Heimberg, R. G. (1995). Shynessand public self-consciousness:Additive or interactive relation withsocial interaction? Journal of Personality ,63,4 7 –63.Bruch , M. A., Kaflowitz, N. G., & Pearl, L. (1988). Mediated and nonme- diated relationships of personality components to loneliness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology , 6, 346–355. Brunstein , J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two separate but inter-acting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,89, 205–222. Brunstein , J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2004). Assessing individual differ- ences in achievement motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Research in Personality , 38, 536–555. Buchholz, E. S., & Helbraun, E. (1999). A psychobiological developmentalmodel for an “alonetime ”need in infancy. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic ,63, 143 –158. Buckley , M. E., Klein, D. N., Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., & Moerk, K. C. (2002). Development and validation of a Q-Sort procedure to assess temperament and behavior inpreschool-age children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology ,31, 525 –539. Buhler , C., & Allen, M. (1972). Introduction to humanistic psy- chology . Monterey, CA: Brooks/ Cole. Bulkeley , K. (1997). An introduction to the psychology of dreaming . Westport, CT: Praeger. Bullock , W. A., & Gilliland, K. (1993). Eysenck ’s arousal theory of introversion-extraversion: A con- verging measures investigation. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,64, 113 –123. Bunde , J., & Suls, J. (2006). A quanti- tative analysis of the relationship between the Cook-Medley Hostility Scale and traditional coronary artery disease risk factors. Health Psychology ,25, 493 –500. Burger , J. M. (1984). Desire for control, locus of control, and proneness todepression. Journal of Personality , 52,7 1–89. Burger , J. M. (1995). Individual differ- ences in preference for solitude. Journal of Research in Personality , 29,8 5–108. Burger , J. M. (1998). Solitude. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), The ency- clopedia of mental health (pp. 563 –569). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Burger , J. M., & Arkin, R. M. (1980). Prediction, control and learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,38, 482–491. Burger , J. M., & Cosby, M. (1999). Do women prefer dominant men? The case of the missing control condi- tion. Journal of Research in Personality ,33, 358 –368. Burns , J. W., & Evon, D. (2007). Common and specific process fac-tors in cardiac rehabilitation:Independent and interactive effects of the working alliance and self- efficacy. Health Psychology ,26, 684–692. Burns , M. O., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1989). Explanatory style across the life span: Evidence for stability over 52 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,56, 471 –477. Bushman , B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the effectsof violent media on aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,69, 950 –960. Bushman , B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,28, 724 –731. Bushman , B. J., Baumeister, R. F., & Phillips, C. M. (2001). Do peopleaggress to improve their mood?Catharsis beliefs, affect regulationopportunity, and aggressive re- sponding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,81,1 7 –32. Bushman , B. J., Baumeister, R. F., & Stack, A. D. (1999). Catharsis, ag-gression, and persuasive influence: Self-fulfilling or self-defeating prophecies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,76, 367–376. Bushman , B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., &Miller, N. (2005). Chewing on it can chew you up: Effects of rumi- nation on triggered displaced ag-gression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,88, 969 –983. Bushman , B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2001). Effects of televised violenceon aggression. In D. G. Singer &J. L. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of children and the media (pp. 223 – 254). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety . San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.REFERENCES 461
Buss, A. H. (1991). The EAS theory of temperament. In J. Strelau & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations in temperament (pp. 43 –60). London: Plenum. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1986). The EAS approach to temperament. InR. Plomin & J. Dunn (Eds.), The study of temperament: Changes, continuities and challenges (pp. 67–79). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,54, 616 –628. Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences ,12,1–49. Buss, D. M. (1990). The evolution of anxiety and social exclusion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,9, 196 –201. Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary per- sonality psychology. Annual Review of Psychology ,42, 459–491. Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psy- chology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry ,6,1–30. Buss, D. M. (1997). Evolutionary foun- dations of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychol-ogy (pp. 317 –344). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Buss, D. M. (2009). The great struggles of life: Darwin and the emergenceof evolutionary psychology. American Psychologist ,64, 140–148. Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selec- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,50, 559 –570. Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., &Wakefield, J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist , 53, 533–548. Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). From vigilance to violence:Mate retention tactics in marriedcouples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,72, 346 –361.Bussey , K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review ,106, 676–713. Butcher , J. N. (Ed.). (2006). MMPI-2: A practitioner ’s guide . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Butler , A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review ofmeta-analyses. Clinical Psychology Review ,26,1 7 –31. Butler , J. M. (1968). Self-ideal congru- ence in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice ,5,1 3 –17. Byrne , D. (1964). Repression- sensitization as a dimension of personality. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personal- ity research (Vol. 1, pp. 169 –220). New York: Academic Press. Cacioppo , J. T., Ernest, J. M., Burleson, M. H., McClintock, M. K., Malarkey, W. B., Hawkley, L. C., et al. (2000). Lonely traits andconcomitant physiological pro- cesses: The MacArthur Social Neuroscience Studies. International Journal of Psychophysiology ,35, 143–154. Cacioppo , J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Berntson, G. G., Ernst, J. M., Gibbs, A. C., Stickgold, R., &Hobson, J. A. (2002). Do lonely days invade the night? Potential social modulation of sleep effi- ciency. Psychological Science ,13, 384–387. Cacioppo , J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski,R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine ,64, 407–417. Cacioppo , J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M., Berntson, G. G., Nouriani, B., et al. (2006). Loneliness within a nomologicalnet: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Research in Personality , 40, 1054 –1085. Cacioppo , J. T., & Patrick, B. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection . New York: Norton. Cain, D. J., & Seeman, J. (2002). Humanistic psychotherapies:Handbook of research and practice .Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Cairns , R. B. (1986). An evolutionary and developmental perspective on aggressive patterns. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, & R. Iannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins (pp. 58 –87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Caldwell , D. F., & Burger, J. M. (1998). Personality characteristics of job applicants and success in screeninginterviews. Personnel Psychology , 51, 119–136. Camara , W. J., Nathan, J. S., & Puente, A. E. (2000). Psychological test us-age: Implications in professional psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice , 31, 141–154. Cameron , L. D., & Nicholls, G. (1998). Expression of stressful experiences through writing: Effects of a self- regulation manipulation for pessi-mists and optimists. Health Psychology ,17,8 4 –92. Campbell , J. B. (1983). Differential re- lationships of extraversion, impul- sivity, and sociability to study habits. Journal of Research in Personality ,17, 308 –314. Campbell , J. B., & Hawley, C. W. (1982). Study habits and Eysenck ’s theory of extraversion-introversion. Journal of Research in Personality , 16, 139–146. Campbell , J. D., & Fairey, P. J. (1985). Effects of self-esteem, hypothetical explanations, and verbalization of expectancies on future perfor-mance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,48, 1097 –1111. Campbell , L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2005). Perceptions of conflict and support in romantic relationships: The role of attach-ment anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 88, 510–531. Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg,C. (1983). Socioemotional devel- opment. In M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy and psychobiology (pp. 783 –915). New York: Wiley. Canli, T. (2004). Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neu-roticism: Learning from individual differences in emotion processing.462 REFERENCES
Journal of Personality ,72, 1105 –1132. Canli , T. (2006). Genomic imaging of extraversion. In T. Canli (Ed.), Biology of personality and individ- ual differences (pp. 93 –115). New York: Guilford. Cantor , N., Norem, J. K., Niedenthal, P. M., Langston, C. A., & Brower,A. M. (1987). Life tasks, self- concept ideals, and cognitive strat- egies in a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 53, 1178 –1191. Cappella , E., & Weinstein, R. S. (2001). Turning around reading achieve-ment: Predictors of high schoolstudents ’academic resilience. Journal of Educational Psychology , 93, 758–771. Card , N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani, G. M., & Little, T. D. (2008). Direct and indirect aggression dur- ing childhood and adolescence:A meta-analytic review of genderdifferences, intercorrelations, and relations to maladjustment. Child Development ,79, 1185 –1229. Carlson , M., Marcus-Newhall, A., & Miller, N. (1990). Effects of situational aggression cues: A quantitative review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 58, 622–633. Carnagey , N. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2005). The effects of reward and punishment in violent video gameson aggressive affect, cognition, and behavior. Psychological Science , 16, 882–889. Carnelley , K. B., Pietromonaco, P. R., & Jaffe, K. (1994). Depression, work-ing models of others, and relation- ship functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 66, 127–140. Carvajal , S. C., Clair, S. D., Nash, S. G., & Evans, R. I. (1998). Relatingoptimism, hope, and self-esteem tosocial influences in deterring sub- stance use in adolescents. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,17, 443–465. Carver , C. S. (1997). Adult attachment and personality: Converging evi- dence and a new measure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,23, 865 –883. Carver , C. S. (2001). Affect and the functional bases of behavior: On thedimensional structure of affectiveexperience. Personality and Social Ps ychology Review ,5,3 4 5 –356.Carver , C. S., Lehman, J. M., & Antoni, M. H. (2003). Dispositional pessi- mism predicts illness-related dis- ruption of social and recreationalactivities among breast cancer pa-tients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,84, 813 –821. Carver , C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M. F., Robinson, D. S., Ketcham, A. S., Moffat, F. L., & Clark, K. C. (1993). How coping mediates theeffect of optimism on distress: Astudy of women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 65, 375–390. Carver , C. S., Smith, R. G., Antoni, M. H., Petronis, V. M., Weiss, S., & Derhagopian, R. P. (2005). Optimistic personality and psycho-logical well-being during treatment predict psychosocial well-being among long-term survivors ofbreast cancer. Health Psychology , 24, 508–516. Caspi , A. (1998). Personality develop- ment across the life course. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional and personality develop-ment (5th ed., pp. 311 –388). New York: Wiley. Caspi , A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continuities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 158 –172. Caspi , A., Harrington, H., Milne, B., Amell, J. W., Theodore, R. F., &Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Children ’s behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. Journal of Personality ,71, 495–513. Catalano , R., Dooley, D., Novaco, R. W., Wilson, G., & Hough, R. (1993). Using ECA survey data to examine the effect of job layoffs onviolent behavior. Hospital and Community Psychiatry ,44, 874–879. Cattell , H. E. P. (2004). The Sixteen Personality Factor (16PF) Questionnaire. In M. J. Hilsenroth & M. Hersen (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psy-chological assessment (Vol. 2) (pp. 39–49). New York: Wiley. Cattell , H. E. P., & Mead, A. D. (2008). The Sixteen Personality FactorQuestionnaire (16PF). In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, &D. H. Saklofske (Eds.). The Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment (Vol. 2) (pp. 135 –159). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cattell , R. B. (1974). Raymond B. Cattell. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiog- raphy (Vol. 6, pp. 61 –100). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Hall. Cepeda-Benito , A., & Short, P. (1998). Self-concealment, avoidance of psychological services, and per-ceived likelihood of seeking profes- sional help. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,45,5 8 –64. Chang , E. C. (1996). Cultural differ- ences in optimism, pessimism andcoping: Predictors of subsequent adjustment in Asian American and Caucasian American collegestudents. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,43, 113 –123. Chang , E. C. (1998). Dispositional op- timism and primary and secondary appraisal of a stressor: Controlling for confounding influences and re- lations to coping and psychologicaland physical adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1109 –1120. Chang , E. C. (2001). Cultural influences on optimism and pessimism:Differences in Western and Eastern construals of the self. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pes- si mism: Implications for theory, re- search, and practice (pp. 257 –280). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Charles , S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 80, 136–151. Cheek , J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 41, 330–339. Chelune , G. J., Sultan, F. E., & Williams, C. L. (1980). Loneliness,self-disclosure, and interpersonaleffectiveness. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,27, 462 –468. Cheng , C. (1999). Gender-role differ- ences in susceptibility to the influ- ence of support availability and depression. Journal of Personality , 67, 439–467. Cheng , C. (2001). Assessing coping flexibility in real-life and laboratorysettings: A multi-method approach.REFERENCES 463
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,80, 814 –833. Cheng , C. (2005). Processes underlying gender-role flexibility: Do andro- gynous individuals know more orknow how to cope? Journal of Personality ,73, 645 –673. Cheng , C. (2009). Dialectical thinking and coping flexibility: A multi- method approach. Journal of Personality ,77, 471 –493. Cheng , C., & Cheung, M. W. L. (2005). Cognitive processes underlyingcoping flexibility: Differentiation and integration. Journal of Personality ,73, 859 –886. Chess , S., & Thomas, A. (1986). Temperament in clinical practice . New York: Guilford. Chess , S., & Thomas, A. (1991). Temperament and the concept of goodness of fit. In J. Strelau & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations in temperament (pp. 15 –28). London: Plenum. Chess , S., & Thomas, A. (1996). Temperament: Theory and practice . New York: Brunner/Mazel. Chipperfield , J. G., & Perry, R. P. (2006). Primary- and secondary-control strategies in later life: Predicting hospital outcomes inmen and women. Health Psychology ,25, 226 –236. Chitsabesan , P., Corbett, S., Walker, L., Spencer, J., & Barton, J. R. (2006).Describing clinical teachers ’charac- teristics and behaviours using critical incidents and repertory grids. Medical Education ,40,6 4 5 –653. Christensen , A., Atkins, D. C., Yi, J., Baucom, D. H., & George, W. H.(2009). Couple and individual adjustment for 2 years following a randomized clinical trial comparingtraditional versus integrative be- havioral couple therapy. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology ,74, 1180 –1191. Christensen , P. N., & Kashy, D. A. (1998). Perceptions of and by lonely people in initial social inter-action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,24, 322 –329. Church, A. T. (Ed.). (2001). Culture and personality [Special issue]. Journal of Personality ,69(6). Church, A. T., & Burke, P. J. (1994). Exploratory and confirmatory testsof the Big Five and Tellegen ’s three- and four-dimensional models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,66,9 3 –114.Churchill , S. L. (2003). Goodness-of-fit in early childhood settings. Early Childhood Education Journal ,31, 113–118. Clancy , S. M., & Dollinger, S. J. (1993). Photographic depictions of theself: Gender and age differences in social connectedness. Sex Roles ,29, 477–495. Clark , D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy for depression . New York: Wiley. Clark , D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variations in clinicaldepression and accessibility ofmemories of positive and negativeexperiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,91,8 7 –95. Clark , L. A. (2005). Temperament as a unifying basis for personality and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,114, 505–521. Clark , L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self- reported mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 296–308. Clark , L. A., & Watson, D. (1999). Temperament: A new paradigm fortrait psychology. In L. A. Pervin &O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 399 –423). New York: Guilford. Clearfield , M. W., & Nelson, N. M. (2006). Sex differences in mothers ’ speech and play behavior with 6-9-and 14-month infants. Sex Roles , 54, 127–137. Coe, W. C. (1989). Posthypnotic amne- sia: Theory and research. InN. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral perspective (pp. 110 –148). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus. Coe, W. C., & Sarbin, T. R. (1991). Role theory: Hypnosis from adramaturgical and narrational per- spective. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue(Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp. 303 –323). New York: Guilford. Cohen , D. B. (1979). Sleep and dream- ing: Origins, nature and function . New York: Pergamon. Cohen , S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P., Fireman, P., Gwaltney, J. M., Jr., &Newsom, J. T. (1995). State and trait affect as predictors of objectiveand subjective symptoms of respi- ratory viral infections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 68, 159–169. Cohen , S., Doyle, W., Turner, R., Alper, C. M., & Skoner, D. P. (2003). Sociability and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychological Science ,14, 389 –395. Cohen , S., & Janicki-Deverts, D. (2009). Can we improve our physical healthby altering our social networks? Perspectives on PsychologicalScience ,4, 375 –378. Cohen , S., & Pressman, S. D. (2006). Positive affect and health. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 15, 122–125. Cohen , S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the bufferinghypothesis. Psychological Bulletin , 98, 310–357. Cohn, N. B., & Strassberg, D. S. (1983). Self-disclosure reciprocity amongpreadolescents. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,9, 97–102. Coleman , M., & Ganong, L. H. (1985). Love and sex-role stereotypes: Do“macho” men and “feminine ” women make better lovers? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,49, 170 –176. Collins , N. L., Cooper, M. L., Albino, A., & Allard, L. (2002).Psychosocial vulnerability fromadolescence to adulthood: A pro-spective study of attachment style differences in relationship func- tioning and partner choice. Journal of Personality ,70, 965 –1008. Collins , N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000). A safe haven: An attachment theoryperspective on support seeking andcaregiving in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 1053 –1073. Collins , N. L., Ford, M. B., Guichard, A. C., & Allard, L. M. (2006). Working models of attachment and attribution process in intimate relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 201–219. Collins , N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin ,116, 457 –475. Collins , N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models,and relationship quality in datingcouples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 644 –663.464 REFERENCES
Compas , B. E., Worsham, N. L., Ey, S., & Howell, D. C. (1996). When Mom or Dad has cancer: II. Coping, cognitive appraisals, andpsychological distress in children ofcancer patients. Health Psychology , 15, 167–175. Conley , J. J. (1984). Longitudinal consistency of adult personality:Self-reported psychological charac- teristics across 45 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 47, 1325 –1333. Conley , J. J. (1985). Longitudinal stability of personality traits: A multitrait-multimethod-multi- occasion analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 49, 1266 –1282. Constantinople , A. (1973). Masculinity- femininity: An exception to afamous dictum. Psychological Bulletin ,80, 389 –407. Conti , D. (2005, February 1). Convicted murderer will get new trial. Pittsburgh Tribune Review . Conti , R. (2001). Time flies: Investigating the connection be-tween intrinsic motivation and theexperience of time. Journal of Personality ,69,1–26. Conway , F., Magai, C., Springer, C., & Jones, S. C. (2008). Optimism and pessimism as predictors of physical and psychological health among grandmothers raising their grand-children. Journal of Research in Personality ,42, 1352 –1357. Conway , M. A., & Holmes, A. (2004). Psychosocial stages and the acces-sibility of autobiographical memo-ries across the life cycle. Journal of Personality ,72, 461 –480. Cooper , H. M., & Good, T. E. (1983). Pygmalion grows up: Studies in the expectation communication pro- cess . New York: Longman. Cooper , M. L., Wood, P. K., Orcutt, H. K., & Albino, A. (2003).Personality and the predisposition to engage in risky or problem be- haviors during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,84, 390 –410. Cooper , T., Detre, T., & Weiss, S. M. (1981). Coronary-prone behaviorand coronary heart disease: Acritical review. Circulation ,63, 1199 –1215. Coopman , S. J. (1997). Personal constructs and communication ininterpersonal and organizational contexts. In G. J. Neimeyer &R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.), Advances in personal construct psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 101 –147). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Coplan , R. J., Barber, A. M., & Lagace- Seguin, D. G. (1999). The role ofchild temperament as a predictor of early literacy and numeracy skills inpreschoolers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly ,14, 537 –553. Coplan , R. J., Bowker, A., & Cooper, S. M. (2003). Parenting daily has-sles, child temperament, and socialadjustment in preschool. Early Childhood Research Quarterly ,18, 376–395. Cordova , J. V., Gee, C. B., & Warren, L. Z. (2005). Emotional skillfulness in marriage: Intimacy as a mediator of the relationship between emo-tional skillfulness and marital sat-isfaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,24, 218 –235. Cornell , D. G. (1997). Post hoc expla- nation is not prediction. American Psychologist ,52, 1380. Cornette , M. M., Strauman, T. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Busch, A. M.(2008). Self-discrepancy and sui-cidal ideation. Cognition and Emotion ,23, 504 –527. Costa , P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual for the NEO PI-R . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa , P. T., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and sur- prising findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 81, 322–331. Costello , C. G. (1978). A critical review of Seligman ’s laboratory experi- ments on learned helplessness anddepression in humans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,87,2 1 –31. Cowen , E. I., Wyman, P. A., & Work, W. C. (1992). The relationship be- tween retrospective reports of early child temperament and adjustment at ages 10 –12. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology ,20,3 9 –50. Cox-Fuenzalida , L., Angie, A., Holloway, S., & Sohl, L. (2006).Extraversion and task performance:A fresh look through the workloadhistory lens. Journal of Research in Personality ,40, 432 –439. Cox-Fuenzalida , L., Gilliland, K., & Swickert, R. J. (2001). Congruency of the relationship between extra- version and the brainstem auditoryevoked response based on the EPI versus the EPQ. Journal of Research in Personality ,35, 117 –126. Crabbe , J. C. (2002). Genetic contribu- tions to addiction. Annual Review of Psychology ,53, 435 –462. Craik , K. H. (1986). Personality re- search methods: An historical perspective. Journal of Personality , 54,1 8–51. Cramer , K. M., & Lake, R. P. (1998). The Preference for Solitude Scale:Psychometric properties and factorstructure. Personality and Individual Di fferences ,24,1 9 3 –199. Cramer , K. M., Ofosu, H. B., & Barry, J. E. (2000). An abbreviated form of the social and emotional loneli- ness scale for adults (SELSA). Personality and Individual Differences ,28, 1125 –1131. Cramer , P. (1991). The development of defense mechanisms: Theory, re- search, and assessment . New York: Springer-Verlag. Cramer , P. (1997). Evidence for change in children ’s use of defense me- chanisms. Journal of Personality , 65, 233–247. Cramer , P. (1998a). Freshman to senior year: A follow-up study of identity, narcissism, and defense mechan- isms. Journal of Research in Personality ,32, 156 –172. Cramer , P. (1998b). Threat to gender representation: Identity and identi-fication. Journal of Personality ,66, 335–357. Cramer , P. (1999). Personality, person- ality disorders, and defense me-chanisms. Journal of Personality , 67, 535–554. Cramer , P. (2000). Defense mechanisms in psychology today: Further pro-cesses for adaptation. American Psychologist ,55, 637 –646. Cramer , P. (2002). Defense mechanisms, behavior, and affect in youngadulthood. Journal of Personality , 70, 103–126. Cramer , P. (2006). Protecting the self: Defense mechanisms in action . New York: Guilford. Cramer , P. (2007). Longitudinal study of defense mechanisms: Late child- hood to late adolescence. Journal of Personality ,75,1–23. Cramer , P., & Blatt, S. J. (1990). Use of the TAT to measure change in defense mechanisms followingintensive psychotherapy. Journal of Personality Assessment ,54, 236–251.REFERENCES 465
Cramer , P., Blatt, S. J., & Ford, R. Q. (1988). Defense mechanisms in the anaclitic and introjective personal- ity configuration. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,56, 610 –616. Cramer , P., & Block, J. (1998). Preschool antecedents of defensemechanism use in young adults: Alongitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 159–169. Cramer , P., & Brilliant, M. A. (2001). Defense use and defense under- standing in children. Journal of Personality ,69, 297 –322. Cramer , R. E., Manning-Ryan, B., Johnson, L. M., & Barbo, E.(2000). Sex differences in subjective distress to violations of trust: Extending an evolutionary per-spective. Basic and Applied Social Psychology ,22, 101 –109. Crane , F. G., & Crane, E. C. (2007). Dispositional optimism and en- trepreneurial success. Psychologist- Manager Journal ,10,1 3 –25. Crick , F., & Mitchison, G. (1983). The function of dream sleep. Nature , 304, 111–114. Crick , N. R., & Dodge, K, A. (1996). Social information-processing me- chanisms in reactive and proactiveaggression. Child Development ,67, 993–1002. Crocker , J., Brook, A. T., Niiya, Y., & Villacorta, M. (2006). The pursuitof self-esteem: Contingencies of self-wroth and self-regulation. Journal of Personality ,74, 1749 –177. Crocker , J., Karpinski, A., Quinn, D. M., & Chase, S. K. (2003). When grades determine self-worth: Consequences of contingent self-worth for male and female engi- neering and psychology majors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,85, 507 –516. Crocker , J., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2003). Level of self-esteem and contingen- cies of self-worth: Unique effects onacademic, social, and financialproblems in college students. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,29, 701 –712. Crocker , J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M. L., & Bouvrette, A. (2003). Contingencies of self-worth in college students: Theory and mea- surement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,85, 894–908.Crocker , J., & Park, L. E. (2003). Seeking self-esteem: Construction, maintenance, and protection of self-worth. In M. R. Leary &J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 291 –313). New York: Guilford. Crocker , J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological Bulletin ,130, 392–414. Crocker , J., Sommers, S. R., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2002). Hopes dashed and dreams fulfilled: Contingencies of self-worth and graduate school admissions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,28, 1275 –1286. Crocker , J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological Review ,108, 593–623. Cross , S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational- interdependent self-construaland relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 78, 791–808. Cross , S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin , 122,5–37. Cross , S. E., Morris, M. L., & Gore, J. S. (2002). Thinking about oneselfand others: The relational- interdependent self-construal andsocial cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 82, 399–418. Crossen , C. (1996, March 8). Solitude is a casualty of the war with time. Wall Street Journal , p. R4. Crost , N. W., Pauls, C. A., & Wacker, J. (2008). Defensiveness and anxietypredict frontal EEG asymmetryonly in specific situational contexts. Biological Psychology ,78,4 3 –52. Crowson , J. J., & Cromwell, R. L. (1995). Depressed and normal in- dividuals differ both in selection and in perceived tonal quality of positive-negative messages. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,104, 305–311. Csikszentmihalyi , M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience . New York: Harper & Row. Csikszentmihalyi , M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren ’t we happy? American Psychologist ,54, 821–827. Csikszentmihalyi , M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988).Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness . New York: Cambridge. Csikszentmihalyi ,M . ,&L e F e v r e ,J . (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality an d Social Psychology ,56,8 1 5 –822. Cunningham, J. A., Strassberg, D. S., & Haan, B. (1986). Effects of intimacy and sex-role congruency of self- disclosure. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,4, 393 –401. Cunningham, M. R. (1988). Does hap- piness mean friendliness? Inducedmood and heterosexual self- disclosure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,14, 283 –297. Curran , J. P., Wallander, J. L., & Fischetti, M. (1980). The impor- tance of behavioral and cognitivefactors in heterosexual-social anxi-ety. Journal of Personality ,48, 285–292. Cury , F., Elliot, A. J., Da Fonseca, D., & Moller, A. C. (2006). The social-cognitive model of achievement motivation and the 2 X 2 achieve- ment goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 90, 666–679. Cutrona , C. E. (1982). Transition to college: Loneliness and the processof social adjustment. In L. A.Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness (pp. 291 –309). New York: Wiley. Cyranowski , J. M., & Andersen, B. L. (2000). Evidence of self-schematic cognitive processing of women with differing sexual self-views. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology , 19, 519–543. Dadds , M. R., Bovbjerg, D. H., Redd, W. H., & Cutmore, T. R. H.(1997). Imagery in human classicalconditioning. Psychological Bulletin ,122,8 9 –103. Danoff-Burg , S., Revenson, T. A., Trudeau, K. J., & Paget, S. A. (2004). Unmitigated communion, social constraints, and psychologi- cal distress among women withrheumatoid arthritis. Journal of Personality ,72,2 9 –46. Darnon , C., Harackiewicz, J. M., Butera, F., Mugny, G., &Quiamzade, A. (2007).Performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals: When uncertainty makes a differ-ence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,33, 813–827.466 REFERENCES
Davidson , K., & MacGregor, M. W. (1996). Reliability of an idiographic Q-Sort measure of defense me- chanisms. Journal of Personality Assessment ,66, 624 –639. Davidson , K., & MacGregor, M. W. (1998). A critical appraisal ofself-report defense mechanismmeasures. Journal of Personality , 66, 965–992. Davidson , K., MacGregor, M. W., Johnson, E. A., Woody, E. Z., &Chaplin, W. F. (2004). The relationbetween defense use and adaptive behavior. Journal of Research in Personality ,38, 105 –129. Davidson , K. W., Gidron, Y., Mostofsky, E., & Trudeau, K. J.(2007). Hospitalization cost offset of a hostility intervention for coro- nary heart disease patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,75, 657 –662. Davidson , R. J. (1991). Biological ap- proaches to the study of personality. In V. J. Derlega, B. A. Winstead, & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Personality: Co ntemporary theory and research (pp. 87 –11 2). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Davidson , R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymme-try: Emotional expression and brain physiology I. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 330–341. Davidson , R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1982). Asymmetrical brain activity discri- minates between positive versus negative affective stimuli in humaninfants. Science ,218, 1235 –1237. Davidson , R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1989). Frontal brain asymmetry predictsinfants ’response to maternal separation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,98, 127 –131. Davidson , R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1989). Laterality and emotion: An electrophysiological approach. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 3, pp. 419 –441). New York: Elsevier Science. Davila , J., Burge, D., & Hammen, C. (1997). Why does attachment style change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,73, 826 –838. Davila , J., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Attachment changeprocesses in the early years of mar-riage. Journal of Personality and So cial Psychology ,76, 783 –802.Davis , J. D. (1976). Self-disclosure in an acquaintance exercise: Responsibility for level of intimacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,33, 787 –792. Davis , J. D. (1977). Effects of commu- nication about interpersonal process on the evolution of self-disclosure in dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 35,3 1–37. Davis , P. J. (1999). Gender differences in autobiographical memory forchildhood emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,76, 498 –510. Dawes , R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychotherapy built on myth . New York: Free Press. Deckers , L., & Carr, D. E. (1986). Cartoons varying in low-level pain ratings, not aggression ratings, correlate positively with funninessratings. Motivation and Emotion , 10, 207–216. Deffenbacher , J., Huff, M., Lynch, R., Oetting, E., & Salvatore, N. (2000).Characteristics and treatments ofhigh-anger drivers. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,47,5–17. del Valle , C. H. C., & Mateos, P. M. (2008). Dispositional pessimism, defensive pessimism and optimism: The effect of induced mood on prefactual and counterfactualthinking and performance. Cognition and Emotion ,22, 1600 –1612. Dembroski , T. M., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Coronary-prone behavior:Components of the Type A pattern and hostility. Journal of Personality ,55, 211 –235. DeNeve , K. M. (1999). Happy as an extraverted clam? The role of per-sonality for subjective well-being. Current Directions in Psychological Science ,8, 141 –144. DeNeve , K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits andsubjective well-being. Psychological Bu lletin ,12 4 ,1 9 7 –229. Denson , T. F., Aviles, F. E., Pollock, V. E., Earleywine, M., Vasquez,E. A., & Miller, N. (2008). Theeffects of alcohol and the salience of aggressive cues on triggered dis- placed aggression. Aggressive Behavior ,34,2 5 –33. DePaulo , B. M., Dull, W. R., Greenberg, J. M., & Swaim, G. W. (1989). Areshy people reluctant to ask for help? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,56, 834 –844. DePaulo , B. M., Epstein, J. A., & LeMay, C. S. (1990). Responses of the socially anxious to the prospect of interpersonal evaluation. Journal of Personality ,58, 623 –640. DePaulo , B. M., Kenny, D. A., Hoover, C. W., Webb, W., & Oliver, P. V.(1987). Accuracy of person percep- tion: Do people know what kinds of impressions they convey? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 303 –315. Depue , R. A., & Monroe, S. M. (1978). Learned helplessness in the per- spective of the depressive disorders: Conceptual and definitional issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 87,3–20. de Rivera , J. (1997). The construction of false memory syndrome: The expe- rience of retractors. Psychological Inquiry ,8, 271 –292. Derlega , V. J., & Chaikin, A. L. (1976). Norms affecting self-disclosure in men and women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,44, 376 –380. Derlega , V. J., Wilson, M., & Chaikin, A. L. (1976). Friendship anddisclosure reciprocity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 34, 578–582. Derlega , V. J., Winstead, B. A., & Greene, K. (2008). Self-disclosureand starting a close relationship. In S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel & J. Harvey (Eds.), Handbook of relationship initiation (pp. 153 –174). New York: Psychology Press. Derry , P. A., & Kuiper, N. A. (1981). Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 90, 286–297. De Schipper, J. C., Tavecchio, L. W. C., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & VanZeijl, J. (2004). Goodness-of-fit in center day care: Relations of tem- perament, stability, and quality ofcare with the child ’s adjustment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly ,19, 257 –272. DeSteno , D., Bartlett, M. Y., Braverman, J., & Salovey, P. (2002). Sex dif- ferences in jealousy: Evolutionary mechanism or artifact of measure- ment? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,83, 1103 –1116. DeVellis , R. F., DeVellis, B. M., & McCauley, C. (1978). VicariousREFERENCES 467
acquisition of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,36, 894 –899. Dickens , W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2006). Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardi- zation samples. Psychological Science ,17, 913 –920. Diehl , M., Elnick, A. B., Bourbeau, L. S., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (1998). Adultattachment styles: Their relations to family context and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,74, 1656 –1669. Diener , E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin ,95, 542–575. Diener , E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross- cultural correlates of life satisfac- tion and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 68, 653–663. Diener , E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 47, 1105 –1117. Diener , E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science ,13,8 1 –84. Digman , J. M. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability,and utility. Journal of Personality , 57, 195–214. Digman , J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology ,41, 417 –440. Digman , J. M., & Inouye, J. (1986). Further specification of the fiverobust factors of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,50, 116 –123. DiLalla , D. L., Carey, G., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (1996). Heritability of MMPI personality indicators of psychopathology in twins reared apart. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,105, 491–499. Dindia , K., & Allen, M. (1992). Sex differences in self-disclosure: Ameta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin ,112, 106 –124. Di Paula , A., & Campbell, J. D. (2002). Self-esteem and persistence in theface of failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 83, 711–724. Dodge , K. A. (2006). Translational sci- ence in action: Hostile attributionalstyle and the development of aggressive behavior problems.Development and Psychopathol- ogy ,18, 791 –814. Dodge , K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Pettit, G. S., Price, J. M., Fontaine, R., et al. (2003). Peerrejection and social information- processing factors in the develop- ment of aggressive behaviorproblems in children. Child Development ,74, 374 –393. Dodgson , P. G., & Wood, J. V. (1998). Self-esteem and the cognitive ac-cessibility of strengths and weak-nesses after failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 75, 178–197. Dollard , J., Doob, L., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and aggression . New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Dollinger , S. J. (2000). Locus of control and incidental learning: An appli- cation to college student success. College Student Journal ,34, 537–540. Domhoff , G. W. (1996). Finding meaning in dreams: A quantitative approach . New York: Plenum. Domhoff , G. W. (1999). New directions in the study of dream content usingthe Hall and Van de Castle codingsystem. Dreaming ,9, 115 –137. Domhoff , G. W. (2001). A new neuro- cognitive theory of dreams. Dreaming ,11,1 3 –33. Domhoff , G. W. (2004). Why did em- pirical dream researchers reject Freud? A critique of historical claims by Mark Solms. Dreaming , 14,3–17. Dompnier , B., Darnon, C., & Butera, F. (2009). Faking the desire to learn:A clarification of the link between mastery goals and academicachievement. Psychological Science , 20, 939–943. Donnellan , M. B., Conger, R. D., & Bryant, C. M. (2004). The Big Fiveand enduring marriages. Journal of Research in Personality ,38, 481–504. Donnelly , D. A., & Murray, E. J. (1991). Cognitive and emotional changes in written essays and ther- apy interviews. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,10, 334–350. Doob , L. W., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Factors determining substitute be-havior and the overt expression ofaggression. Journal of Abnormal and So cial Psychology ,34,2 9 3 –313.Doucet , C., & Stelmack, R. M. (2000). An event-related potential analysis of extraversion and individual dif- ferences in cognitive processingspeed and response execution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 956 –964. Dozois , D. J. A., & Dobson, K. S. (2001). A longitudinal investigation of information processing and cognitive organization in clinical depression: Stability of schematicinterconnectedness. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,69, 914 –925. Draguns , J. G. (2008). What have we learned about the interplay of cul- ture with counseling and psycho- therapy? In U. P. Gielen, J. G. Draguns, & J. M. Fish (Eds.), Principles of multicultural counsel-ing and therapy (pp. 393 –417). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis. Duke , T., & Davidson, J. (2002). Ordinary and recurrent dream recall of active, past and non- recurrent dreamers during and afteracademic stress. Dreaming ,12, 185–197. Dunbar , E. (2006, April 1). Bizarre death four years after trial leads totossed conviction. Associated Press State and Local Wire . Retrieved November 27, 2006, from http:// www.lexis-nexis.com Durante , K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes in women ’s choice of dress across the ovulatorycycle: Naturalistic and laboratorytask-based evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,34, 1451 –1460. Dutton , K. A., & Brown, J. D. (1997). Global self-esteem and specific self-views as determinants of people ’s reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,73, 139 –148. Dwivedi , Y., Mondal, A. C., Payappagoudar, G. V., & Rizavi,H. S. (2005). Differential regulationof serotonin (5HT)-sub(2A) recep-tor mRNA and protein levels after single and repeated stress in rat brain: Role in learned helplessnessbehavior. Neuropharmacology ,48, 204–214. Eagly, A. H. (1997). Sex differences in social behavior: Comparing socialrole theory and evolutionary psy-chology. American Psychologist , 52, 1380 –1382.468 REFERENCES
Eastwick , P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Sex differences in mate preferences revisted: Do people know what they initially desire in a romanticpartner? Journal of Personality an d Social Psychology ,94, 24 5–264. Eaves , L., & Eysenck, H. (1975). Utilization of self-schemas as amechanism of interpretational bias in children. Social Cognition ,5, 280–300. Eccles , J. (1985). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In T.B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 32 , pp. 97 –132). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Eccles , J. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. InA. J. Elliott & C. W. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 105 –121). New York: Guilford Press. Eisenberger , R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth? American Psychologist ,51, 1153 –1166. Elliot , A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 X 2 achievement goal frame- work. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,80, 501 –519. Elliot , A. J., Shell, M. M., Bouas, H. K., & Maier, M. A. (2005). Achievement goals, performancecontingencies, and performanceattainment: An experimental test. Journal of Educational Psychology , 97, 630–640. Elliott , A. J., Miltenberger, R. G., Kaster-Bundgaard, J., & Lumley, V. (1996). A national survey of assessment and therapy techniquesused by behavior therapists. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice , 3, 107–125. Elliott , R. (2002). The effectiveness of humanistic therapies: A meta-analysis. In D. J. Cain & J. Seeman (Eds.), Humanistic psychotherapies: Handbook of research and practice (pp. 57 –81). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Ellis, A. E. (1971). Growth through reason: Verbatim cases in rational- emotive therapy . North Hollywood, CA: Wilshire. Ellis, A. E. (1987). The impossibility of achieving consistently good mental health. American Psychologist ,42, 364–375.Ellis, A. E. (2003). Early theories and practices of rational emotive be- havior therapy and how they have been augmented and revised duringthe last three decades. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive- Behavior Therapy ,21, 219 –243. Else-Quest , N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin ,132,3 3 –72. Emmons , R. A., & Diener, E. (1986). Influence of impulsivity and socia-bility on subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,50, 1211 –1215. Endler , N. S., & Hunt, J. M. (1966). Sources of behavioral variance asmeasured by the S-R inventory ofanxiousness. Psychological Bulletin ,65, 336 –346. Endler , N. S., & Hunt, J. M. (1968). S-R inventories of hostility and com-parisons of the proportions of var-iance from persons, responses, and situations for hostility and anx- iousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,9, 309 –315. Epstein , S. (1979). The stability of be- havior: I. On predicting most of thepeople much of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 37, 1097 –1126. Epstein , S. (1980). The stability of behavior: II. Implications for psy-chological research. American Psychologist ,35, 790 –806. Epstein , S. (1983). Aggregation and beyond: Some basic issues on theprediction of behavior. Journal of Personality ,51, 360 –392. Erickson , M. H. (1967). Advanced techniques of hypnosis and therapy: Selected papers of Milton H. Erickson . New York: Grune & Stratton. Erikson , E. H. (1950/1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: Norton. Erikson , E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis . New York: Norton. Erikson , E. H. (1975). Life history and the historical moment . New York: Norton. Ernst , J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1999). Lonely hearts: Psychological per- spectives on loneliness. Applied Preventive Psychology ,8,1–22. Eron , L. D. (1987). The development of aggressive behavior from the per- spective of a developing behaviorism. American Psychologist ,42,4 3 5 –442.Etaugh , C., & Liss, M. B. (1992). Home, school, and playroom: Training grounds for adult gender roles. Sex Roles ,26, 129 –147. Evans , D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult tem- perament. Journal of Research in Personality ,41, 868 –888. Evans , J., Heron, J., Lewis, G., Araya, R., & Wolke, D. (2005). Negativeself-schemas and the onset of de- pression in women: Longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychiatry , 186, 302–307. Evans , K. K., & Singer, J. A. (1995). Studying intimacy through dreamnarratives: The relationship ofdreams to self-report and projective measures of personality. Imagination, Cognition and Personality ,14, 211 –226. Evans , R. I. (1976). The making of psychology . New York: Knopf. Evans , W. P., Owens, P., & Marsh, S. C. (2005). Environmental factors, locus of control, and adolescent suicide risk. Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal ,22, 301 –319. Eysenck , H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality . Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Eysenck , H. J. (1982). Development of a theory. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Personality, genetics and behavior:Selected papers (pp. 1– 38). New York: Praeger. Eysenck , H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. InL. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 244 –27 6). New York: Guilford. Eysenck , H. J. (1997). Personality and experimental psychology: The uni- fication of psychology and the pos-sibility of a paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 73, 1224 –1237. Eysenck , H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory . San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Eysenck , S. B. G., & Long, F. Y. (1986). A cross-cultural comparison of personality in adults and children: Singapore and England. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 50, 124–130. Factbox : China ’s dreaded “gaokao, ”the world ’s largest exam (2008, June 6). Reuters . Retrieved from http:// www.reuters.com/articlePrint? articleId=USPEK26587420080606REFERENCES 469
Fancher , R. E. (2000). Snapshots of Freud in America, 1899 –1999. American Psychologist ,55, 1025 –1028. Farabee , D. J., Holcom, M. L., Ramsey, S. L., & Cole, S. G. (1993). Social anxiety and speaker gaze in a persuasive atmosphere. Journal of Research in Personality ,27, 365–376. Farber , B. A. (2006). Self-disclosure in psychotherapy . New York: Guilford. Farber , B. A., & Sohn, A. E. (2007). Patterns of self-disclosure in psychotherapy and marriage. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training ,44, 226 –231. Farley , F. (2000). Hans J. Eysenck (1916 –1997). American Psychologist ,55, 674 –675. Fassler , O., Lynn, S. J., & Knox, J. (2008). Is hypnotic suggestibility astable trait? Consciousness and Cognition ,17, 240 –253. Feeney , B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001). Predictors of caregiving in adultintimate relationships: An attach-ment theoretical perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,80, 972 –994. Feeney , B. C., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1996). Effects of adult attachment and presence of romantic partners on physiological responses to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,70, 255 –270. Feeney , J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor ofadult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,58, 281 –291. Feeney , J. A., Noller, P., & Patty, J. (1993). Adolescents ’interactions with the opposite sex: Influence ofattachment style and gender. Journal of Adolescence ,16, 169–186. Feingold , A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of physical attractiveness on romantic attraction: A compari- son across five research paradigms. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,59, 981 –993. Feingold , A. (1992). Gender differences in mate selection preferences: A testof the parental investment model. Psychological Bulletin ,112, 125–139. Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artisticcreativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,2, 290 –309.Feixas , G., Erazo-Caicedo, M. I., Harter, S. L., & Bach, L. (2008). Construction of self and others in unipolar depressive disorders: Astudy using repertory grid tech-nique. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,32, 386 –400. Feldman Barrett , L., Lane, R. D., Sechrest, L., & Schwartz, G. E. (2000). Sex differences in emotional awareness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,26, 1027 – 1035. Fenichel , O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis . New York: Norton. Ferguson , S. M., Brodkin, J. D., Lloyd, G. K., & Menzaghi, F. (2000).Antidepressant-like effects of the subtype selective nicotinic acetylcho-line receptor agonist, SIB-1508Y, inthe learned helplessness rat model of depression. Psychopharmacology , 15 2,2 9 5 –303. Findley , M. J., & Cooper, H. M. (1983). Locus of control and academic achievement: A literature review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 419 –427. Finkel , D., & McGue, M. (1997). Sex differences and nonadditivity inheritability of the multidimensionalpersonality questionnaire scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,72, 929 –938. Finkelstein , L. E., & Levy, B. R. (2006). Disclosure of Holocaust experi- ences: Reasons, attributions and health implications. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,25, 117–140. Fischer , A. R., Jome, L. M., & Atkinson, D. R. (1998). Reconceptualizing multicultural counseling: Universal healing conditions in a culturallyspecific context. Counseling Psychologist ,26, 525 –588. Fischer , P., & Greitemeyer, T. (2006). Music and aggression: The impact of sexual-aggressive song lyrics on aggression-related thoughts, emo- tions, and behavior toward thesame and the opposite sex. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 1165 –1176. Fischer , R., & Chalmers, A. (2008). Is optimism universal? A meta- analytical investigation of optimism across 22 nations. Personality and In dividual Differences ,45,3 7 8 –382. Fisher , M. (1995, December 6). Freudian slip. San Jose Mercury News , p. 25A.Fletcher , G. J. O., Tither, J. M., O’Loughlin, C., Friesen, M., & Overall, N. (2004). Warm and homely or cold and beautiful? Sexdifferences in trading off traits inmate selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,30, 659–672. Floderus-Myrhed , B., Pedersen, N., & Rasmuson, I. (1980). Assessment of heritability for personality, based on a short-form of the EysenckPersonality Inventory: A study of12,898 twin pairs. Behavior Genetics ,10, 153 –162. Flynn , J. R. (1999). Searching for justice: The discovery of IQ gains over time. American Psychologist ,54, 5–20. Fodor , E. M., & Carver, R. A. (2000). Achievement and power motives, performance feedback, and creativ- ity. Journal of Research in Personality ,34, 380 –396. Folkman , S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior ,21, 219–239. Fontaine , R. G., & Dodge, K. A. (2006). Real-time decision making and ag- gressive behavior in youth: A heu- ristic model of response evaluationand decision (RED). Aggressive Behavior ,32, 604 –624. Foulkes , D., & Cavallero, C. (1993). Dreaming as cognition . New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1986). Taste-elicited changes in facial signsof emotion and the asymmetry ofbrain electrical activity in human newborns. Neuropsychologia ,24, 417–422. Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1987). Electroencephalogram asymmetryin response to the approach of a stranger and maternal separation of10-month-old infants. Develop- mental Psychology ,23, 233 –240. Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Patterns of electrical activity duringfacial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology ,24, 230 –236. Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinu- ity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiologicaland behavioral influences across thefirst four years of life. Child Development ,72,1–21.470 REFERENCES
Fraley , R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Airport separation: A naturalistic study of adult attachment dynamics in separating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 75, 1198 –1212. Frankel , A., & Prentice-Dunn, S. (1990). Loneliness and the processing ofself-relevant information. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology , 9, 303–315. Fransella , F. (2003). International handbook of personal construct psychology . London: Wiley. Fransella , F. (2005). The essential prac- titioner ’s handbook of personal construct psychology . London: Wiley. Fransella , F., Bell, R., & Bannister, D. (2003). A manual for repertory grid technique (2nd ed.). London: Wiley. Frattaroli , J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators:A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin ,132, 823 –865. Frazier , L. D., Hooker, K., Johnson, P. M., & Kaus, C. R. (2000).Continuity and change in possibleselves in later life: A 5-year longi- tudinal study. Basic and Applied Social Psychology ,22, 237 –243. Fredrickson , B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory:Toward understanding women ’s lived experience and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly ,21, 173 –206. Frensch , K. M., Pratt, M. W., & Norris, J. E. (2007). Foundations of gener-ativity: Personal and family corre-lates of emerging adults ’generative life-story themes. Journal of Research in Personality ,41,4 5 –62. Fresco , D. M., Alloy, L. B., & Reilly- Harrington, N. (2006). Association of attributional style for negative and positive events and the occur- rence of life events with depressionand anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,25, 1140 –1159. Freud , A. (1965). Normality and pathology in childhood . New York: International Universities Press. Freud , S. (1886 –1936/1964). The com- plete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vols. 1 –24). London: Hogarth. Friedman , H. S., & Miller-Herringer, T. (1991). Nonverbal display of emo-tion in public and private: Self- monitoring, personality, andexpressive cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 61, 766–775. Friedman , H. S., Prince, L. M., Riggio, R. E., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1980). Understanding and assessing non- verbal expressiveness: The Affective Communication Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 39, 333–351. Friedman , L. C., Nelson, D. V., Baer, P. E., Lane, M., Smith, F. E., &Dworkin, R. J. (1992). The rela-tionship of dispositional optimism, daily life stress, and domestic envi- ronment to coping methods used bycancer patients. Journal of Behavioral Medicine ,15, 127 –141. Friedman , M., & Rosenman, R. (1974). Type A behavior and your heart . New York: Knopf. Friedrich-Cofer , L., & Huston, A. C. (1986). Television violence and aggression: The debate continues. Psychological Bulletin ,100, 364–371. Frisina , P. G., Borod, J. C., & Lepore, S. J. (2004). A meta-analysis of the effects of written emotional disclo-sure on the health outcomes in clinical populations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease ,192, 629–634. Fritz, H. L., & Helgeson, V. S. (1998). Distinctions of unmitigated com- munion from communion: Self- neglect and overinvolvement withothers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,75, 121 –140. Froming , W. J., Nasby, W., & McManus, J. (1998). Pro-social self-schemas, self-awareness, and children ’s prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,75, 766 –777. Fromm , E. (1950). Psychoanalysis and religion . New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Fromm , E. (1966). You shall be as gods . Greenwich, CT: Fawcett. Fromm , E., & Nash, M. R. (1997). Psychoanalysis and hypnosis . Madison, CT: International Universities Press. Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in nega-tive affect and well-being: The casefor emotional intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 61, 427–434. Funder , D. C. (2009). Persons, beha- viors and situations: An agenda for personality psychology in thepostwar era. Journal of Research in Personality ,43, 120 –126. Funder , D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (1983). Behavior as a function of the situa- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 107 –112. Gabbard , G. O., Gunderson, J. G., & Fonagy, P. (2002). The place ofpsychoanalytic treatments withinpsychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry ,59, 505 –510. Gabriel , S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there “his”and“her”types of interdependence? The implications of gender differences in collective versus relational interdependencefor affect, behavior, and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,77, 642 –655. Gacono , C. B., & Evans, F. B. (Eds.) (2008). The handbook of forensic Rorschach assessment . New York: Routledge. Gaeddert , W. P. (1985). Sex and sex role effects on achievement strivings: Dimensions of similarity and dif- ference. Journal of Personality ,53, 286–305. Gale, C. R., Batty, G. D., & Deary, I. J. (2008). Locus of control at age10 years and health outcomes and behaviors at age 30 years: The 1970British Cohort Study. Psychoso- matic Medicine ,70, 397 –403. Gangestad , S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). Human sexual selectionand developmental stability. In J.A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 169–195). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Ganiban , J. M., Saudino, K. J., Ulbricht, J., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Reiss, D. (2008). Stability and change in temperament during adolescence. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,95, 222 –236. Garb , H. N., Wood, J. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2005).Roots of the Rorschach contro-versy. Clinical Psychology Review , 25,9 7–118. Garcia , J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence inavoidance learning. Psychometric Science ,4, 123 –124. Garcia , S., Stinson, L., Ickes, W., Bissonnette, V., & Briggs, S. R. (1991). Shyness and physical attractiveness in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,61,3 5 –49. Gasper , K., Lozinski, R. H., & LeBeau, L. S. (2009). If you plan, then youREFERENCES 471
can: How reflection helps defensive pessimists pursue their goals. Motivation and Emotion ,33, 203–216. Gastorf , J. W. (1980). Time urgency of the Type A behavior pattern. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,48, 299. Gaudiano , B. A. (2005). Cognitive behavior therapies for psychotic disorders: Current empirical status and future directions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice , 12,3 3–50. Geary , D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin ,126,5 5 –77. Geen , R. G. (1983). The psychophysi- ology of extraversion-introversion.In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty(Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook (pp. 391 –416). New York: Guilford. Geen , R. G. (1984). Preferred stimula- tion levels in introverts and extra- verts: Effects on arousal and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,46, 1303 –1312. Geen , R. G. (1998). Aggression and an- tisocial behavior. in D. T. Gilbert,S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, 4th ed., pp. 317 –356). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Geen , R. G., & Quanty, M. B. (1977). The catharsis of aggression: An evaluation of a hypothesis. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 1 –37). New York: Academic Press. Geen , R. G., Stonner, D., & Shope, G. L. (1975). The facilitation of aggres-sion by aggression: Evidence against the catharsis hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,31, 721 –726. Geen , R. G., & Thomas, S. L. (1986). The immediate effects of mediaviolence on behavior. Journal of Social Issues ,42,7–27. Geers , A. L., Wellman, J. A., & Lassiter, G. D. (2009). Dispositional opti-mism and engagement: The moder-ating influence of goal prioritization. Journal of Personality and Social Ps ychology ,96,9 1 3 –932. Gemar , M. C., Segal, Z. V., Sagrati, S., & Kennedy, S. J. (2001). Mood- induced changes on the implicit as- sociation test in recovered depressedpatients. Journal of Abnormal Ps ychology ,11 0 ,2 8 2 –289. Gendlin , E. T. (1988). Carl Rogers (1902– 1987). American Psychologist ,43, 127 –128. Gentile , D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of violent video game habits onadolescent hostility, aggressivebehaviors, and school performance. Journal of Adolescence ,27,5–22. Geraerts , E., Lindsay, D. S., Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M., Raymaekers, L., Arnold, M. M., et al. (2009). Cognitive mechanisms underlying recovered memoryexperiences of childhood sexualabuse. Psychological Science ,20, 92–98. Gershoff , E. T. (2002). Corporal pun- ishment by parents and associatedchild behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical re- view. Psychological Bulletin ,128, 539–579. Gersten, M. (1989). Behavioral inhibi- tion in the classroom. In J. S. Reznick (Ed.), Perspectives on behavioral inhibition (pp. 71 –91). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gest, S. D. (1997). Behavioral inhibition: Stability and associations with adap- tation from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of Personality an d Social Psychology ,72,4 6 7 –475. Geuens , M., & De Pelsmacker, P. (1999). Affect intensity revisited:Individual differences and the com- munication effects of emotional sti- muli. Psychology and Marketing , 16, 195–209. Gibbons , F. X., Blanton, H., Gerrard, M., Buunk, B., & Eggleston, T.(2000). Does social comparisonmake a difference? Optimism as a moderator of the relation between comparison level and academicperformance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,26, 637–648. Gibson , H. B. (1981). Hans Eysenck: The man and his work . London: Peter Owen. Gidron , Y., Davidson, K., & Bata, I. (1999). The short-term effects of ahostility-reduction intervention onmale coronary heart disease patients. Health Psychology ,18,4 1 6 –420. Giesler , R. B., Josephs, R. A., & Swann, W. B. (1996). Self-verification in clinical depression: The desire for negative evaluation. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology ,105, 358–368. Gilbert , L., Deutsch, C. L., & Strahan, R. F. (1978). Feminine and mascu- line dimensions of the typical,desirable and ideal woman and man. Sex Roles ,4, 767 –778. Gill, M. M., & Brenman, M. (1967). The metapsychology of regression and hypnosis. In J. E. Gordon (Ed.), The handbook of clinical and ex- perimental hypnosis (pp. 281 –318). New York: Macmillan. Gillespie , N. A., Zhu, G., Evans, D. M., Medland, S. E., Wright, M. J., & Marti, N. G. (2008). A genome- wide scan for Eysenckian personal-ity dimensions in adolescent twin sibships: Psychoticism, extraver- sion, neuroticism and lie. Journal of Personality ,76, 1415 –1445. Giltay , E. J., Geleijnse, K. M., Zitman, F. G., Buijsse, B., & Kromhout, D. (2007). Lifestyle and dietary corre-lates of dispositional optimism inmen: The Zutphen Elderly Study. Journal of PsychosomaticMedicine ,63, 483 –490. Glasberg , R., & Aboud, F. (1982). Keeping one ’s distance from sadness: Children ’s self-reports of emotional experience. Develop- mental Psychology ,18, 287 –293. Glass, C. R., & Shea, C. A. (1986). Cognitive therapy for shyness andsocial anxiety. In W. H. Jones,J. M. Cheek, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 315 –327). New York: Plenum. Glass, D. C. (1977). Behavior patterns, stress, and coronary disease . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Glisky , M. L., Tataryn, D. J., Tobias, B. A., Kihlstrom, J. F., & McConkey, K. M. (1991). Absorption, openness to experi- ence, and hypnotizability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,60, 263 –272. Gohm , C. L., & Clore, G. L. (2000). Individual differences in emotionalexperience: Mapping availablescales to processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,26, 679–697. Gol, A. R., & Cook, S. W. (2004). Exploring the underlying dimen- sions of coping: A concept mapping approach. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,23, 155 –171. Goldberg, J. F., Gerstein, R. K., Wenze, S. J., Beck, A. T., & Welker, T. A.472 REFERENCES
(2008). Dysfunctional attitudes and cognitive schemas in bipolar manic and unipolar depressed outpatients: Implications for cognitively basedpsychotherapeutics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease ,196, 207–210. Goldberg , L. R. (1992). The develop- ment of markers for the Big-Fivefactor structure. Psychological Assessment ,4,2 6 –42. Goldberg , L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist ,48,2 6 –34. Goldberg , L. R. (2001). Analyses of Digman ’s child-personality data: Derivation of Big-Five factor scoresfrom each of six samples. Journal of Personality ,69, 709 –743. Goode, E. (1999, April 23). Homosexuality-gene study re- leased: Research fails to support report of chromosomal link. San Jose Mercury News , p. 8A. Goodwin , R., Cook, O., & Yung, Y. (2001). Loneliness and life satisfac-tion among three cultural groups. Personal Relationships ,8,2 2 5 –230. Gorassini , D. R., Sowerby, D., Creighton, A., & Fry, G. (1991).Hypnotic susceptibility enhance- ment through brief cognitive skill training. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,61, 289 –297. Gorassini , D. R., & Spanos, N. P. (1986). A social-cognitive skillsapproach to the successful modifi-cation of hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,50, 1004 –1012. Goswick , R. A., & Jones, W. H. (1981). Loneliness, self-concept, and adjustment. Journal of Psychology , 88, 258–261. Gotay , C. C. (1981). Cooperation and competition as a function of Type Abehavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,7, 386 –392. Gough , H. G., Fioravanti, M., & Lazzari, R. (1983). Some implica-tions of self versus ideal-self con- gruence on the Revised Adjective Check List. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 1214 –1220. Gough , H. G., Lazzari, R., & Fioravanti, M. (1978). Self versusideal self: A comparison of five adjective check list indices. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,46, 1085 –1091. Graham , S. M., Huang, J. Y., Clark, M. S., & Helgeson, V. S. (2008).The positives of negative emotions: Willingness to express negative emotions promotes relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,34, 394 –406. Granberg , E. (2006). “Is that all there is?”Possible selves, self-change, and weight loss. Social Psychology Quarterly ,69, 109 –126. Gray , J. A. (1982). The neuropsychol- ogy of anxiety: An inquiry of the septo-hippocampal system . Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Gray , J. A. (1987). Perspectives on anxiety and impulsivity: A com- mentary. Journal of Research in Personality ,21, 493 –509. Gray , J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety . Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Graziano , W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Steele, R. G., & Hair, E. C. (1998). Unknown words in self-reported personality: Lethargic andprovincial in Texas. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,24, 893–905. Graziano , W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Sullivan-Logan, G. M. (1998). Temperament, activity, and expectations for later personalitydevelopment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1266 –1277. Graziano , W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Todd, M., & Finch, J. F. (1997).Interpersonal attraction from an evolutionary psychology perspec- tive: Women ’s reactions to domi- nant and prosocial men. InJ. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psy- chology (pp. 141 –167). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Graziano , W. G., Habashi, M. M., Sheese, B. E., & Tobin, R. M. (2007). Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: A person X situationperspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,93, 583–599. Green , B. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (1994). The attractiveness of gender-typed traits at different relationship levels: Androgynous characteristics maybe desirable after all. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,20, 244–253. Green , J. P. (2004). The five factor model of personality and hypnotizability:Little variance in common. Con- te mporary Hypnosis ,21,1 6 1 –168.Green , J. P., Page, R. A., Handley, G. W., & Rasekhy, R. (2005). The “hidden observer ”and ideomotor responding: A real-simulator com- parison. Contemporary Hypnosis , 22, 123–137. Green , L. R., Richardson, D. S., Lago, T., & Schatten- Jones, E. C. (2001). Network correlates of social andemotional loneliness in young and older adults. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 281 –288. Greenberg , M. A., Wortman, C. B., & Stone, A. A. (1996). Emotionalexpression and physical health: Revising traumatic memories or fostering self-regulation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 71, 588–602. Greenberg , R., Pillard, R., & Pearlman, C. (1978). The effect of dream(stage REM) deprivation on adap- tation to stress. In S. Fisher & R. P. Greenberg (Eds.), The scien- tific evaluation of Freud ’s theories and therapy (pp. 40 –48). New York: Basic Books. Greening , L., Stoppelbein, L., & Docter, R. (2002). The mediating effects ofattributional style and event- specific attributions on postdisaster adjustment. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,26, 261 –274. Greve , W., & Wentura, D. (2003). Immunizing the self: Self-conceptstabilization through reality-adaptive self-definitions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,29,3 9 –50. Gronnerod , C. (2004). Rorschach assessment of changes following psychotherapy: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality Assessment ,83, 256 –276. Gross , J. J., & John, O. P. (1998). Mapping the domain of expressiv-ity: Multimethod evidence for a hierarchical model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 170–191. Grove , J. R., Hanrahan, S. J., & McInman, A. (1991). Success/ failure bias in attributions acrossinvolvement categories in sport. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,17,9 3 –97. Gruzelier , J. H. (2006). Frontal func- tions, connectivity and neural effi- ciency underpinning hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. Contempo- rary Hypnosis ,23,1 5 –23. Guerra , N. G., Huesmann, L. R., Tolan, P. H., Van Acker, R., & Eron, L. D.REFERENCES 473
(1995). Stressful events and indi- vidual beliefs as correlates of eco- nomic disadvantage and aggression among urban children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,63, 518 –528. Guimond , S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., & Redersdorff, S.(2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 90, 221–242. Gunthert , K. C., Cohen, L. H., & Armeli, S. (1999). The role of Neuroticism in daily stress and coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,77, 1087 –1100. Gur, R. C., & Reivich, M. (1980). Cognitive task effects on hemi-spheric blood flow in humans:Evidence for individual differences in hemispheric activation. Brain and Language ,9,7 8 –92. Guyll , M., & Contrada, R. J. (1998). Trait hostility and ambulatorycardiovascular activity: Responses to social interaction. Health Psychology ,17,3 0 –39. Gwaltney , C. J., Shiffman, S., Balabanis, M. H., & Paty, J. A. (2005). Dynamic self-efficacy and outcomeexpectancies: Prediction of smokinglapse and relapse. Journal of Ab- normal Psychology ,114, 661 –675. Haans , A., Kaiser, F. G., & de Kort, Y. A. W. (2007). Privacy needs in office environments: Development of two behavior-based scales. European Psychologist ,12,9 3 –102. Haeffel , G. J., Abramson, L. Y., Voelz, Z. R., Metalsky, G. I., Halberstadt,L., Dykman, B. M., et al. (2003). Cognitive vulnerability to depres- sion and lifetime history of Axis Ipsychopathology: A comparison of negative cognitive styles (CSQ) and dysfunctional attitudes (DAS). Journal of CognitivePsychotherapy ,17,3–22. Haemmerlie , F. M., & Montgomery, R. L. (1986). Self-perception theory and the treatment of shyness. InW. H. Jones, J. M. Cheek, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treat- ment (pp. 329 –342). New York: Plenum. Hall, C., Smith, K., & Chia, R. (2008). Cognitive and personality factors in relation to timely completion of acollege degree. College Student Journal ,42, 1087 –1098.Hall, C. S. (1953). A cognitive theory of dream symbols. Journal of General Psychology ,48, 169 –186. Hall, C. S. (1984). “A ubiquitous sex difference in dreams ”revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,46, 1109 –1117. Hall, C. S., & Domhoff, B. (1963). A ubiquitous sex difference in dreams. Journal of Abnormal and SocialPsychology ,66, 278 –280. Hall, M. H. (1968, August). A conver- sation with the president of the American Psychological Association: The psychology of universality. Psychology Today , pp. 35–37, 54 –57. Handler , L. (1996). The clinical use of drawings: Draw-a-person, house- tree-person, and kinetic family drawings. In C. S. Newmark (Ed.), Major psychological assessment in- struments (2nd ed., pp. 206 –293). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Hankin , B. L. (2008). Stability of cog- nitive vulnerabilities to depression: A short-term prospective multiwavestudy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,117, 324 –233. Hankin , B. L., Fraley, R. C., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2005). Daily depressionand cognitions about stress:Evidence for a traitlike depresso- genic cognitive style and the pre- diction of depressive symptoms in aprospective daily diary study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,88, 673 –685. Hanley-Dunn , P., Maxwell, S. E., & Santos, J. F. (1985). Interpretation of interpersonal interaction: The influence of loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,11, 445–456. Harackiewicz , J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessaryand illuminating. Journal of Edu- cational Psychology ,94, 638 –645. Hardin , E. E., & Lakin, J. L. (2009). The Integrated Self-Discrepancy Index: A reliable and valid measure of self-discrepancies. Journal of Per- so nality Assessment ,91,2 4 5 –253. Harmon-Jones , E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1997). Behavioral activation sensi-tivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: Covariation of putative indicators related to risk for mooddisorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,106, 159 –163.Harmon-Jones , E., Lueck, L., Fearn, M., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2006). The effect of personal relevance and approach-related action expecta-tion on relative left frontal corticalactivity. Psychological Science ,17, 434–440. Harmon-Jones , E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that insult- related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with expe-rienced anger and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,80, 797 –803. Harris , C. R. (2003). A review of sex differences in sexual jealousy, including self-report data, psycho- physiological responses, interper- sonal violence, and morbidjealousy. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,7, 102 –128. Harris , M. B. (1974). Mediators be- tween frustration and aggression ina field experiment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 10, 561–571. Harrison , A. A., & Saeed, L. (1977). Let’s make a deal: An analysis of revelations and stipulations in lonely hearts advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,35, 257 –264. Harrist , S., Carlozzi, B. L., McGovern, A. R., & Harrist, A. W. (2007).Benefits of expressive writing andexpressive talking about life goals. Journal of Research in Personality , 41, 923–930. Hartshorne , H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character:Studies in deceit . New York: Macmillan. Harvey , J. H., & Omarzu, J. (1997). Minding the close relationship. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,1, 224 –240. Havermans , R., Nicolson, N. A., & deVries, M. W. (2007). Daily hassles, uplifts, and time use in individuals with bipolar disorder in remission. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease ,195, 745 –751. Hawkley , L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T.(2003). Loneliness in everyday life:Cardiovascular activity, psychoso-cial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,85, 105 –120. Hawkley , L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Aging and loneliness: Downhill quickly? Current474 REFERENCES
Directions in Psychological Science , 16, 187–191. Hawkley , L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Loneliness is a unique predictor ofage-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Psychology and Aging ,21, 152 –164. Hawkley , L. C., Thisted, R. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2009). Lonelinesspredicts reduced physical activity: Cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychology ,28, 354–363. Haynes , S. N. (2001). Introduction to the special section on clinical ap-plications of analogue behavioralobservation. Psychological Assessment ,13,3–4. Hazan , C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 52, 511–524. Headey , B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjec- tive well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 57, 731–739. Heath , A. C., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Eaves, L. J., & Kendler, K. S.(1992). Evidence for genetic influ-ences on personality from self- reports and informant ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,63,8 5 –96. Heatherton , T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of ascale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,60, 895 –910. Hedlund , S., & Rude, S. S. (1995). Evidence of latent depressive sche-mas in formerly depressed indivi-duals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,104, 517 –525. Heery , E. A., & Kring, A. M. (2007). Interpersonal consequences of so- cial anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,116, 125 –134. Heimpel , S. A., Wood, J. V., Marshall, M. A., & Brown, J. D. (2002). Dopeople with low self-esteem really want to feel better? Self-esteem dif- ferences in motivation to repairnegative moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 82, 128–147. Heine , S. J. (2001). Self as cultural product: An examination of EastAsian and North American selves. Journal of Personality ,69,8 8 1 –906.Heine , S. J., & Hamamura, T. (2007). In search of East Asian self- enhancement. Personality and So cial Psychology Review ,11,4–27. Heine , S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1995). Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: Does the West feel more invulnerable than the East? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,68, 595 –607. Heine , S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1999). Culture, self-discrepancies, and self- satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,25, 915 –925. Heine , S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Isthere a universal need for positiveself-regard? Psychological Review , 106, 766–794. Heinrich , L. M., & Gullone, E. (2006). The clinical significance of loneli-ness: A literature review. Clinical Psychology Review ,26, 695 –718. Heinrichs , N., Rapee, R. M., Alden, L. A., Bogels, S., Hofmann, S. G., Oh, K. J., & Sakano, Y. (2005). Cultural differences in perceived social norms and social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy , 44, 1187 –1197. Helgeson , V. S. (1994). Relation of agency and communion to well-being: Evidence and potential ex-planations. Psychological Bulletin , 116, 412–428. Helgeson , V. S. (2003). Unmitigated communion and adjustment tobreast cancer: Associations and ex- planations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology ,33, 1643 –1661. Helgeson , V. S., Cohen, S., & Fritz, H. L. (1998). Social ties and cancer.In J. C. Holland & W. Breitbart (Eds.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 99 – 109). New York: Oxford University Press. Helgeson , V. S., Escobar, O., Siminerio, L., & Becker, D. (2007). Unmitigated communion andhealth among adolescents with and without diabetes: The mediating role of eating disturbances. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,33, 519 –536. Helgeson , V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (1999). Unmitigated agency and unmiti- gated communion: Distinctionsfrom agency and communion. Journal of Research in Personality , 33, 131–158. Helgeson , V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (2000). The implications of unmitigated agency and unmitigated communionfor domains of problem behavior. Journal of Personality ,68, 1031 –1057. Helgeson , V. S., & Lepore, S. J. (1997). Men ’s adjustment to prostate can- cer: The role of agency andunmitigated agency. Sex Roles ,37, 251–267. Helgeson , V. S., & Lepore, S. J. (2004). Quality of life following prostatecancer: The role of agency and unmitigated agency. Journal of Applied Social Psychology ,34, 2559 –2585. Helmes , E., & Reddon, J. R. (1993). A perspective on developments in as-sessing psychopathology: A criticalreview of the MMPI and MMPI-2. Psychological Bulletin ,113, 453–471. Hemenover , S. H. (2003). The good, the bad, and the healthy: Impacts of emotional disclosure of trauma on resilient self-concept and psycho-logical distress. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,29, 1236 –1244. Henriques , J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (1990). Regional brain electricalasymmetries discriminate between previously depressed and healthy control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,99,2 2 –31. Herrnstein , R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence andclass structure in American life . New York: Free Press. Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2004). On the selective relation of frontal cortical asymmetry andanger-out versus anger-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,87, 926 –939. Hibbard , S., Farmer, L., Wells, C., Difillipo, E., Barry, W., Korman, R., & Sloan, P. (1994). Validation of Cramer ’s Defense Mechanism Manual for the TAT. Journal of Personality Assessment ,63, 197–210. Higgins , E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review ,94, 319 –340. Higgins , E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs cause people to suffer? In L. Berko-witz (Ed.), Advances in experimen- tal social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 93–136). San Diego: Academic Press. Hilgard , E. R. (1973). A neodissociation interpretation of pain reduction inREFERENCES 475
hypnosis. Psychological Review , 80, 396–411. Hilgard , E. R. (1977). Divided con- sciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action . New York: Wiley. Hilgard , E. R. (1992). Dissociation and theories of hypnosis. In E. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 69 –101). New York: Guilford. Hilgard , E. R. (1994). Neodissociation theory. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical, theo- retical and research perspectives (pp. 32 –51). New York: Guilford. Hill, C. E. (1996). Working with dreams in psychotherapy . New York: Guilford. Hill, C. E., & Knox, S. (2001). Self- disclosure. Psychotherapy ,38, 413–417. Hill, G. J. (1989). An unwillingness to act: Behavioral appropriateness, situational constraint, and self-efficacy in shyness. Journal of Personality ,57, 871 –890. Hiroto , D. S. (1974). Locus of control and learned helplessness. Journal of Experimental Psychology ,102, 187–193. Hiroto , D. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Generality of learned help-lessness in man. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 31, 311–327. Hoffman , L. W. (1985). The changing genetics/socialization balance. Journal of Social Issues ,41, 127–148. Hoffman , L. W. (1991). The influence of the family environment on person- ality: Accounting for sibling differ- ences. Psychological Bulletin ,110, 187–203. Hogan , R. (1991). Personality and personality measurement. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol.2, pp. 873 –919). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Hojnoski , R. L., Morrison, R., Brown, M., & Matthews, W. J. (2009). Projective test use among school psychologists. Journal of Psychoe- du cational Assessment ,24,1 4 5 –159. Hollon , S. D., Stewart, M. O., & Strunk, D. (2006). Enduring effects forcognitive behavior therapy in thetreatment of depression and anxi-ety. Annual Review of Psychology , 57, 285–315.Holloway , R. A., Waldrip, A. M., & Ickes, W. (2009). Evidence that a simpatico self-schema accounts for differences in the self-concepts and social behavior of Latinos versus Whites (and Blacks). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 96, 1012 –1028. Hoobler , J. M., & Brass, D. J. (2006). Abusive supervision and family undermining as displaced aggres- sion. Journal of Applied Psychology ,91, 1125 –1133. Hooker , K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K., & Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mentaland physical health of spouse care-givers: The role of personality. Psychology and Aging ,7, 367 –375. Hopkins, J. R. (1995). Erik Homburger Erikson (1902– 1994). American Psychologist ,50, 796 –797. Horley , J. (1996). Content stability in the repertory grid: An examinationusing a forensic sample. International Journal of OffenderTherapy and Comparative Criminology ,40,2 6 –31. Horn , J. (2001). Raymond Bernard Cattell (1905– 1998). American Psychologist ,56,7 1 –72. Horney , K. (1945/1966). Our inner conflicts: A constructive theory of neurosis . New York: Norton. Horney , K. (1967). Feminine psychol- ogy . New York: Norton. Hornstein , G. A. (1985). Intimacy in conversational style as a function of the degree of closeness between members of a dyad. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 49, 671–681. Hornstein , G. A., & Truesdell, S. E. (1988). Development of intimate conversation in close relationships. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,7,4 9 –64. Horowitz , L. M., & de Sales French, R. (1979). Interpersonal problems ofpeople who describe themselves aslonely. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,47, 762 –764. Howard , M. L., & Coe, W. C. (1980). The effects of context and subjects ’ perceived control in breaching posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Personality ,48, 342 –359. Hoyt , M. F., & Singer, J. L. (1978). Psychological effects of REM(“dream ”) deprivation upon wak- ing mentation. In A. M. Arkin, J. S. Antrobus, & S. J. Ellman(Eds.), The mind in sleep: Psychology and psychophysiology(pp. 487 –510). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Huesmann , L. R. (1988). An information-processing model forthe development of aggression. Aggressive Behavior ,14,1 3 –24. Huesmann , L. R., Eron, L. D., Dubow, E. F., & Seebauer, E. (1987).Television viewing habits in child-hood and adult aggression. Child Development ,58, 357 –367. Huesmann , L. R., Eron, L. D., & Yarmel, P. W. (1987). Intellectual functioning and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 232 –240. Huesmann , L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children ’s normative beliefs about aggression and aggressivebehavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,72, 408–419. Hui, C. H. (1988). Measurement of individualism-collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality ,22, 17–36. Hunsley , J., & Bailey, J. M. (1999). The clinical utility of the Rorschach: Unfulfilled promises and an uncer-tain future. Psychological Assessment ,11, 266 –277. Hurtz , G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: The Big Five revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology ,85, 869 –879. Hyde , J. S., & Kling, K. C. (2001). Women, motivation and achieve-ment. Psychology of Women Quarterly ,25, 364 –378. Ickes, W. (1993). Traditional gender roles: Do they make, and thenbreak, our relationships? Journal of Social Issues ,49,7 1 –85. Ickes, W., & Barnes, R. D. (1978). Boys and girls together and alienated: Onenacting stereotyped sex roles in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 36, 669–683. Ickes, W., Robertson, E., Tooke, W., & Teng, G. (1986). Naturalistic social cognition: Methodology, assess- ment, and validation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 51,6 6–82. Ickes, W., Schermer, B., & Steeno, J. (1979). Sex and sex-role influencein same-sex dyads. Social Psychology Quarterly ,42, 373–385. Ihilevich , D., & Gleser, G. C. (1993). Defense mechanisms: Their classi- fication, correlates, and476 REFERENCES
measurement with the Defense Mechanism Inventory . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Ilardi , S. S., & Craighead, W. E. (1999). The relationship between personal- ity pathology and dysfunctional cognitions in previously depressedadults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,108,5 1 –57. Ilgen, M., McKellar, J., & Tiet, Q. (2005). Abstinence self-efficacy andabstinence 1 year after substanceuse disorder treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,73, 1175 –1180. Ingram , R. E., & Ritter, J. (2000). Vulnerability to depression: Cognitive reactivity and parental bonding in high-risk individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 109, 588–596. Jackson , B., Kubzansky, L. D., Cohen, S., Jacobs, D. R., & Wright, R. J. (2007). Does harboring hostilityhurt? Associations between hostility and pulmonary function in the Coronary Artery Risk Developmentin (young) Adults (CARDIA) study. Health Psychology ,26, 333 –340. Jackson , C. J. (2009). Jackson-5 scales of revised Reinforcement SensitivityTheory (r-RST) and their applica-tion to dysfunctional real world outcomes. Journal of Research in Personality ,43, 556 –569. Jackson , L. A. (1983). The perception of androgyny and physical attractive-ness: Two is better than one. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,9, 405 –413. Jackson , L. A., Ialongo, N., & Stollak, G. E. (1986). Parental correlates ofgender role: The relations betweenparents ’masculinity, femininity, and child-rearing behaviors and their children ’s gender roles. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,4, 204 –224. Jahoda , M. (1977). Freud and the di- lemmas of psychology . New York: Basic Books. Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the Big Five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. Journal of Personality ,64, 577 –591. Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J.(1998). Heritability of facet-leveltraits in a cross-cultural twin sam-ple: Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology , 74, 1556 –1565. Janoff-Bulman , R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psy- chology of trauma . New York: Free Press. Jefferson , T., Herbst, J. H., & McCrae, R. R. (1998). Associations between birth order and personality traits:Evidence from self-reports and ob- server ratings. Journal of Research in Personality ,32, 498 –509. Jeffrey , A., & Austin, T. (2007). Perspectives and practices of clini-cian self-disclosure to clients: A pilot comparison study of two disciplines. American Journal of Family Therapy ,35,9 5 –108. Jenkins , C. D. (1971). Psychologic and social precursors of coronary dis-ease. New England Journal of Medicine ,284, 244 –255, 307 –317. Jenkins , C. D. (1976). Recent evidence supporting psychologic and socialrisk factors for coronary disease. New England Journal of Medicine , 294, 987–994, 1033 –1038. Jenkins , C. D., Zyzanski, S. J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1976). Risk ofnew myocardial infarction in middle-age men with manifest cor- onary heart disease. Circulation , 53, 342–347. Jenkins , S. R. (1987). Need for achieve- ment and women ’s careers over 14 years: Evidence for occupationalstructure effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 53, 922–932. Jensen , A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achieve-ment? Harvard Educational Review ,39,1–123. Jensen-Campbell , L. A., & Graziano, W. G. (2001). Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict. Journal of Personality ,69, 323 –362. Jensen-Campbell , L. A., Graziano, W. G., & West, S. G. (1995).Dominance, prosocial orientation, and female preferences: Do nice guys really finish last? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 68, 427–440. Joca, S. R. L., Zanelati, T., & Guimaraes, F. S. (2006). Post-stressfacilitation of serotonergic, but notnoradrenergic, neurotransmission in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development inrats. Brain Research ,1087 ,6 7 –74. Johansson, B., Grant, J. D., Plomin, R., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Berg, S.,et al. (2001). Health locus of con- trol in late life: A study of genetic and environmental influences in twins aged 80 years and older. Health Psychology ,20,3 3 –40. John, O. P., Nauman, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy:History, measurement, and con-ceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality (3rd ed., pp. 114 –158). New York: Guilford. Johnson , J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes, E. M., Kasen, S., & Brook, J. S.(2002). Television viewing and ag-gressive behavior during adoles- cence and adulthood. Science ,295, 2468 –2471. Joiner , T. E., Catanzaro, S. J., Rudd, M. D., & Rajab, M. H. (1999). Thecase for a hierarchical, oblique, and bidimensional structure of loneli-ness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,18,4 7 –75. Jones , A., & Crandall, R. (1986). Validation of a short index of self-actualization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,12, 63–73. Jones , E. (1953– 1957). The life and work of Sigmund Freud (Vols. 1– 3). New York: Basic Books. Jones , R. E., Leen-Felder, E. W., Olatunji, B. O., Reardon, L. E., &Hawks, E. (2009). Psychometricproperties of the Affect Intensity and Reactivity Measure adapted for youth (AIR-Y). Psychological Assessment ,21, 162 –175. Jones , W. H., Freemon, J. E., & Goswick, R. A. (1981). The persis-tence of loneliness: Self and otherdeterminants. Journal of Personality ,49,2 7 –48. Jones , W. H., Hobbs, S. A., & Hockenbury, D. (1982). Lonelinessand social skill deficits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 42, 682–689. Jones , W. H., Sansone, C., & Helm, B. (1983). Loneliness and interpersonaljudgments. Personality and Social Ps ychology Bulletin ,9,4 3 7 –441. Jorgensen , R. S., Johnson, B. T., Kolodziej, M. E., & Schreer, G. E. (1996). Elevated blood pressure and personality: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin ,120, 293–320. Josephs , R. A., Markus, H. R., & Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). GenderREFERENCES 477
and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 63, 391–402. Jourard , S. M. (1971). The transparent self (2nd ed.). New York: Van Nostrand. Judge , T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self- evaluations traits —self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability — with job satisfaction and job per- formance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology ,86,8 0 –92. Judge , T. A., Erez, A., & Bono, J. E. (1998). The power of being positive: The relationship between positiveself-concept and job performance. Human Performance ,11,1 6 7 –187. Judge , T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The Big Five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Personnel Psychology ,52, 621 –652. Jung, C. G. (1902 –1961/1961). The collected works of Carl Jung (Vols. 1–17). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Jung, C. G. (1961). Memories, dreams, reflections . New York: Pantheon. Jung, C. G. (1964). Approaching the unconscious. In C. G. Jung (Ed.), Man and his symbols (pp. 3– 94). New York: Dell. Kagan , J. (1989). Temperamental con- tributions to social behavior. American Psychologist ,44, 668–674. Kagan , J. (2003). Biology, context, and developmental inquiry. Annual Review of Psychology ,54,1–23. Kagan , J., & Moss, H. A. (1962). Birth to maturity . New York: Wiley. Kagan , J., & Snidman, N. (1991a). Infant predictors of inhibited and uninhibited profiles. Psychological Science ,2,4 0 –44. Kagan , J., & Snidman, N. (1991b). Temperamental factors in humandevelopment. American Psychologist ,46, 856 –862. Kagan , J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kahn , J. H., & Hessling, R. M. (2001). Measuring the tendency to concealversus disclose psychological dis- tress. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,20,4 1 –65. Kalechstein , A. D., & Nowicki, S. (1997). A meta-analyticexamination of the relationship between control expectancies and academic achievement: An 11-yr. follow-up to Findley and Cooper. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs ,123, 27–56. Kallio , S., & Revonsuo, A. (2003). Hypnotic phenomena and altered states of consciousness: A multilevel framework of description and explanation. Contemporary Hypnosis ,20, 111 –164. Kallio , S., & Revonsuo, A. (2005). Altering the state of the altered statedebate: Reply to commentaries. Contemporary Hypnosis ,20, 46–55. Kamen-Siegel , L., Rodin, J., Seligman, M. E. P., & Dwyer, J. (1991).Explanatory style and cell-mediatedimmunity in elderly men and women. Health Psychology ,10, 229–235. Kang , S-M., Shaver, P. R., Sue, S., Min, K-H., & Jing, H. (2003). Culture-specific patterns in the prediction of life satisfaction: Roles of emotion,relationship quality, and self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Bu lletin ,29,1 5 9 6 –1608. Kaplan , A., & Maehr, M. L. (2007). The contributions and prospects of goal orientation theory. Educational Psychology Review ,19, 141 –184. Kaplan , G. D., & Cowles, A. (1978). Health locus of control and healthvalue in the prediction of smoking cessation. Health Education Monographs ,6, 129 –137. Kasen , S. Chen, H., Sneed, J., Crawford, T., & Cohen, P. (2006). Social roleand birth cohort influences on gender-linked personality traits in women: A 20-year longitudinalanalysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,91, 944 –958. Kashdan , T. B., & Roberts, J. E. (2006). Affective outcomes in superficial and intimate interactions: Roles of social anxiety and curiosity. Journal of Research in Personality ,40, 140–167. Katz, I. M., & Campbell, J. D. (1994). Ambivalence over emotional ex- pression and well-being: Nomothetic and idiographic tests ofthe stress-buffering hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,67, 513 –524. Kawachi , I., Sparrow, D., Spiro, A., Vokonas, P., & Weiss, S. T. (1996). A prospective study of anger andcoronary heart disease: The Normative Aging Study. Circulation ,94, 2090 –2095. Keelan , J. P. R., Dion, K. L., & Dion, K. K. (1994). Attachment style and heterosexual relationships among young adults: A short-term panel study. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships ,11, 201–214. Keller , J., & Bless, H. (2008). Flow and regulatory compatibility: An exper-imental approach to the flow modelof intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,34, 196–209. Kelly , A. E. (1998). Clients ’secret keeping in outpatient therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology , 45,5 0–57. Kelly , A. E., & Archer, J. A. (1995). Self- concealment and attitudes toward counseling in university students. Journal of Counseling Psychology , 42,4 0–46. Kelly , A. E., Klusas, J. A., von Weiss, R. T., & Kenny, C. (2001). What is it about revealing secrets that is beneficial? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 651 –665. Kelly , A. E., & McKillop, K. J. (1996). Consequences of revealing personalsecrets. Psychological Bulletin ,120, 450–465. Kelly , G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs . New York: Norton. Kelly , G. A. (1969). Clinical psychology an d personality: The selected papers of George Kelly . New York: Wiley. Kendzierski , D. (1988). Self-schemata and exercise. Basic and Applied Social Psychology ,9,4 5 –61. Kendzierski , D. (1990). Exercise self- schemata: Cognitive and behavioral correlates. Health Psychology ,9, 69–82. Kenrick , D. T., Keefe, R. C., Gabrielidis, C., & Cornelius, J. S. (1996). Adolescents ’age preferences for dating partners: Support for anevolutionary model of life-historystrategies. Child Development ,67, 1499 –1511. Kent, G. (1997). Dental phobias. In G. C. Davey (Ed.), Phobias: A handbook of theory, research and treatment (pp. 107 –127). Chichester, England: Wiley. Keogh , B. K. (1986). Temperament and schooling: Meaning of “Goodness of Fit ”? In J. V. Lerner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Temperament478 REFERENCES
and social interaction during in- fancy and childhood (pp. 89 –108). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Keogh , B. K. (1989). Applying temper- ament research to schools. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 437–450). New York: Wiley. Keogh , B. K. (2003). Temperament in the classroom: Understanding indi- vidual differences . Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Kern , M. L., & Friedman, H. S. (2008). Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychology ,27, 505 –512. Kernis , M. H., Brockner, J., & Frankel, B. S. (1989). Self-esteem and reac- tions to failure: The mediating roleof overgeneralization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 57, 707–714. Kihlstrom , J. F. (1985). Hypnosis. Annual Review of Psychology ,36, 385–418. Kihlstrom , J. F. (1998). Dissociations and dissociation theory in hypnosis:Comment on Kirsch and Lynn(1998). Psychological Bulletin ,123, 186–191. Kihlstrom , J. F. (2005). Is hypnosis an altered state of consciousness or what ? Contemporary Hypnosis ,22, 34–38. King, L. A. (1998). Ambivalence over emotional expression and readingemotions in situations and faces. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,74, 753 –762. King, L. A. (2001). The health benefits of writing about life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 798 –807. King, L. A., & Emmons, R. A. (1990). Conflict over emotional expression: Psychological and physical corre- lates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 864 –877. King, L. A., & Miner, K. N. (2000). Writing about the perceived bene- fits of traumatic events:Implications for physical health. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,26, 220 –230. Kirkpatrick , L. A., & Davis, K. E. (1994). Attachment style, gender, and relationship stability: A longi- tudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 66, 502–512. Kirsch , I. (1996). Hypnotic enhance- ment of cognitive-behavioral weightloss treatments —another meta- reanalysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,64, 517–519. Kirsch , I. (2000). The response set the- ory of hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis ,42, 274 –292. Kirsch , I. (2005). Empirical resolutions of the altered state debate. Contemporary Hypnosis ,22, 18–23. Kirsch , I., & Council, J. R. (1992). Situational and personality corre- lates of hypnotic responsiveness. In E. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 267 –291). New York: Guilford. Kirsch , I., & Lynn, J. L. (1998). Dissociation theories of hypnosis. Psychological Bulletin ,123, 100–115. Kirsch , I., & Lynn, S. J. (1995). The al- tered state of hypnosis: Changes in the theoretical landscape. American Psychologist ,50, 846 –858. Kirschenbaum , H. (1979). On becoming Carl Rogers . New York: Delacorte. Nisbett , R. E. (2007, December 9). All brains are the same color. New York Times . Nisbett , R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and culture count . New York: Norton. Kitayama , S., & Markus, H. R. (Eds.). (1994). Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influ- ence . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kivimaki , M., Vahtera, J., Elovainio, M., Helenius, H., Singh-Manoux,A., & Pentti, J. (2005). Optimism and pessimism as predictors of change in health after death or on-set of severe illness in family. Health Psychology ,24, 413 –421. Klein , D. C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Reversal of performancedeficits and perceptual deficits inlearned helplessness and depres- sion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,85,1 1 –26. Kleinke , C. L., & Kahn, M. L. (1980). Perceptions of self-disclosers:Effects of sex and physical attrac- tiveness. Journal of Personality ,48, 190–205. Klinesmith , J., Kasser, T., & McAndrew, F. T. (2006). Guns,testosterone, and aggression: An experimental test of a mediationalhypothesis. Psychological Science , 17, 568–572.Klinger, B. I. (1970). Effect of peer model responsiveness and length of induction procedure on hypnotic responsiveness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,75,1 5 –18. Klohnen , E. C., & Bera, S. (1998). Behavioral and experiential pat- terns of avoidantly and securely at-tached women across adulthood: A31-year longitudinal perspective. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,74, 211 –223. Klonowicz , T. (2001). Discontented people: Reactivity and locus of control as determinants of subjec- tive well-being. European Journal of Personality ,15,2 9 –47. Knee , C. R., Canevello, A., Bush, A. L., & Cook, A. (2008). Relationship- contingent self-esteem and the upsand downs of romantic relation-ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,95, 608 –627. Knowles , E. S., & Nathan, K. T. (1997). Acquiescent responding in self- reports: Cognitive style or social concern? Journal of Research in Personality ,31, 293 –301. Kobasa , S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and health: Aninquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 37,1–11. Konecni , V. J., & Doob, A. N. (1972). Catharsis through displacement of aggression. Journal of Personality an d Social Psychology ,23,3 7 9 –387. Kop, W. J., Berman, D. S., Gransar, H., Wong, N. D., Miranda-Peats, R., White, M. D., et al. (2005). Socialnetwork and coronary artery calci-fication in asymptomatic indivi- duals. Psychosomatic Medicine ,67, 343–352. Koppitz , E. M. (1968). Psychological evaluation of children ’s human figure drawings . New York: Grune & Stratton. Korabik, K. (1982). Sex-role orientation and impressions: A comparison ofdiffering genders and sex roles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,8,2 5 –30. Koriat , A., Melkman, R., Averill, J. R., & Lazarus, R. S. (1972). The self-control of emotional reactions to astressful film. Journal of Personality ,40, 601 –619. Korn , J. H., Davis, R., & Davis, S. F. (1991). Historians ’and chairper- sons’judgments of eminence among psychologists. American Psychologist ,46, 789 –792.REFERENCES 479
Kram , M. L., Kramer, G. L., Steciuk, M., Ronan, P. J., & Petty, F. (2000). Effects of learned helpless- ness on brain GABA receptors. Neuroscience Research ,38, 193–196. Krantz , D. S., & McCeney, M. K. (2002). Effects of psychological andsocial factors on organic disease: Acritical assessment of research on coronary heart disease. Annual Review of Psychology ,53, 341–369. Kring , A. M., & Gordon, A. H. (1998). Sex differences in emotion: Expression, experience, and physi- ology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,74, 686 –703. Kring , A. M., Smith, D. A., & Neale, J. M. (1994). Individual differencesin dispositional expressiveness:Development and validation of the Emotional Expressivity Scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,66, 934 –949. Krokoff , L. J. (1990). Job distress is no laughing matter in marriage, or isit? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships ,8,5–25. Krueger , R. F., & Johnson, W. (2008). Behavioral genetics and personality.In L. A. Pervin, O. P. John, &R. W. Robins (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 287 –310). New York: Guilford. Krueger , R. F., South, S., Johnson, W., & Iacono, W. (2008). The herita- bility of personality is not always 50%: Gene-environment interac-tions and correlations betweenpersonality and parenting. Journal of Personality ,76, 1485 –1521. Kuebli , J., Butler, S., & Fivush, R. (1995). Mother-child talk about past emotions: Relations of mater- nal language and child gender over time. Cognition and Emotion ,9, 265–283. Kuhlman , T. L. (1985). A study of sa- lience and motivational theories of humor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,49, 281 –286. Kuiper , N. A., & Derry, P. A. (1981). The self as a cognitive prototype: An application to person perceptionand depression. In N. Cantor &J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and social interaction (pp. 215 –231). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kuiper , N. A., MacDonald, M. R., & Derry, P. A. (1983). Parameters of adepressive self-schema. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 191 –217). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kuiper , N. A., & Martin, R. A. (1998). Laughter and stress in daily life: Relation to positive and negative affect. Motivation and Emotion , 22, 133–143. Kuiper , N. A., McKenzie, S. D., & Belanger, K. A. (1995). Cognitiveappraisal and individual differencesin sense of humor: Motivational and affective implications. Personality and Individual Differences ,19, 359 –372. Kulick, J. A., & Harackiewicz, J. (1979). Opposite-sex interpersonal attrac-tion as a function of the sex roles ofthe perceiver and the perceived. Sex Roles ,5, 443 –452. Kuppens , P., Realo, A., & Diener, E. (2008). The role of positive andnegative emotions in life satisfac-tion judgment across nations. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,95,6 6 –75. Kurdek, L. A., & Schmitt, J. P. (1986). Interaction of sex role self-concept with relationship quality and relationship beliefs in married, heterosexual cohabiting, gay andlesbian couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 51, 365–370. Kwon , P. (2000). Hope and dysphoria: The moderating role of defense mechanisms. Journal of Personality ,68, 199 –223. LaFraniere , S. (2009, June 13). An all- nighter? For this test, some Chinesecram all year. New York Times , p. A4. LaGasse , L., Gruber, C., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1989). The infantile expression ofactivity in relation to later assess- ments. In J. S. Reznick (Ed.), Perspectives on behavioral inhibi-tion (pp. 159 –176). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lahey , B. B. (2009). Public health sig- nificance of Neuroticism. American Psychologist ,64, 241 –256. Lai, J. C. L., & Wong, W. S. (1998). Optimism and coping with unem-ployment among Hong KongChinese women. Journal of Research in Personality ,32, 454–479. Lamke , L. K., & Bell, N. J. (1982). Sex-role orientation and relation- ship development in same-sexdyads. Journal of Research in Personality ,16, 343 –354. Landau, S. F. (1988). Violent crime and its relation to subjective social stress indicators: The case of Israel. Aggressive Behavior ,14, 337 –362. Landau, S. F., & Raveh, A. (1987). Stress factors, social support, andviolence in Israeli society: A quan-titative analysis. Aggressive Behavior ,13,6 7 –85. Landy , F. J., Shankster, L. J., & Kohler, S. S. (1994). Personnel selection and placement. Annual Review of Psychology ,45, 261 –296. Langens , T. A., & Schuler, J. (2007). Effects of written emotional ex-pression: The role of positive expectancies. Health Psychology , 26, 174–182. Langer , E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and enhanced per-sonal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an institutionalsetting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,34, 191 –198. Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Can peoplefeel happy and sad at the sametime? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,81, 684 –696. Larsen, R. J. (1987). The stability of mood variability: A spectral ana- lytic approach to daily mood as- sessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 1195 –1204. La rsen, R. J., Billings, D. W., & Cutler, S. E. (1996). Individual differences in informational style: Associations with dispositional affect intensity. Journal of Personality ,64,1 8 5 –207. Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individualdifference characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Personality , 21,1–39. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Cropanzano, R. S. (1987). Cognitive operationsassociated with individual differ- ences in affect intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 53, 767–774. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). An evaluation of subjective well-being measures. Social Indicators Research ,17, 1–17. Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1986). Affect intensity and reactions to daily life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,51, 803 –814.480 REFERENCES
Larsen, R. J., & Kasimatis, M. (1990). Individual differences in entrain- ment of mood to the weekly calendar. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 164–171. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extraversion, neuroticism and sus-ceptibility to positive and negativemood induction procedures. Personality and IndividualDifferences ,10, 1221 –1228. Larson , D. G., & Chastain, R. L. (1990). Self-concealment: Conceptualization, measurement, and health implica- tions. Journal of Social and Clinical Ps ychology ,9,4 3 9 –455. Larson , R. W. (1990). The solitary side of life: An examination of the time people spend alone from childhoodto old age. Developmental Review , 10, 155–183. Larson , R. W. (1997). The emergence of solitude as a constructive domain ofexperience in early adolescence. Child Development ,68,8 0 –93. Larson , R. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1980). The significance of solitudein adolescents ’development. Journal of Current Adolescent Medicine ,2,3 3 –40. Larson , R. W., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1982). Time alone in daily experience: Loneliness or renewal? In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 40 –53). New York: Wiley. Larson , R. W., & Johnson, C. (1985). Bulimia: Disturbed patterns ofsolitude. Addictive Behaviors ,10, 281–290. Larson , R. W., & Lee, M. (1996). The capacity to be alone as a stressbuffer. Journal of Social Psychology ,136,5–16. Larson , R. W., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Daily companionship in late childhood and early adoles- cence: Changing developmental contexts. Child Development ,62, 284–300. Larson , R. W., Zuzanek, J., & Mannell, R. (1985). Being alone versus being with people: Disengagement in the daily experience of older adults. Journal of Gerontology ,40, 375–381. Lau, J. Y. F., & Eley, T. C. (2008). Attributional style as a risk markerof genetic effects for adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology ,117, 849–859. Lau, R. R., Hartman, K. A., & Ware, J. E. (1986). Health as value: Methodological and theoreticalconsiderations. Health Psychology , 5,2 5–43. Lauder , W., Mummery, K., Jones, M., & Caperchione, C. (2006). A com-parison of health behaviours in lonely and non-lonely populations. Psychology, Health, & Medicine , 11, 233–245. Laurence , J-R, Beaulieu-Prevost, D., & du Chene, T. (2008). Measuring and understanding individual dif- ferences in hypnotizability. InM. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 225 –253). New York: Oxford University Press. Laurenceau , J-P., Feldman Barrett, L., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal pro-cess: The importance of self- disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness ininterpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1238 –1251. Lavee , Y., & Ben-Ari, A. (2004). Emotional expressiveness and neu-roticism: Do they predict marital quality? Journal of Family Psychology ,18, 620 –627. Lax, E. (1991). Woody Allen: A biography . New York: Knopf. Lazarus , R. (1968). Emotions and adaptation. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motiva- tion (pp. 175 –266). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Lazarus , R. S. (1974). Cognitive and coping processes in emotion. InB. Weiner (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 21 –32). New York: Academic Press. Lazarus , R. S. (2006). Emotions and interpersonal relationships: Toward a person-centered conceptualization of emotions and coping. Journal of Personality ,74,9–46. Lazarus , R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping . New York: Springer. Leak , G. K. (1974). Effects of hostility arousal and aggressive humor on catharsis and humor preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,30, 736 –740. Leary , M. R. (1983a). Social anxious- ness: The construct and itsmeasurement. Journal of Personality Assessment ,47,6 6 –75. Leary , M. R. (1983b). Understanding social anxiety: Social, personality, and clinical perspectives . Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Leary , M. R. (1986). The impact of in- teractional impediments on socialanxiety and self-presentation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology ,22, 122 –135. Leary , M. R., & Atherton, S. C. (1986). Self-efficacy, social anxiety, and in- hibition in interpersonal encoun- ters. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,4, 256 –267. Leary , M. R., Herbst, K. C., & McCrary, F. (2003). Finding plea-sure in solitary activities: Desire for aloneness or disinterest in social contact? Personality and Individual Differences ,35,5 9 –68. Leary , M. R., Knight, P. D., & Johnson, K. A. (1987). Social anxiety anddyadic conversation: A verbal re-sponse analysis. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,5,3 4 –50. Leary , M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1995). Social anxiety . New York: Guilford. Leary , M. R., & Meadows, S. (1991). Predictors, elicitors, and concomi-tants of social blushing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 60, 254–262. Ledley , D. R., & Heimberg, R. G. (2006). Cognitive vulnerability tosocial anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,25, 755 –778. Lee, K., Ogunfowora, B., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Personality traits be- yond the Big Five: Are they within the HEXACO space? Journal of Personality ,73, 1437 –1463. Lee, Y-T., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1997). Are Americans more optimisticthan the Chinese? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,23, 32–40. Leedham , B., Meyerowitz, B. E., Muirhead, J., & Frist, W. H. (1995). Positive expectations pre- dict health after heart transplant. Health Psychology ,14,7 4 –79. Lefcourt , H. M. (1982). Locus of con- trol: Current trends in theory and research (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lefcourt , H. M., Davidson, K., Prkachin, K. M., & Mills, D. E.(1997). Humor as a stress modera-tor in the prediction of blood pres- sure obtained during five stressfulREFERENCES 481
tasks. Journal of Research in Personality ,31, 523 –542. Lefkowitz , E. S., & Zeldow, P. B. (2006). Masculinity and femininity predict optimal mental health: Abelated test of the androgyny hypothesis. Journal of Personality Assessment ,87,9 5 –101. Lefkowitz , M. M., Eron, L. D., Walder, L. O., & Huesmann, L. R. (1977). G r o w i n gu pt ob ev i o l e n t :Al o n - gitu dinal study of the development of aggression . New York: Pergam on. Leichsenring , F. (2007). Psychodynamic psychotherapy: A systematic review of techniques, indications andempirical evidence. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice ,80, 217 –228. Leichsenring , F., & Rabung, S. (2008). Effectiveness of long-term psycho- dynamic psychotherapy: A meta- analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association ,300, 1551 –1565. Lenney , E. (1991). Sex roles: The mea- surement of masculinity, femininity, and androgyny. In J. P. Robinson,P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 573 –660). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Lent, R. W., & Lopez, F. G. (2002). Cognitive ties that bind: A tripartite view of efficacy beliefs in growth-promoting relationships. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,21, 256–286. Leon , G. R., Gillum, B., Gillum, R., & Gouze, M. (1979). Personality sta- bility and change over a 30-year period —middle age to old age. Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology ,47, 517 –524. Lepore , S. J. (1997). Expressive writing moderates the relation between in-trusive thoughts and depressivesymptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,73, 1030 –1037. Lerner , P., & Lerner, H. (1990). Rorschach measures of psychoana- lytic theories of defense. In J. N. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger(Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 8, pp. 121 –160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Leventhal , E. A., Hansell, S., Diefenbach, M., Leventhal, H., &Glass, D. C. (1996). Negative affect and self-report of physicalsymptoms: Two longitudinal stud- ies of older adults. Health Psychology ,15, 193 –199. Levin , I., & Stokes, J. P. (1986). An examination of the relation of in- dividual difference variables to loneliness. Journal of Personality , 54, 717–733. Levy, K. N., Blatt, S. J., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Attachment styles and pa-rental representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 407–419. Lewin , K. (1938). The conceptual re- presentation and measurement of psychological forces . Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Lewin , T. (2008, May 27). 2 colleges end entrance exam requirement. New York Times , p. A19. Lewinsohn , P. M., Joiner, T. E., & Rohde, P. (2001). Evaluation ofcognitive diathesis-stress models in predicting major depressive disor- der in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,110, 203–215. Lewis-Fernandez , R., & Kleinman, A. (1994). Culture, personality, andpsychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,103,6 7 –71. Lim, S-L., & Kim, J-H. (2005). Cognitive processing of emotional information in depression, panic, and somatoform disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,114, 50–61. Linde , J. A., Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A. S., & Jeffery, R. W. (2006). The impact of self-efficacy on behavior change and weight change amongoverweight participants in a weight loss trial. Health Psychology ,25, 282–291. Lindsay , J. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2000). From antecedent conditionsto violent actions: A general affec- tive aggression model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,26, 533–547. Lips, H. M. (2004). The gender gap in possible selves: Divergence of aca-demic self-views among high schooland university students. Sex Roles , 50, 357–371. Littig , L. W., & Yeracaris, C. A. (1965). Achievement motivation and inter-generational occupational mobility. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,1, 386 –389. Loehlin , J. C. (1992). Genes and the environment in personality devel- opment . Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Loehlin , J. C., McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1998). Heritabilities of com- mon and measure-specific compo- nents of the Big Five personalityfactors. Journal of Research in Personality ,32, 431 –453. Loehlin , J. C., Willerman, L., & Horn, J. M. (1982). Personality resem- blances between unwed mothersand their adopted-away offspring. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,42, 1089 –1099. Loehlin , J., Willerman, L., & Horn, J. M. (1987). Personality resem- blance in adoptive families: A 10-year follow-up. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 53, 961–969. Long , B. C., & Sangster, J. I. (1993). Dispositional optimism/pessimism and coping strategies: Predictors ofpsychosocial adjustment of rheu- matoid and osteoarthritis patients. Journal of Applied Social Psychology ,23, 1069 –1091. Long , C. R., & Averill, J. R. (2003). Solitude: An exploration of benefitsof being alone. Journal for the The- or y of Social Behaviour ,33,2 1 –44. Long , C. R., Seburn, M., Averill, J. R., & More, T. A. (2003). Solitudeexperiences: Varieties, settings, andindividual differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,29, 578–583. Lopez , S. J. (Ed.). (2009). The encyclo- pedia of positive psychology . New York: Wiley-Blackwell. Lord , C. G. (1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: Two modes ofprocessing social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,38, 257 –269. Lucas , R. E., & Baird, B. M. (2004). Extraversion and emotional reac- tivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,86, 473 –485. Lucas , R. E., & Diener, E. (2001). Understanding extraverts ’enjoy- ment of social situations: The im- portance of pleasantness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 81, 343–356. Lucas , R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross- cultural evidence for the funda- mental features of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,79, 452 –468. Lucas , R. E., Le, K., & Dyrenforth, P. S. (2008). Explaining the extraver- sion/positive affect relation: Sociability cannot account for482 REFERENCES
extraverts ’greater happiness. Journal of Personality ,76, 385–414. Luhtanen , R. K., & Crocker, J. (2005). Alcohol use in college students: Effects of level of self-esteem, narcissism, and contingencies of self-worth. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors ,19,9 9 –103. Lyness, S. A. (1993). Predictors of dif- ferences between Type A and B in-dividuals in heart rate and bloodpressure reactivity. Psychological Bulletin ,114, 266 –295. Lynn , R., & Martin, T. (1995). National differences for thirty-seven nationsin extraversion, neuroticism, psy-choticism and economic, demo- graphic and other correlates. Personality and Individual Differences ,19, 403 –406. Lynn , S. J., Fassler, O., & Knox, J. (2005). Hypnosis and the alteredstate debate: Something more ornothing more? Contemporary Hypnosis ,22,3 9 –45. Lynn , S. J., Kirsch, I., & Hallquist, M. N. (2008). Social cognitive the-ories of hypnosis. In M. R. Nash &A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford ha ndbook of hypnosis: Theory, re- search and practice (pp. 111 –13 9). New York: Oxford University Press. Lynn , S. J., Kirsch, I., Knox, J., Fassler, O., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Hypnosis and neuroscience: Implications for the altered statedebate. In G. A. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective . New York: Oxford University Press. Lynn , S. J., & Sherman, S. J. (2000). The clinical importance of sociocogni- tive models of hypnosis: Responseset theory and Milton Erickson ’s strategic interventions. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis ,42, 294–315. Lynn , S. J., Weekes, J. R., Neufeld, V., Zivney, O., Brentar, J., & Weiss, F. (1991). Interpersonal climate and hypnotizability level: Effects onhypnotic performance, rapport, and archaic involvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 60, 739–743. Lytton , H. (1977). Do parents create, or respond to, differences in twins? Developmental Psychology ,13, 456–459. MacDonald , D. A. (2000). Spirituality: Description, measurement, andrelation to the five factor model of personality. Journal of Personality , 68, 153–197. Mackie, M. (1983). The domestication of self: Gender comparisons of self-imagery and self-esteem. Social Psychology Quarterly ,46, 343–350. Maddux , J. E., Norton, L. W., & Leary, M. R. (1988). Cognitive compo- nents of social anxiety: An investi- gation of the integration ofself-presentation theory and self-efficacy theory. Journal of Social and Cl inical Psychology ,6,1 8 0 –190. Magnus , K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion and neuroticism as predictors of objec- tive life events: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,65, 1046 –1053. Mahalik , J. R., Cournoyer, R. J., DeFranc, W., Cherry, M., &Napolitano, J. M. (1998). Men ’s gender role conflict and use of psy-chological defenses. Journal of Counselin g Psychology ,45, 247– 255. Mahoney , M. J., & Arnkoff, D. B. (1979). Self-management. In O. F. Pomerleau & J. P. Brady (Eds.), Behavioral medicine: Theory and practice (pp. 75 –96). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Maier , S. F. (2001). Exposure to the stressor environment prevents the temporal dissipation of behavioral depression/learned helplessness. Biological Psychiatry ,49, 763 –773. Maier , S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (2005). Stressor controllability and learnedhelplessness: The roles of dorsalraphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews ,29, 829 –841. Major, B., Carnevale, P. J. D., & Deaux, K. (1981). A different perspectiveon androgyny: Evaluations of mas-culine and feminine personalitycharacteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 41, 988–1001. Mallon , S. D., Kingsley, D., Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1998). Methodological trends in Journal of Personality: 1970 –1995. Journal of Personality ,66, 671 –685. Mansfield , E. D., & McAdams, D. P. (1996). Generativity and themes of agency and communion in adultautobiography. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,22, 721–731.Marcus-Newhall , A., Pedersen, W. C., Carlson, M., & Miller, N. (2000). Displaced aggression is alive and well: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 670 –689. Markey , P. M., Markey, C. N., Tinsley, B. J., & Ericksen, A. J. (2002). A preliminary validation of preado-lescents ’self-reports using the five- factor model of personality. Journal of Research in Personality ,36, 173–181. Marks , G., Richardson, J. L., Graham, J. W., & Levine, A. (1986). Role of health locus of control beliefs and expectations of treatment efficacyin adjustment to cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 51, 443–450. Markus , H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,35,6 3 –78. Markus , H. (1983). Self-knowledge: An expanded view. Journal of Personality ,51, 543 –565. Markus , H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and moti- vation. Psychological Review ,98, 224–253. Markus , H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective:Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,20, 568 –579. Markus , H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of theself-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,51, 858–866. Markus , H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist ,41, 954 –969. Markus , H., & Sentis, K. (1982). The self and social information proces-sing. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 41–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Markus , H., & Smith, J. (1981). The influence of self-schemata on theperception of others. In N. Cantor& J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and socialinteraction (pp. 233 –262). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Marsh , H. W., Antill, J. K., & Cunningham, J. D. (1987). Masculinity, femininity, and an- drogyny: Relations to self-esteemand social desirability. Journal of Personality ,55, 661 –683.REFERENCES 483
Marsh , H. W., & Byrne, B. M. (1991). Differentiated additive androgyny model: Relations between mascu- linity, femininity, and multipledimensions of self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,61, 811 –828. Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. B., Vickers, R. R., Kusulas, J. W., & Hervig, L. K. (1994). The five- factor model of personality as a framework for personality-healthresearch. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,67, 278 –286. Martin , C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children ’s gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development ,66, 1453 –1471. Martin , R., & Watson, D. (1997). Style of anger expression and its relationto daily experience. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,23, 285–294. Martinez , J. C. (1994). Perceived control and feedback in judgment andmemory. Journal of Research in Personality ,28, 374 –381. Maslow , A. H. (1968). Toward a psy- chology of being (2nd ed.). New York: Van Nostrand. Maslow , A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row. Maslow , A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature . New York: Viking. Mathersul , D., Williams, L. M., Hopkinson, P. J., & Kemp, A. H. (2008). Investigating models of affect: Relationships among EEGalpha asymmetry, depression, andanxiety. Emotion ,8, 560 –572. Matthews , K. A., & Haynes, S. G. (1986). Type A behavior patternand coronary risk: Update andcritical evaluation. American Journal of Epidemiology ,123, 923–960. Matto , H. C. (2002). Investigating the validity of the Draw-A-Person: Screening procedure for emotional disturbance: A measurement vali-dation study with high-risk youth. Personality Assessment ,14, 221–225. Mayer , J. D. (2005). A tale of two visions. Can a new view of person-ality help integrate psychology? American Psychologist ,60, 294–307. Mayer , J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta- experience of mood. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology , 55, 102–111. Mayne , T. J., Norcross, J. C., & Sayette, M. A. (1994). Admission require- ments, acceptance rates, and finan-cial assistance in clinical psychology programs: Diversity across the practice-research continuum. American Psychologist ,49, 806–811. McAdams , D. P. (1993). Stories we live by: Personal myths and the making of the self . New York: Morrow. McAdams , D. P. (2004). Generativity and the narrative ecology of familylife. In M. W. Pratt & B. H. Fiese(Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and genera- tions . Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. McAdams , D. P., Bauer, J. J., Sakaeda, A. R., Anyidoho, N. A., Machado, M. A., Magrino-Failla, K., et al. (2006). Continuity and change in the life story: A longitudinal studyof autobiographical memories inemerging adulthood. Journal of Personality ,74, 1371 –1400. McAdams , D. P., Diamond, A., de St. Aubin, E., & Mansfield, E. (1997). Stories of commitment: The psy- chosocial construction of generative lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,72, 678 –694. McAdams , D. P., Hart, H. M., & Maruna, S. (1998). The anatomy ofgenerativity. In D. P. McAdams &E. de St. Aubin (Eds.), Generativity and adult development: How and why we care for the next generation (pp. 7– 43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McAdams , D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A new big five. Fundamental princi- ples for an integrative science ofpersonality. American Psychologist , 61, 204–217. McAdams , D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman, P. J.(2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and con-tamination in life narrative andtheir relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 474–485. McCarthy , E. D., Langner, T. S., Gersten, J. C., Eisenberg, J. G., & Orzeck, L. (1975). Violence andbehavior disorders. Journal of Communication ,25,7 1 –85.McCauley , C., Woods, K., Coolidge, C., & Kulick, W. (1983). More ag- gressive cartoons are funnier. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 817 –823. McClelland , D. C. (1961). The achiev- ing society . Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. McClelland , D. C. (1980). Motive dis- positions: The merits of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and so- ci al psychology (Vol. 1,p p .1 0 –41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. McClelland , D. C. (1985). How mo- tives, skill, and values determinewhat people do. American Psychologist ,40, 812 –825. McClelland , D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L.(1953). The achievement motive . New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts. McClelland , D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). Leadership motive patternand long-term success in manage- ment. Journal of Applied Psychology ,67, 737 –743. McClelland , D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult motives inpatterns of parent behavior in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 564–574. McClure , E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and theirdevelopment in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin ,126, 424 –453. McCrae , R. R. (2001). 5 years of progress: A reply to Block. Journal of Research in Personality ,35, 108–113. McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1983). Social desirability scales:More substance than style. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,51, 882 –888. McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping, and coping effectiveness in an adult sample. Journal of Personality ,54, 385–405. McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1995). Positive and negative valence withinthe five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality ,29, 443–460. McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as ahuman universal. American Psychologist ,52, 509 –516.484 REFERENCES
McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2008). The five-factor theory of personal- ity. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford. Mc Crae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Martin, T. A., Oryol, V. E., Rukavishnikov, A. A.,Senin, I. G., et al. (2004). Consensualvalidation of personality traits across cultures. Journal of Research in Per sonality ,38,1 7 9 –201. McCrae , R. R., Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Riemann, R., & Angleitner,A. (2001). Sources of structure: Genetic, environmental and arti- factual influences on the covaria-tion of personality traits. Journal of Personality ,69, 511 –535. McCrae , R. R., Terracciano, A., et al. (2005a). Universal features of per- sonality traits from observer ’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,88, 547 –561. McCrae , R. R., Terracciano, A., et al. (2005b). Personality profiles ofcultures: Aggregate personalitytraits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,89, 407 –425. McGhee , P. E. (1979). Humor: Its origin and development . San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. McGrath , M. J., & Cohen, D. B. (1978). REM sleep facilitation of adaptive waking behavior: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin , 85,2 4–57. McGue , M., & Christensen, K. (1997). Genetic and environmental contri-butions to depression symptom-atology: Evidence from Danish twins 75 years of age and older. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 106, 439–448. McGuire , M. T., & Troisi, A. (1990). Anger: An evolutionary view. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (Vol. 5, pp. 43 –57). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. McGuire , P. A. (1999, March). Therapists see new sense in use ofhumor. APA Monitor , pp. 1, 10. McGuire , W. J., & McGuire, C. V. (1982). Significant others in self-space: Sex differences and develop-mental trends in the social self. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological per- spectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 71–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McNally , R. J., & Geraerts, E., (2009). A new solution to the recoveredmemory debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science ,4, 126 –134. McPherson , M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Brashears, M. E. (2006). Social isolation in America: Changes incore discussion networks over two decades. American Sociological Review ,71, 353 –375. Measelle , J. R., John, O. P., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan,C. P. (2005). Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid self- reports on the Big Five dimensions?A longitudinal study from ages 5 to 7. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,89,9 0 –106. Mednick , M., & Thomas, V. (2008). Women and achievement. In F.L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (2nd ed., pp. 625–651). Westport, CT: Praeger. Meeker , W. B., & Barber, T. X. (1971). Toward an explanation of stagehypnosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,77,6 1 –70. Meece , J. L., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2006).Classroom goal structure, studentmotivation, and academic achieve- ment. Annual Review of Psychology ,57, 487 –503. Meichenbaum , D. H. (1985). Stress in- oculation training . New York: Pergamon. Meichenbaum , D. H., & Cameron, R. (1983). Stress inoculation training: Toward a general paradigm for training coping skills. In D. Meichenbaum & M. E. Jaemko(Eds.), Stress reduction and pre- vention (pp. 115 –157). New York: Plenum. Meichenbaum , D. H., & Deffenbacher, J. L. (1988). Stress inoculationtraining. Counseling Psychologist , 16,6 9–90. Meissner , W. W. (1984). Psychoanalysis and religious experience . New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Meleshko , K. G. A., & Alden, L. E. (1993). Anxiety and self-disclosure:Toward a motivational model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,64, 1000 –1009. Melges , F. T., & Weisz, A. E. (1971). The personal future and suicidal ideation. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease ,153, 244 –250. Mellman , T. A., David, D., Bustamante, V., Torres, J., & Fins, A. (2001).Dreams in the acute aftermath of trauma and their relationship toPTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress , 14, 241–247. Mershon , B., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1988). Number of factors in the personal- ity sphere: Does increase in factorsincrease predictability of real-life criteria? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,55, 675 –680. Metropolitan Area Child Study Research Group (2007). Changing the way children “think ”about aggression: Social-cognitive effects of a preventive intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,75, 160 –167. Meyer , G. J. (1997). Assessing reliabil- ity: Critical corrections for a critical examination of the Rorschach Comprehensive System. Psycholog- ical Assessment ,9, 480 –489. Meyer , G. J., & Shack, J. R. (1989). The structural convergence of mood andpersonality: Evidence for old and new directions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 57, 691–706. Michalski , R. L., & Shackelford, T. K. (2002). An attempted replication of the relationships between birthorder and personality. Journal of Research in Personality ,36, 182–188. Mickelson , K. D., Kessler, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attach- ment in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,73, 1092 –1106. Mikulincer , M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and pat-terns of self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 61, 321–331. Miles , D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental archi-tecture of human aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,72, 207 –217. Miller , I. W., & Norman, W. H. (1979). Learned helplessness in humans: A review and attribution theory model. Psychological Bulletin ,86, 93–118. Miller , M. G., & Ostlund, N. M. (2006). The effect of a parentingprime on sex differences in mate selection criteria. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 1459 –1468. Miller , N. E. (1941). The frustration- aggression hypothesis. Psychologi- cal Review ,48, 337 –346. Miller , N., Pedersen, W. C., Earleywine, M., & Pollock, V. E. (2003).REFERENCES 485
A theoretical model of triggered displaced aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,7, 75–97. Milling , L. S., Reardon, J. M., & Carosella, G. M. (2006). Mediation and moderation of psychological pain treatments: Response expec-tancies and hypnotic suggestibility. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,74, 253 –262. Mineka , S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory per- spective on the etiology of anxiety disorders. American Psychologist , 61,1 0–26. Mischel , W. (1968). Personality and assessment . New York: Wiley. Mischel , W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review ,80, 252 –283. Many Americans fed up with diet advice. (2001, January 2). New York Times , p. F10. Mischel , W. (1983). Alternatives in the pursuit of the predictability andconsistency of persons: Stable datathat yield unstable interpretations. Journal of Personality ,51, 578–604. Mischel , W. (1990). Personality dispo- sitions revisited and revised: A viewafter three decades. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 111 –134). New York: Guilford. Mischel , W. (2009). From Personality and Assessment (1968) to personality science 2009. Journal of Research in P ersonality ,43,2 8 2 –290. Mischel , W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing si-tuations, dispositions, dynamics,and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review , 102, 246–268. Mischel , W., & Shoda, Y. (2008). Toward a unified theory of per-sonality: Integrating dispositions and processing dynamics within thecognitive-affective processing sys-tem. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 208 –241). New York: Guilford. Mitchell , R. E., Cronkite, R. C., & Moos, R. H. (1983). Stress, coping, and depression among marriedcouples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,92, 433 –448.Mitte , K. (2005). Meta-analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatments for generalized anxiety disorder: A comparison with pharmacotherapy. Psychological Bulletin ,131, 785–795. Moehler , E., Kagan, J., Brunner, R., Wiebel, A., Kaufmann, C., & Resch, F. (2006). Association ofbehavioral inhibition with hair pigmentation in a European sam- ple. Biological Psychology ,72, 344–346. Moehler , E., Kagan, J., Oelkers-Ax, R., Brunner, R., Poustka, L., Haffner, J., et al. (2008). Infant predictors of behavioural inhibition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology ,26, 145 –150. Moffitt , T. E., Caspi, A., Milne, B. J., Melchior, M., Goldberg, D., & Poulton, R. (2007). Generalized anxiety disorder and depression: Childhood risk factors in a birthcohort followed to age 32. Psychological Medicine ,37, 441–452. Moilanen , D. L. (1993). Depressive information processing amongnonclinic, nonreferred college stu- dents. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,40, 340 –347. Moller , I., & Krahe, B. (2009). Exposure to violent video gamesand aggression in German adoles- cents: A longitudinal analysis. Aggressive Behavior ,35,7 5 –89. Moneta , G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the qualityof subjective experience. Journal of Personality ,64, 275 –310. Moretti, M. M., Segal, Z. V., McCann, C. D., Shaw, B. F., Miller, D. T., &Vella, D. (1996). Self-referent ver-sus other-referent information pro- cessing in dysphoric, clinically depressed, and remitted depressedsubjects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,22,6 8 –80. Morfei , M. Z., Hooker, K., Fiese, B. H., & Cordeiro, A. M. (2001).Continuity and change in parentingpossible selves: A longitudinal follow-up. Basic and Applied Social Psychology ,23, 217 –223. Morton , T. L. (1978). Intimacy and re- ciprocity of exchange: A compari-son of spouses and strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,36,7 2 –81. Mosher , C. E., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2006). Health effects of expressiveletter writing. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,25, 1122 – 1139. Mount , M. K., Barrick, M. R., & Strauss, J. P. (1994). Validity ofobserver ratings of the Big Five personality factors. Journal of Applied Psychology ,79, 272 –280. Moustakas, C. E. (1968). Individuality and encounter . Cambridge, MA: Doyle. Mundorf , N., Bhatia, A., Zillmann, D., Lester, P., & Robertson, S. (1988). Gender differences in humor ap- preciation. Humor ,1, 231 –243. Murayama , K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint influence of personal achievement goals and classroom goal structures on achievement- related outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology ,101, 432–447. Murray , B. (1998, June). Study says TV violence still seen as heroic, glam-orous. APA Monitor , p. 16. Murray , E. J., Lamnin, A. D., & Carver, C. S. (1989). Emotional expressionin written essays and psychother-apy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,8, 414 –429. Murray , H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A clinical and experi- mental study of fifty men of college age . New York: Oxford University Press. Murray , H. A. (1967). Henry A. Murray. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 5, pp. 285 –310). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Murray , J. A., & Terry, D. J. (1999). Parental reactions to infant death:The effects of resources and copingstrategies. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,18, 341 –369. Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness: Who is happy —and why . New York: Morrow. Nadon , R., Hoyt, I. P., Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991).Absorption and hypnotizability:Context effects reexamined. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,60, 144 –153. Nasby , W., & Read, N. W. (1997). The life voyage of a solo circumnaviga- tor: Integrating theoretical and methodological perspectives. Journal of Personality ,65, 785–1068. Nash , M. (1987). What, if anything, is regressed about hypnotic age486 REFERENCES
regression? A review of the empiri- cal literature. Psychological Bulletin ,102,4 2 –52. Neale , M. C., Rushton, P., & Fulker, D. W. (1986). Heritability of item responses on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences ,7, 771 –779. Neale , M. C., & Stevenson, J. (1989). Rater bias in the EASITemperament Scales: A twin study. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,56, 446 –455. Neimeyer , R. A. (2001). Repertory Grid Technique. In W. E. Craighead &C. B. Nemeroff (Eds.), The Corsini encyclopedia of psychology and behavioral science (3rd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 1394 –1395). New York: Wiley. Neisser , U., Boodoo, G., Bourchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., &Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence:Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist ,51,7 7 –101. Nelson , N. W., Sweet, J. J., & Demakis, G. J. (2006). Meta-analysis of the MMPI-2 Fake Bad Scale: Utility in forensic practice. Clinical Neuropsychologist ,20,3 9 –58. Nes, L. S., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2006). Dispositional optimism and coping:A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,10, 235–251. Nestoriuc , Y., Rief, W., & Martin, A. (2008). Meta-analysis of biofeed- back for tension-type headache: Efficacy, specificity, and treatmentmoderators. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology ,76, 379–396. Nevid , J. S., & Spencer, S. A. (1978). Multivariate and normative data pertaining to the RAS with the college population. Behavior Therapy ,9, 675. Nevo , O., & Nevo, B. (1983). What do you do when asked to answerhumorously? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 188–194. Ng, T. W. H., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control atwork: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior ,27, 1057 –1087. Niaura , R., Todaro, J. F., Stroud, L., Spiro, A., Ward, K. D., & Weiss, S.(2002). Hostility, the metabolic syndrome, and incident of coronaryheart disease. Health Psychology , 21, 588–593. Nicholson , I. A. M. (1997). To “Correlate Psychology and Social Ethics ”: Gordon Allport and the first course in American personalitypsychology. Journal of Personality , 65, 733–742. Nicholson , R. A., Mouton, G. J., Bagby, R. M., Buis, T., Peterson, S. A., &Buigas, R. A. (1997). Utility of MMPI-2 indicators of responsedistortion: Receiver operatingcharacteristic analysis. Psychologi- cal Assessment ,9, 471 –479. Nikles , C. D., Brecht, D. L., Klinger, E., & Bursell, A. L. (1998). The effects of current-concern- and nonconcern-related waking sugges- tions on nocturnal dream content. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,75, 242 –255. Niles , S. (1998). Achievement goals and means: A cultural comparison. Journal of Cross-CulturalPsychology ,29, 656 –667. Noguchi, K., Gohm, C. L., & Dalsky, D. J. (2006). Cognitive tendenciesof focusing on positive and negativeinformation. Journal of Research in Personality ,40, 891 –910. Nolen-Hoeksema , S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,109, 504 –511. Nolen-Hoeksema , S. (2001). Gender differences in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 10, 173–176. Noll, R. (1997). The Aryan Christ: The secret life of Carl Jung . New York: Random House. Noller , P. (1984). Nonverbal communi- cation and marital interaction . Oxford: Pergamon. Nordstrom , C. R., & Segrist, D. J. (2009). Predicting the likelihood of going to graduate school: The im-portance of locus of control. College St udent Journal ,43,2 0 0 –206. Norem , J. K. (2001). The positive power of negative thinking: Using defen- sive pessimism to harness anxiety and perform at your peak . Cambridge, MA: Basic Books. Norem , J. K. (2008). Defensive pessi- mism as a positive self-critical tool. InE. C. Chang (Ed.), Self-criticism and se lf-enhancement: Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 89 – 10 4). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Norem , J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986a). Anticipatory and post hoc cushion- ing strategies: Optimism and de- fensive pessimism in “risky ” situations. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,10, 347 –362. Norem , J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986b). Defensive pessimism: Harnessing anxiety as motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 51, 1208 –1217. Norem , J. K., & Illingworth, K. S. S. (1993). Strategy-dependent effectsof reflecting on self and tasks: Some implications of optimism and defen- sive pessimism. Journal of Personal- it y and Social Psychology ,65, 82 2–835. Norem , J. K., & Illingworth, K. S. S. (2004). Mood and performanceamong defensive pessimists and stra-tegic optimists. Journal of Research in Personality ,38,3 5 1 –366. Norman , P., & Bennett, P. (1996). Health locus of control. In M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting health behaviour: Research and practice within social cognition models (pp. 62 –94). Buckingham, England: Open University Press. Northey , W. F. (2002). Characteristics and clinical practices of marriageand family therapists: A national survey. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy ,28, 487 –494. Oakley , D. A. (2008). Hypnosis, trance and suggestion: Evidence fromneuroimaging. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, re- search and practice (pp. 365 –392). New York: Oxford University Press. O’Brien, M., Peyton, V., Mistry, R., Hruda, L., Jacobs, A., Caldera, Y., et al. (2000). Gender-role cognition in three-year-old boys and girls. Sex Roles ,42, 1007 –1025. Ochse , R., & Plug, C. (1986). Cross- cultural investigation of the validityof Erikson ’s theory of personality development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 50, 1240 –1252. O’Connor , B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (2001). Rigid and extreme: A geometricrepresentation of personality disor- ders in five-factor model space. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,81, 1119 –1130. O’Connor , S. C., & Rosenblood, L. K. (1996). Affiliation motivation inREFERENCES 487
everyday experience: A theoretical comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,70, 513–522. O’Hea, E. L., Grothe, K. B., Bodenlos, J. S., Boudreaux, E. D., White, M. A., & Brantley, P. J. (2005). Predicting medical regimen adher-ence: The interactions of healthlocus of control beliefs. Journal of Health Psychology ,10, 705–717. O’Heron , C. A., & Orlofsky, J. L. (1990). Stereotypic and nonstereo- typic sex role trait and behavior orientations, gender identity, andpsychological adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 134 –143. Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Goals, culture, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 1674 –1682. Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D-W., Kim-Prieto, C., & Choi, I. (2007). The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: When less is more. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 93, 685–698. Okazaki , S. (1997). Sources of ethnic differences between AsianAmerican and White Americancollege students on measures of depression and social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 106,5 2–60. Orgler ,H .( 1 9 6 3 ) . Alfred Adler: The man an d his work . New York: Liveright. Orlofsky , J. L., & O ’Heron, C. A. (1987). Stereotypic and nonstereo- typic sex role trait and behavior orientations: Implications for per- sonal adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 52, 1034 –1042. Otto, M. W., Teachman, B. A., Cohen, L. S., Soares, C. N., Vitonis, A. F.,& Harlow, B. L. (2007).Dysfunctional attitudes and epi- sodes of major depression: Predictive validity and temporalstability in never-depressed, de- pressed, and recovered women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 116, 475–483. Ouellette , J. A., Hessling, R., Gibbons, F. X., Reis-Bergan, M., & Gerrard, M. (2005). Using images to increase exercise behavior: Prototypes ver-sus possible selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,31, 610–620.Ovcharchyn , C. A., Johnson, H. H., & Petzel, T. P. (1981). Type A be- havior, academic aspirations, and academic success. Journal of Personality ,49, 248 –256. Overmier , J. B. (2002). On learned helplessness. Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science ,37,4–8. Overmier , J. B., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape andavoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology ,63,2 8 –33. Overskeid, G. (2007). Looking for Skinner and finding Freud. American Ps ychologist ,62,5 9 0 –595. Oyserman , D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006). Possible selves and aca-demic outcomes: How and whenpossible selves impel action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,91, 188 –204. Oyserman , D., & Markus, H. R. (1990). Possible selves and delinquency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,59, 112 –125. Oyserman , D., & Saltz, E. (1993). Competence, delinquency, and attempts to attain possible selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,65, 360 –374. Paik, H., & Comstock, G. (1994). The effects of television violence on an-tisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Communication Research ,21, 516–546. Palker-Corell , A., & Marcus, D. K. (2004). Partner abuse, learnedhelplessness, and trauma symp-toms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,23, 445 –462. Papsdorf , M., & Alden, L. (1998). Mediators of social rejection in social anxiety: Similarity, self- disclosure, and overt signs of anxi- ety. Journal of Research in Personality ,32, 351 –369. Park, C. L., & Adler, N. E. (2003). Coping style as a predictor of healthand well-being across the first yearof medical school. Health Psychology ,22, 627 –631. Park, L. E. (2007). Appearance-based rejection sensitivity: Implicationsfor mental and physical health,affect, and motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,33, 490–504. Park, L. E., Crocker, J., & Kiefer, A. K. (2007). Contingencies of self- worth, academic failure, and goalpursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,33, 1503 –1517. Parker , D. R., & Rogers, R. W. (1981). Observation and performance of aggression: Effects of multiple models and frustration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,7, 302–308. Parker , K. C. H., Hanson, R. K., & Hunsley, J. (1988). MMPI, Rorschach, and WAIS: A meta- analytic comparison of reliability,stability, and validity. Psychological Bul letin ,103, 367– 373. Parker , W. D. (1998). Birth order effects in the academically talented. Gifted Child Quarterly ,42,2 9 –38. Parton, D. A., & Geshuri, Y. (1971). Learning of aggression as a functionof presence of a human model, re-sponse intensity, and target of the response. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology ,20, 304 –318. Pasupathi , M., McLean, K. C., & Weeks, T. (2009). To tell or not totell: Disclosure and the narrative self. Journal of Personality ,77, 89–123. Patterson , D. R., & Jensen, M. P. (2003). Hypnosis and clinical pain. Psychological Bulletin ,129, 495–521. Paulhus , D. (1983). Sphere-specific measures of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,44, 1253 –1265. Paulhus , D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable re-sponding. Journal of Personality and So cial Psychology ,46,5 9 8 –609. Paulhus , D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. InJ. P. Robinson, P. S. Shaver, &L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psycholog- ical attitudes (Vol. 1,p p .1 7 –59). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Paulhus , D. L., Duncan, J. H., & Yik, M. S. M. (2002). Patterns of shyness in East-Asian and European-heritage students. Journal of Research in Pe rsonality ,36,4 4 2 –462. Paulhus , D. L., & Martin, C. L. (1987). The structure of personality capa-bilities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 354 –365. Paulhus , D. L., & Morgan, K. L. (1997). Perception of intelligence in lead-erless groups: The dynamic effectsof shyness and acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,72, 581 –591.488 REFERENCES
Paunonen , S. V. (1998). Hierarchical organization of personality and prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 538–556. Paunonen , S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big Five factors and facets and the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,81, 524 –539. Paunonen , S. V., & Jackson, D. N. (2000). What is beyond the BigFive? Plenty! Journal of Personality , 68, 821–835. Payne , S. C., Youngcourt, S. S., & Beaubein, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of the goalorientation nomological net. Journal of Applied Psychology ,92, 128–150. Peacock , E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1996). Anticipatory stress: The relation of locus of control, optimism, and control appraisals to coping. Journal of Research in Personality , 30, 204–222. Pedersen , D. M. (1999). Model for types of privacy by privacy functions. Journal of EnvironmentalPsychology ,19, 397 –405. Pedersen , N. L., Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., & Friberg, L. (1988).Neuroticism, extraversion, and re-lated traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,55, 950 –957. Pedersen , P. B. (2008). Ethics, compe- tence, and professional issues in cross-cultural counseling. InP. B. Pedersen, J. G. Draguns,W. J. Lonner, & J. E. Trimble (Eds.)., Counseling across cultures (6th ed., pp. 5 –20). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Pedersen , W. C., Bushman, B. J., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2008). Kicking the (barking) dog effect: The moderating role of targetattributes on triggered displaced ag- gression. Personality and Social Ps ychology Bulletin ,34,1 3 8 2 –1395. Pedhazur , E. J., & Tetenbaum, T. J. (1979). Bem Sex Role Inventory: Atheoretical and methodological cri- tique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,37, 996 –1016. Pennebaker , J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and disease. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 211 –244). New York: Academic Press.Pennebaker , J. W. (2000). The effects of traumatic disclosure on physical and mental health: The values of writing and talking about upsettingevents. In J. M. Violanti, D. Paton, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Posttraumatic stress intervention:Challenges, issues, and perspectives (pp. 97 –114). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Pennebaker , J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an understanding of inhi-bition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,95, 274–281. Pennebaker , J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the cop- ing process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 528–537. Pennebaker , J. W., & O ’Heeron, R. C. (1984). Confiding in others and illness rates among spouses of sui- cide and accidental-death victims. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 93, 473–476. Peplau, L. A., Russell, D., & Heim, M. (1979). The experience of loneli-ness. In I. Frieze, D. Bar-Tel, & J. Carroll (Eds.), New approaches to social problems (pp. 53 –78). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Perez-Edgar , K., Roberson-Nay, R., Hardin, M. G., Poeth, K., Guyer, A. E., Nelson, E. E., et al. (2007). Attention alters neural responses toevocative faces in behaviorally in- hibited adolescents. NeuroImage , 35, 1538 –1546. Perrig-Chiello , P., Perrig, W. G., & Staehelin, H. B. (1999). Healthcontrol beliefs in old age: Relationship with subjective andobjective health, and health behav-iour. Psychology, Health and Medicine ,4,8 3 –94. Peterson , C., & Bossio, L. M. (2001). Optimism and physical well-being. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 127 – 145). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Peterson , C., Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1993). Learned helpless- ness: A theory for the age of personal control . New York: Oxford University Press. Peterson , C., Seligman, M. E. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Pessimistic explanatory style is a risk factor for physical illness: A thirty-five-yearlongitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 55,2 3–27. Peterson , C., Seligman, M. E. P., Yurko, K. H., Martin, L. R., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Catastrophizing and untimely death. Psychological Science ,9, 127 –130. Peterson , C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky,G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,6, 287 –300. Peterson , C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An expanded Attributional StyleQuestionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,97,8 7 –89. Petrie , K. J., Booth, R. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1998). The immunologicaleffects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,75, 1264 –1272. Petrocelli , J. V., & Smith, E. R. (2005). Who I am, who we are, and why: Links between emotions and causal attributions for self- and group discrepancies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,31, 1628 –1642. Phares , E. J. (1976). Locus of control in personality . Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Phillips , A. G., & Silva, P. J. (2005). Self-awareness and the emotional consequences of self-discrepancies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,31, 703 –713. Phillips , S. D., & Bruch, M. A. (1988). Shyness and dysfunction in careerdevelopment. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,35, 159–165. Piccione , C., Hilgard, E. R., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1989). On thedegree of stability of measured hypnotizability over a 25-year pe- riod. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,56, 289 –295. Piedmont , R. L. (1999). Does spirituality represent the sixth factor of per-sonality? Spiritual transcendenceand the five-factor model. Journal of Personality ,67, 985 –1013. Piedmont , R. L., McCrae, R. R., Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A.(2000). On the invalidity of validityscales: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings in volunteer samples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 582 –593. Pilkonis , P. A. (1977a). Shyness, public and private, and its relationship toREFERENCES 489
other measures of social behavior. Journal of Personality ,45, 585–595. Pilkonis , P. A. (1977b). The behavioral consequences of shyness. Journal of Personality ,45, 596 –611. Pinderhughes , E. E., & Zigler, E. (1985). Cognitive and motivational deter- minants of children ’s humor responses. Journal of Research in Personality ,19, 185 –196. Pinquart , M., & Sorensen, S. (2001). Influences on loneliness in older adults: A meta-analysis. Basic and Applied Social Psychology ,23, 245–266. Piotrowski , C., & Keller, J. W. (1989). Psychological testing in outpatient mental health facilities: A national study. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice ,20, 423–425. Piotrowski , C., & Zalewski, C. (1993). Training in psychodiagnostic test-ing in APA-approved PsyD andPhD clinical psychology programs. Journal of Personality Assessment , 61, 394–405. Pistole , M. C. (1989). Attachment in adult romantic relationships: Style of conflict resolution and relation- ship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships ,6, 505–510. Pizzagalli , D. A., Sherwood, R. J., Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J.(2005). Frontal brain asymmetryand reward responsiveness. Psychological Science ,16, 805 –813. Plomin , R., & Caspi, A. (1999). Behavioral genetics and personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 251 – 276). New York: Guilford. Plomin , R., Chipuer, H. M., & Loehlin, J. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 225 –243). New York: Guilford. Plomin , R., & Crabbe, J. (2000). DNA. Psychological Bulletin ,126, 806–828. Plomin , R., Corley, R., Caspi, A., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. (1998).Adoption results for self-reportedpersonality: Evidence for nonaddi- tive genetic effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 75, 211–218. Plomin , R., & DeFries, J. C. (1998, May). Genetics of cognitive abilitiesand disabilities. Scientific American ,6 2–69. Polman , H., de Castro, B. O., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2008). Experimental study of the differen-tial effects of playing versus watching violent video games on children ’s aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior ,34, 256 –264. Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue:Environmental gender stereotypesin the first two years of life. Sex Roles ,22, 359 –367. Poropat , A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the Five-Factor Model of per-sonality and academic perfor-mance. Psychological Bulletin ,135, 322–338. Powch, I. G., & Houston, B. K. (1996). Hostility, anger-in, and cardiovas-cular reactivity in White women. Health Psychology ,15, 200 –208. Powers , S. I., Pietromonaco, P. R., Gunlicks, M., & Sayer, A. (2006). Dating couples ’attachment styles and patterns of cortisol reactivity and recovery in response to arelationship conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 90, 613–628. Pozo , C., Carver, C. S., Wellens, A. R., & Scheier, M. F. (1991). Socialanxiety and social perception: Construing others ’reactions to the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,17, 355 –362. Prager , K. J. (1986). Intimacy status: Its relationship to locus of control, self- disclosure, and anxiety in adults. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,12,9 1 –109. Pratt, M. W., Norris, J. E., Hebblethwaite, S., & Arnold, M. L.(2008). Intergenerational transmis-sion of values: Family generativity and adolescents ’narratives of parent and grandparent value teaching. Journal of Personality ,76,1 7 1 –198. Pressman, S. D., Cohen, S., Miller, G. E., Barkin, A., Rabin, B. S., & Treanor, J. J. (2005). Loneliness, social net-work size, and immune response toinfluenza vaccination in college freshmen. Health Psychology ,24, 297–306. Ptacek , J. T., Pierce, G. R., & Thompson, E. L. (2006). Findingevidence of dispositional coping. Journal of Research in Personality , 40, 1137 –1151. Ptacek , J. T., Smith, R. E., & Dodge, K. L. (1994). Gender differences incoping with stress: When stressor and appraisals do not differ. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,20, 421 –430. Puca, R. M., & Schmalt, H-D. (2001). The influence of the achievement motive on spontaneous thoughts in pre- and postdecisional action phases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,27, 302 –308. Quinlan, S. L., Jaccard, J., & Blanton, H. (2006). A decision theoretic andprototype conceptualization ofpossible selves: Implications for the prediction of risk behavior. Journal of Personality ,74, 599 –630. Raag, T., & Rackliff, C. L. (1998). Preschoolers ’awareness of social expectations of gender: Relationships to toy choices. Sex Roles ,38, 685 –700. Raikkonen , K., Matthews, K. A., Flory, J. D., & Owens, J. F. (1999). Effects of hostility on ambulatory bloodpressure and mood during dailyliving in healthy adults. Health Psychology ,18,4 4 –53. Raikkonen , K., & Matthews, K. A. (2008). Do dispositional pessimism and optimism predict ambulatory blood pressure during schooldays and nights in adolescents? Journal of Personality ,76, 605 –629. Rasmussen , H. N., Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2006). Self-regulation processes and health:The importance of optimism andgoal adjustment. Journal of Personality ,74, 1721 –1747. Rathus , S. A. (1973). A 30-item schedule for assessing assertive behavior. Behavior Therapy ,4, 398 –406. Rawsthorne, L. J., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Achievement goals and intrinsicmotivation: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,3, 326 –344. Raymond B. Cattell. (1997). American Psychologist ,52, 797 –799. Raz, A., Kirsch, I., Pollard, J., & Nitkin- Kaner, Y. (2006). Suggestion re- duces the Stroop effect. Psychological Science ,17,9 1 –95. Regalado , A. (2006, June 16). Scientist ’s study of brain genes sparks abacklash. Wall Street Journal , pp. A1, A12. Reich , J. W., & Zautra, A. J. (1997). Locus of control influences diathesis-stress effects in rheuma- toid arthritis patients. Journal of Research in Personality ,31, 423–438.490 REFERENCES
Reilly , R. R., & Chao, G. T. (1982). Validity and fairness of some alter- nativeemployeeselectionprocedures. Personnel Psychology ,35,1–62 . Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and intimacy: Component processes. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Ha ndbook of basic principles (pp. 523 –56 3). New York: Guilford. Reise , S. P., & Waller, N. G. (1993). Traitedness and the assessment of response pattern scalability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,65, 143 –151. Renaud , J. M., & McConnell, A. R. (2007). Wanting to be better but thinking you can ’t: Implicit theories of personality moderate the impact of self-discrepancies on self-esteem. Self and Identity ,6,4 1 –50. Rescorla , R. A. (1988). Pavlovian con- ditioning: It ’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist ,43, 151–160. Revelle , W., & Oehlberg, K. (2008). Integrating experimental andobservational personality research – the contributions of Hans Eysenck. Journal of Personality ,76, 1387 –1414. Reynolds , S. K., & Clark, L. A. (2001). Predicting dimensions of personal- ity disorder from domains and facets of the five-factor model. Journal of Personality ,69, 199–222. Reznick , J. S., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Gersten, M., Baak, K., & Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibitedand uninhibited children: A follow- up study. Child Development ,57, 660–680. Rhee , S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influ-ences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoptionstudies. Psychological Bulletin ,128, 490–529. Rhodewalt , F., & Comer, R. (1982). Coronary-prone behavior andreactance: The attractiveness of aneliminated choice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,8, 152–158. Rhodewalt , F., & Davison, J. (1983). Reactance and the coronary-prone behavior pattern: The role of self- attribution in responses to reducedbehavioral freedom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 44, 220–228.Rholes , W. S., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Grich, J. (2001). Adult at- tachment and transition to parent- hood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,81, 421 –435. Riemann , R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environ- mental influences on personality: Astudy of twins reared together usingthe self- and peer-report NEO-FFI Scales. Journal of Personality ,65, 449–475. Richards , J. C., Hof, A., & Alvarenga, M. (2000). Serum lipids and theirrelationships with hostility and angry affect and behaviors in men. Health Psychology ,19, 393 –398. Rimer , S. (2008, September 22). College panel calls for less focus on SATs. New York Times , p. A14. Rimm-Kaufman , S. E., & Kagan, J. (2005). Infant predictors of kinder- garten behavior: The contribution of inhibited and uninhibited temperament types. Behavioral Disorders ,30, 331 –347. Riordan, C. A., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1983). Attraction in aversiveenvironments: Some evidence forclassical conditioning and negative reinforcement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 44, 683–692. Riso, L. P., du Toit, P. L., Blandino, J. A., Penna, S., Dacey, S., Duin, J. S., et al. (2003). Cognitive aspects of chronic depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,112, 72–80. Ritts, V., & Patterson, M. L. (1996). Effects of social anxiety and actionidentification on impressions and thoughts in interaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,15, 191–205. Robbins , P. R., Tanck, R. H., & Houshi, F. (1985). Anxiety and dream sym- bolism. Journal of Personality ,53, 17–22. Roberts , B., & Hogan, R. (2001). Personality psychology in theworkplace . Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Roberts , B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality:The comparative validity of per- sonality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability forpredicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science ,2, 313 –345.Roberts , B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adult- hood. Current Directions in Psychological Science ,17,3 1 –35. Roberts , B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta- analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin ,132,1–25. Robins , R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2002). It ’s not just who you’re with, it ’s who you are: Personality and relationship ex- periences across multiple relation- ships. Journal of Personality ,70, 925–964. Robinson , B. S., Davis, K. L., & Meara, N. M. (2003). Motivational attri- butes of occupational possible selves for low-income rural women. Journal of Counseling Psychology , 50, 156–164. Robinson , M. S., & Alloy, L. B. (2003). Negative cognitive styles and stress-reactive rumination interact to predict depression: A prospective study. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,27, 275 –292. Robles , T. F., Brooks, K. P., & Pressman, S. D. (2009). Trait posi-tive affect buffers the effects ofacute stress on skin barrier recov-ery. Health Psychology ,28, 373–378. Roche , S. M., & McConkey, K. M. (1990). Absorption: Nature, as-sessment, and correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 59,9 1–101. Rodgers, J. L., Cleveland, H. H., van den Oord, E., & Rowe, D. C. (2000). Resolving the debate over birth order, family size, and intelli-gence. American Psychologist ,55, 599–612. Rodin , J., & Langer, E. J. (1977). Long- term effects of a control-relevantintervention with the institutional-ized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,35, 897–902. Roepke , S., & Renneberg, B. (2009). “Effectiveness of long-term psy- chodynamic psychotherapy: Ameta-analysis ”: Comment. Journal of the American Medical Association ,301, 931 –932. Rogers , C. R. (1947). The case of Mary Jane Tildon. In W. U. Snyder (Ed.), Casebook of nondirective counsel-ing (pp. 128 –203). Cambridge, MA: Houghton Mifflin.REFERENCES 491
Rogers , C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, impli- cations, and theory . Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Rogers , C. R. (1954). The case of Mrs. Oak: A research analysis. In C. R. Rogers & R. F. Dymond (Eds.), Psychotherapy and person- ality change (pp. 259 –348). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Rogers , C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist ’s view of psy- chotherapy . Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Rogers , C. R. (1967). Carl R. Rogers. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiog- raphy (Vol. 5, pp. 341 –384). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rogers , C. R. (1969). Freedom to learn: A view of what education might become . Columbus, OH: Merrill. Rogers , C. R. (1970). Carl Rogers on encounter groups . New York: Harper & Row. Rogers , C. R. (1977). Carl Rogers on personal power . New York: Delacorte. Rogers , C. R. (1982, August). Nuclear war: A personal response. APA Monitor , pp. 6 –7. Rogers , M. E., Hansen, N. B., Levy, B. R., Tate, D. C., & Sikkema, K. J. (2005). Optimism and coping with loss in bereaved HIV-infected men and women. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,24, 341 –360. Rogers , T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and theencoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,35, 677 –688. Rokach , A. (1998). The relation of cul- tural background to the causes of loneliness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,17,7 5 –88. Roney , C. J. R., & O ’Connor, M. C. (2008). The interplay betweenachievement goals and specific tar- get goals in determining perfor- mance. Journal of Research in Personality ,42, 482 –489. Rook, K. S., & Peplau, L. A. (1982). Perspectives on helping the lonely.In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman(Eds.), Loneliness (pp. 351 –378). New York: Wiley. Roos , P. E., & Cohen, L. H. (1987). Sex roles and social support as mod-erators of life stress adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 576 –585.Rose , R. J., Koskenvuo, M., Kaprio, J., Sarna, S., & Langinvainio, H. (1988). Shared genes, shared ex- periences, and similarity of person-ality: Data from 14,288 adultFinnish co-twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 161–171. Rosenberg , A., & Kagan, J. (1989). Physical and physiological corre- lates of behavioral inhibition. Developmental Psychobiology ,22, 753–770. Rosenman , R. H., Brand, R. J., Jenkins, C. D., Friedman, M., Straus, R., & Wurm, M. (1975). Coronary heart disease in the western collaborativegroup study: Final follow-up expe- rience of 8-1/2 years. Journal of the American Medical Association , 233, 872–877. Rosenthal , R. (1979). The “file drawer problem ”and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin ,86, 638–641. Rosenthal , R. (1990). How are we doing in soft psychology? American Psychologist ,45, 775 –777. Ross, M., & Holmberg, D. (1992). Are wives ’memories for events in rela- tionships more vivid than their husbands ’memories? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships , 9, 585–604. Rosse , J. G., Stecher, M. D., Miller, J. L., & Levin, R. A. (1998). The impact of response distortion on preem-ployment personality testing and hiring decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology ,83, 634 –644. Rotenberg , K. J. (1994). Loneliness and interpersonal trust. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,13, 152–173. Rotenberg , K. J. (1997). Loneliness and the perception of the exchange ofdisclosures. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,16, 259 –276. Roth , S. (1980). A revised model of learned helplessness in humans. Journal of Personality ,48, 103–133. Rothbart , M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science ,16, 207 –212. Rothbart , M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994). Temperament and the development of personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,103,5 5 –66. Rothbart , M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament andpersonality: Origins and outcomes.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 122 –135. Rotter , J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Rotter , J. B. (1966). Generalized expec- tancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs ,80(1). Rotter , J. B. (1982). The development and applications of social learning theory: Selected papers . New York: Praeger. Rotter , J. B., Chance, J. E., & Phares, E. J. (Eds.). (1972). Applications of a social learning theory of person- ality . New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Rowe , D. C. (1987). Resolving the person-situation debate: Invitation to an interdisciplinary dialogue. American Psychologist ,42, 218–227. Rubenstein , C. M., & Shaver, P. (1980). Loneliness in two northern cities. InJ. Hartog, J. R. Andy, & Y. A. Cohen (Eds.), The anatomy of loneliness (pp. 319 –337). New York: International Universities Press. Rubin , J. A., Provenzano, F. J., & Luria, Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder: Parents ’views of sex of newborns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry ,44, 512 –519. Rubins , J. L. (1978). Karen Horney: Gentle rebel of psychoanalysis . New York: Dial. Rubinstein , G., & Strul, S. (2007). The Five Factor Model (FFM) among four groups of male and femaleprofessionals. Journal of Research in Personality ,41, 931 –937. Rudasill , K. M., & Konold, T. R. (2008). Contribution of children ’s temperament to teacher ’s judg- ments of social competence fromkindergarten through second grade. Early Education and Development , 19, 643–666. Rudasill , K. M., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Justice, L. M., & Pence, K. (2006).Temperament and language skillsas predictors of teacher-child relationship quality in preschool. Early Education and Development , 17, 271–291. Russell , D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 39, 472–480.492 REFERENCES
Russell , J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin ,125,3–30. Rusting , C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three con- ceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin ,124, 165 –196. Rusting , C. L. (1999). Interactive effects of personality and mood on emotion-congruent memory andjudgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,77, 1073 –1086. Rusting , C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1998). Personality and cognitive proces-sing of affective information. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,24, 200 –213. Rutledge , T., Linke, S. E., Olson, M. B., Francis, J., Johnson, B. D., Bittner, V., et al. (2008). Social networks and incident stroke among women with suspected myocardial ische-mia. Psychosomatic Medicine ,70, 282–287. Ruvolo , A. P., & Markus, H. R. (1992). Possible selves and performance:The power of self-relevant imagery. Social Cognition ,10,9 5 –124. Sackett , P. R., Kuncel, N. R., Arneson, J. J., Cooper, S. R., & Waters, S. D. (2009). Does socioeconomic status explain the relationship between admissions tests and post-secondary academic performance? Psychological Bulletin ,135,1–22. Sadalla , E. K., Kenrick, D. T., & Vershure, B. (1987). Dominanceand heterosexual attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 730 –738. Sadler , W. A., & Johnson, T. B. (1980). From loneliness to anomie. In J. Hartog, J. R. Audy, & Y. A. Cohen (Eds.), The anatomy of loneliness (pp. 34 –64). New York: International Universities Press. Salgado , J. F. (1997). The five factor model of personality and job performance in the European community. Journal of Applied Psychology ,82,3 0 –43. Salili, F. (1994). Age, sex, and cultural differences in the meaning anddimensions of achievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,20, 635 –648. Sandstrom , M. J., & Cramer, P. (2003). Girls ’use of defense mechanisms following peer rejection. Journal of Personality ,71, 603 –627.Sanford , S., & Eder, D. (1984). Adolescent humor during peer interaction. Social Psychology Quarterly ,47, 235 –243. Sanna , L. J., Chang, E. C., Carter, S. E., & Small, E. M. (2006). The future is now: Prospective temporal self- appraisals among defensive pessi- mists and optimists. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 727–739. Sansone , C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1998). “Reality ”is complicated. American Psychologist ,53, 673–674. Saragovi , C., Koestner, R., Di Dio, L., & Aube, J. (1997). Agency, commu-nion, and well-being: Extending Helgeson ’s (1994) model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,73, 593 –609. Sarbin , T. R. (1950). Contributions to role-taking theory: I. Hypnotic behavior. Psychological Review , 57, 225–270. Sarbin , T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1972). Hypnosis: A social psychological analysis of influence communica- tion . New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Sarbin , T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1979). Hypnosis and psychopathology: Replacing old myths with freshmetaphors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,88, 506 –526. Sargent, R. T., Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2006). Contingencies of self- worth and depressive symptoms in college students. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,25, 628–646. Sarkar , U., Ali, S., & Whooley, M. A. (2009). Self-efficacy as a marker of cardiac function and predictor of heart failure hospitalization andmortality in patients with stable coronary heart disease: Findings from the Heart and Soul study. Health Psychology ,28, 166 –173. Saucier , G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1998). What is beyond the Big Five? Journal of Personality ,66, 495–524. Savickas , M. L. (1997). Constructivist career counseling: Models and methods. In G. J. Neimeyer & R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.), Advances in personal construct psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 149 –182). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Sayers, S. L., Baucom, D. H., & Tierney, A. M. (1993). Sex roles, interper- sonal control, and depression: Whocan get their way? Journal of Research in Personality ,27, 377–395. Scarr , S. (1969). Social introversion- extraversion as a heritable re- sponse. Child Development ,40, 823–832. Scarr , S., & Carter-Saltzman, L. (1979). Twin method: Defense of a criticalassumption. Behavior Genetics ,9, 527–542. Scarr , S., Webber, P. L., Weinberg, R. A., & Wittig, M. A. (1981).Personality resemblances among adolescents and their parents in biologically related and adoptivefamilies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,40, 885 –898. Scarr-Salapatek , S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science ,174, 1286 –1295. Scheier , M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology ,4, 219 –247. Scheier , M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (2001). Optimism, pessi-mism, and psychological well-being. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism:Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 189 –216). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Scheier , M. F., Matthews, K. A., Owens, J. F., Magovern, G. J., Lefebvre,R. C., Abbott, R. A., & Carver, C. S. (1989). Dispositional opti- mism and recovery from coronaryartery bypass surgery: The benefi- cial effects on physical and psycho- logical well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 57, 1024 –1040. Scheier , M. F., Weintraub, J. K., & Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with stress: Divergent strategies of opti- mists and pessimists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 51, 1257 –1264. Schimmack , U., & Diener, E. (1997). Affect intensity: Separating inten-sity and frequency in repeatedly measured affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 73, 1313 –1329. Schlenker , B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological Bulletin ,92,6 4 1 –669. Schlenker , B. R., Weigold, M. F., & Hallam, J. R. (1990). Self-servingREFERENCES 493
attributions in social context: Effects of self-esteem and social pressure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,58, 855 –863. Schmalt , H-D. (1999). Assessing the achievement motive using the grid technique. Journal of Research in Personality ,33, 109 –130. Schmidt , J. A., Shernoff, D. J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007).Individual and situational factors related to the experience of flow in adolescence: A multilevel approach.In A. D. Ong & M. H. M. van Dulmen (Eds.), Oxford handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 542 –558). New York: Oxford University Press. Schmidt , N., Gooding, R. Z., Noe, R. A., & Kirsch, M. (1984). Meta- analyses of validity studies pub-lished between 1964 and 1982 and the investigation of study charac- teristics. Personnel Psychology ,37, 407–422. Schmidt , N., & Sermat, V. (1983). Measuring loneliness in different relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 44, 1038 –1047. Schmukle , S. C., Egloff, B., & Burns, L. R. (2002). The relationship be- tween positive and negative affectin the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. Journal of Research in Personality ,36, 463 –475. Schulz , R., & Heckhausen, J. (1999). Aging, culture and control: Settinga new research agenda. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences ,54B, 139 –145. Schuyler , B. A., & Coe, W. C. (1981). A physiological investigation of voli- tional and nonvolitional experienceduring posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,40, 1160 –1169. Schwartz , C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1996). Early childhood temper- ament as a determinant of exter- nalizing behavior in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology ,8, 527 –537. Schwartz , C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxietyas an outcome of inhibited temper-ament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry ,38, 1008 –1015. Schwartz , C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhibitedinfants “grown up ”: Adult amyg- dalar response to novelty. Science , 300, 1952 –1953. Schwartz , R. D., & Higgins, R. L. (1979). Differential outcome from automated assertion training as a function of locus of control. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,47, 686 –694. Sears , R. R. (1941). Non-aggressive re- actions to frustration. Psychological Review ,48, 343 –346. Segal , D. L., Coolidge, F. L., & Mizuno, H. (2007). Defense mechanism dif-ferences between younger and older adults: A cross-sectional investiga- tion. Aging & Mental Health ,11, 415–422. Segal , D. L., & Murray, E. J. (1993). Emotional processing in cognitivetherapy and vocal expression offeeling. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,13, 189 –206. Segal , Z. V., Gemar, M., & Williams, S. (1999). Differential cognitive re- sponse to a mood challenge fol- lowing successful cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy for unipolardepression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,108,3–10. Segerstrom , S. C. (2007). Optimism and resources: Effects on each other andon health over 10 years. Journal of Research in Personality ,41, 772–786. Segrin , C. (1999). Social skills, stressful life events, and the development ofpsychosocial problems. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology , 18,1 4–34. Segrin , C., & Flora, J. (2000). Poor so- cial skills are a vulnerability factor in the development of psychosocial problems. Human Communication Research ,26, 489 –514. Segura , S. L., & Gonzalez-Roma, V. (2003). How do respondentsconstrue ambiguous responseformats of affect items? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 85, 956–968. Seidlitz , L., & Diener, E. (1998). Sex differences in the recall of affectiveexperiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,74, 262–271. Seligman , M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death . San Francisco: W. H.Freeman. Seligman , M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness and depression in ani- mals and men . Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.Seligman , M. E. P., & Hager, J. L. (Eds.). (1972). Biological bound- aries of learning . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Seligman , M. E. P., & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology ,74,1–9. Seligman , M. E. P., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a pre-dictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance sales agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,50, 832 –838. Seligman , M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positivepsychology progress: Empiricalvalidation of interventions. American Psychologist ,60, 410–421. Senko , C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2005). Achievement goals, task performance, and interest: Why perceived goal difficulty matters. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,31, 1739 –1753. Shadish , W. R., & Baldwin, S. A. (2005). Effects of behavioral mari-tal therapy: A meta-analysis ofrandomized controlled trials. Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology ,73,6–18. Shah , J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Regulatory concerns and appraisal efficiency: The general impact of promotion and prevention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,80, 693 –705. Shaw , J. S. (1982). Psychological an- drogyny and stressful life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,43, 145 –153. Shedler , J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). The illusion of mentalhealth. American Psychologist ,48, 1117 –1131. Sheldon , K. M. (1994). Emotionality differences between artists andscientists. Journal of Research in Personality ,28, 481 –491. Sheldon , W. H. (1942). The varieties of temperament: A psychology of constitutional differences . New York: Harper & Row. Shepperd , J. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1990). Shyness and self-presentation. InW. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives from social psychology (pp. 286 –314). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Shepperd , J. A., Maroto, J. J., & Pbert, L. A. (1996). Dispositional494 REFERENCES
optimism as a predictor of health changes among cardiac patients. Journal of Research in Personality , 30, 517–534. Shiffman , S., Balabanis, M. H., Paty, J. A., Engberg, J., Gwaltney, C. J., Liu, K. S., et al. (2000). Dynamic effects of self-efficacy on smokinglapse and relapse. Health Psychology ,19, 315 –323. Shiner , R. L. (1998). How shall we speak of children ’s personalities in middle childhood? A preliminarytaxonomy. Psychological Bulletin , 124, 308–332. Shoda , Y., Tiernan, S. L., & Mischel, W. (2002). Personality as a dynamicalsystem: Emergence of stability and distinctiveness from intra- and in- terpersonal interactions. Personality and Social PsychologyReview ,6, 316 –325. Showers , C. (1992). The motivational and emotional consequences ofconsidering positive and negativepossibilities for an upcoming event. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,63, 474 –484. Shurcliff , A. (1968). Judged humor, arousal, and the relief theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,4, 360 –363. Siegel , B. (2007, June 12). Stressful times for Chinese students. Time . Siegler , I. C. (1994). Hostility and risk: Demographic and lifestyle vari- ables. In A. W. Siegman &T. W. Smith (Eds.), Anger, hostility, and the heart (pp. 199 –214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Siegman , A. W. (1994). From Type A to hostility to anger: Reflections on the history of coronary-prone behavior. In A. W. Siegman & T. W. Smith(Eds.), Anger, hostility, and the heart (pp. 1– 21). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Siem, F. M. (1998). Metatraits and self-schemata: Same or different? Journal of Personality ,66, 783–803. Simms , L. J. (2007). The Big Seven model of personality and its rele-vance to personality pathology. Journal of Personality ,75,6 5 –94. Simoni , J. M., & Ng, M. T. (2002). Abuse, health locus of control, and perceived health among HIV-positive women. Health Psychology ,21,8 9 –93. Simpson , J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic rela-tionships. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology ,59, 971–980. Simpson , J. A., Collins, A., Tran, S., & Haydon, K. C. (2007). Attachment and the experience and expressionof emotion in romantic relation- ships: A developmental perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,92, 355 –367. Simpson , J. A., Ickes, W., & Grich, J. (1999). When accuracy hurts: Reactions of anxious-ambivalentdating partners to a relationshipthreatening situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 76, 754–769. Simpson , J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving within couples in an anxiety-provokingsituation: The role of attachmentstyles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,62, 434 –446. Simpson , J. A., Rholes, W. S., Orina, M. M., & Grich, J. (2002). Working models of attachment, support giving, and support seeking in a stressful situation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,28, 598–608. Simpson , T. L., & Arroyo, J. A. (1998). Coping patterns associated withalcohol-related negative conse-quences among college women. Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology ,17, 150 –166. Singer , J. A. (2004). Narrative identity and meaning making across the adult lifespan: An introduction. Journal of Pe rsonality ,72,4 3 7 –459. Singer , J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1981). Television, imagination, and ag-gression: A study of preschoolers . Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum. Sipley , C. G., Fischer, R., & Lui, J. H. (2005). Reliability and validity ofthe Revised Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR-R) self-reportmeasure of adult romantic attach-ment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,31, 1524 –1536. Skinner , B. F. (1967). B. F. Skinner. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiog- raphy (Vol. 5, pp. 387 –413). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Skinner , B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity . New York: Bantam. Skinner , B. F. (1974). About behavior- ism . New York: Vintage Books. Skinner , B. F. (1983). A matter of con- sequences . New York: Knopf.Skinner , E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for the structure of coping: A review and critique of category systems forclassifying ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin ,129, 216–269. Slater , J., & Depue, R. A. (1981). The contribution of environmental events and social support to serious suicide attempts in primary depres- sive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,90, 275 –285. Slavkin , M., & Stright, A. D. (2000). Gender role differences in collegestudents from one- and two-parentfamilies. Sex Roles ,42,2 3 –37. Slife, B., & Rychlak, J. F. (1982). Role of affective assessment in modelingaggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 43, 861–868. Sloan, W. W., & Solano, C. H. (1984). The conversational styles of lonelymales with strangers and room-mates. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,10, 293 –301. Sloman, L. (2008). A new comprehen- sive evolutionary model of depres- sion and anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders ,106, 219 –228. Slotter , E. B., & Finkel, E. J. (2009). The strange case of sustained dedicationto an unfulfilling relationship: Predicting commitment and breakup from attachment anxietyand need fulfillment within rela-tionships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,35,8 5 –100. Smillie , L. D., Pickering, A. D., & Jackson, C. J. (2006). The new reinforcement sensitivity theory: Implications for personality mea- surement. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,10, 320 –335. Smith, C. A., Wallston, K. A., & Dwyer, K. A. (1995). On babies and bathwater: Disease impact and negativeaffectivity in the self-reports ofpersons with rheumatoid arthritis. Health Psychology ,14,6 4 –73. Smith, C. E., Fernengel, K., Holcroft, C., Gerald, K., & Marien, M. (1994). Meta-analysis of the associations between social support and health outcomes. Annals of Behavioral Medicine ,16, 352 –362. Smith, D. (1982). Trends in counseling and psychotherapy. American Psychologist ,37, 802 –809. Smith, R. E. (1989). Effects of coping skills training on generalized self- efficacy and locus of control.REFERENCES 495
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,56, 228 –233. Smith , S. L., & Donnerstein, E. (1998). Harmful effects of exposure to me- dia violence: Learning of aggres-sion, emotional desensitization, and fear. In R. G. Geen & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression: Theories, research, and implications for social policy (pp. 167 –202). San Diego: Academic Press. Smith , T. W. (2006). Personality as risk and resilience in physical health. Current Directions in PsychologicalScience ,15, 227 –231. Smith , T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., & Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility, anger, aggressiveness, and coronary heart disease: An interpersonal perspective on personality, emo-tion, and health. Journal of Personality ,72, 1217 –1270. Smyth , J., True, N., & Souto, J. (2001). Effects of writing about traumatic experiences: The necessity for nar- rative structuring. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,20, 161–172. Snyder , C. R. (1988). From defenses to self-protection: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,6, 155 –158. Solano , C. H., Batten, P. G., & Parish, E. A. (1982). Loneliness and pat- terns of self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 43, 524–531. Solano , C. H., & Koester, N. H. (1989). Loneliness and communicationproblems: Subjective anxiety orobjective skills? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,15, 126–133. Soldz , S., & Vaillant, G. E. (1999). The Big Five personality traits and the life course: A 45-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality ,33, 208 –232. Solomon , Z., Mikulincer, M., & Avitzur, E. (1988). Coping, locus ofcontrol, social support, andcombat-related posttraumatic stressdisorder: A prospective study. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,55, 279 –285. Solomon , Z., Weisenberg, M., Schwarzwald, J., & Mikulincer, M. (1988). Combat stress reaction and posttraumatic stress disorder as de- terminants of perceived self-efficacyin battle. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,6, 356 –370.Spangler , W. D., & House, R. J. (1991). Presidential effectiveness and the leadership motive profile. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,60, 439 –455. Spanos , N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp. 324 –361). New York: Guilford. Spanos , N. P., & Hewitt, E. C. (1980). The hidden observer in hypnoticanalgesia: Discovery or experimen- tal creation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,39, 1201 –1214. Spanos , N. P., & Katsanis, J. (1989). Effects of instructional set on attri- butions of nonvolition during hyp- notic and nonhypnotic analgesia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,56, 182 –188. Spanos , N. P., Liddy, S. J., Baxter, C. E., & Burgess, C. A. (1994). Long-term and short-term stability of behav- ioral and subjective indexes of hypnotizability. Journal of Research in Personality ,28, 301–313. Spanos , N. P., Radtke, H. L., & Dubreuil, D. L. (1982). Episodicand semantic memory in posthyp-notic amnesia: A reevaluation. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology ,43, 565 –573. Spector , P. E. (1982). Behavior in organizations as a function of employee ’s locus of control. Psychological Bulletin ,91,4 8 2 –497. Spector , P. E., Cooper, C. L., Sanchez, J. L., O’ Driscoll, M., Sparks, K., Bernin, P., et al. (2001). Do na- tional levels of individualism and internal locus of control relate towell-being: An ecological level international study. Journal of Organizational Behavior ,22, 815–832. Spence , J. T. (1993). Gender-related traits and gender ideology: Evidence for a multifactorial the- ory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,64, 624 –635. Spence , J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1983). Achievement-related mo-tives and behaviors. In J. T. Spence(Ed.), Achievement and achieve- ment motives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 7– 74). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Spence , J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal AttributesQuestionnaire: A measure of sex-role stereotypes and masculinity-femininity. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology ,4, 127 (Ms. No. 617). Spencer , S. M., & Norem, J. K. (1996). Reflection and distraction: Defensive pessimism, strategicoptimism, and performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,22, 354 –365. Spett, M. C. (1983). All psychologists are not members of Divisions 12 and 17. American Psychologist ,38, 498. Spiegel , D. (2005). Multileveling the playing field: Altering our state of consciousness to understand hyp- nosis. Contemporary Hypnosis ,22, 31–33. Sprecher, S., & Hendrick, S. S. (2004). Self-disclosure in intimate relation-ships: Associations with individual and relationship characteristicsover time. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,23, 857 –877. Sprecher, S., Sullivan, Q., & Hatfield, E. (1994). Mate selection preferences:Gender differences examined in anational sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 66, 1074 –1080. Srivastava , S., Angelo, K. M., & Vallereux, S. R. (2009). Extraversion and positive affect: A day reconstruction study of person-environment transactions. Journal of Research in Personality ,42, 1613 –1618. Staats , A. W. (1996). Behaviorism and personality: Psychological behav- iorism . New York: Springer. Stajkovic , A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-relatedperformance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin ,124, 240–261. Stake , J. E. (2000). When situations call for instrumentality andexpressive- ness: Resource appraisal, coping strategy choice, and adjustment. Sex Roles ,42, 865 –885. Stake , J. E., Huff, L., & Zand, D. (1995). Trait self-esteem, positiveand negative events, and event- specific shifts in self-evaluation and affect. Journal of Research in Personality ,29, 223 –241. Stake , J. E., & Nickens, S. D. (2005). Adolescent girls ’and boys ’science peer relationships and perceptionsof the possible self as scientist. Sex Roles ,52,1–11.496 REFERENCES
Stanton , A. L., Kirk, S. B., Cameron, C. L., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2000). Coping through emotional ap- proach: Scale construction andvalidation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,78, 1150 –1169. Steel, P., & Ones, D. S. (2002). Personality and happiness: Anational-level analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 83, 767–781. Steger , M. F., Kawabata, Y., Shimai, S., & Otake, K. (2008). The meaning- ful life in Japan and the United States: Levels and correlates ofmeaning in life. Journal of Research in Personality ,42, 660 –678. Stein, G. L., Kimiecik, J. C., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S. A. (1995).Psychological antecedents of flowin recreational sports. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,21, 125–135. Stelmack , R. M. (1990). Biological bases of extraversion: Psychophysiologi- cal evidence. Journal of Personality , 58, 293–311. Stelmack , R. M., & Pivik, R. T. (1996). Extraversion and the effects ofexercise on spinal motoneuronal excitability. Personality and Individual Differences ,21,6 9 –76. Stephenson , W. (1953). The study of behavior: Q-technique and its methodology . Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Steptoe , A., O’ Donnell, K., Marmot, M., & Wardle, J. (2008). Positive affect and psychosocial processes related to health. British Journal of Psychology ,99, 211 –227. Steptoe , A., & Wardle, J. (2001). Locus of control and health behaviourrevisited: A multivariate analysis ofyoung adults from 18 countries. British Journal of Psychology ,92, 659–672. Sternberg , R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Kidd, K. K. (2005). Intelligence, race, and genetics. American Psychologist ,60,4 6 –59. Stewart , A. J. (1982). Motivation and society . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Stoolmiller , M. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family en- vironments for estimates of herita-bility and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption stud- ies. Psychological Bulletin ,125, 392–409. Storr , A. (1988). Solitude: A return to the self . New York: Free Press.Story , A. L. (1998). Self-esteem and memory for favorable and unfa- vorable personality feedback. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,24,5 1 –64. Strahan , E. J., Lafrance, A., Wilson, A. E., Ethier, N., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. (2008). Victoria ’s dirty secret: How sociocultural normsinfluence adolescent girls and women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,34, 288 –301. Strauman , T. J. (1996). Stability within the self: A longitudinal study of thestructural implications of self- discrepancy theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 71, 1142 –1153. Strelau , J. (1987). Emotion as a key concept in temperament research. Journal of Research in Personality , 21, 510–528. Strentz , T., & Auerbach, S. M. (1988). Adjustment to the stress of simu- lated captivity: Effects of emotion-focused versus problem-focused preparation on hostages differing in locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 55, 652–660. Strickland , B. R. (1978). Internal- external expectancies and health-related behaviors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,46, 1192 –1211. Strickland , B. R. (1989). Internal- external control expectancies: From contingency to creativity. American Psychologist ,44,1–12. Stright , A. D., Gallagher, K. C., & Kelley, K. (2008). Infant tempera-ment moderates relations between maternal parenting in early child- hood and children ’s adjustment in first grade. Child Development ,79, 186–200. Strough , J., Leszczynski, J. P., Neely, T. L., Flinn, J. A., & Margrett, J.(2007). From adolescence to lateradulthood: Femininity, masculinity, and androgyny in six age groups. Sex Roles ,57, 385 –396. Strube , M. J. (1982). Time urgency and Type A behavior: A methodologicalnote. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,8, 563 –565. Strube , M. J., Berry, J. M., & Moergen, S. (1985).Relinquishment of control and the Type A behavior pattern: The role of performance evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,49, 831 –842.Sturmer , T., & Hasselbach, P. (2006). Personality, lifestyle, and risk of cardiovascular disease and cancer: Follow-up of population basedcohort. British Medical Journal , 332, 1359. Suedfeld , P. (1980). Restricted environ- mental stimulation: Research and clinical applications . New York: Wiley. Suedfeld , P. (1982). Aloneness as a healing experience. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 54 –67). New York: Wiley. Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versusnorms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,74, 482 –493. Suinn , R. M. (2001). The terrible twos — anger and anxiety: Hazardous toyour health. American Psychologist ,56,2 7 –36. Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy of avoidant andnonavoidant coping strategies: Ameta-analysis. Health Psychology , 4, 249–288. Surtees , P. G., Wainwright, N. W. J., Luben, R., Khaw, K-T., & Day,N. E. (2006). Mastery, sense of coherence, and mortality: Evidence of independent associations fromthe EPIC-Norfolk prospectivecohort study. Health Psychology , 25, 102–110. Suvak , M. K., Vogt, D. S., Savarese, V. W., King, L. A., & King, D. W.(2002). Relationship of war-zone coping strategies to long-term general life adjustment among Vietnam ve-terans: Combat exposure as a mod-eratorvariable. PersonalityandSocial Ps ychology Bulletin ,28,9 7 4 –985. Swann , W. B., & Seyle, C. (2005). Personality psychology ’s comeback and its emerging symbiosis with social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,31, 155–165. Swickert , R. J., & Gilliland, K. (1998). Relationship between the brainstem auditory evoked response and extraversion, impulsivity, andsociability. Journal of Research in Personality ,32, 314 –330. Tafarodi , R. W., Marshall, T. C., & Katsura, H. (2004). Standing out in Canada and Japan. Journal of Personality ,72, 785 –814.REFERENCES 497
Tafarodi , R. W., & Vu, C. (1997). Two-dimensional self-esteem and reactions to success and failure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,23, 626 –635. Tamir , M. (2009). Differential prefer- ences for happiness: Extraversionand trait-consistent emotion regu-lation. Journal of Personality ,77, 447–470. Tamres , L. K., Janicki, D., & Helgeson, V. S. (2002). Sex differences incoping behavior: A meta-analyticreview and an examination of rela- tive coping. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,6,2–30. Tarabulsky , G. M., Provost, M. A., Larose, S., Moss, E., Lemelin, J-P,Moran, G., et al. (2008). Similarities and differences in mothers ’and observers’ ratings of infant security on the Attachment Q-Sort. Infant Behavior & Development ,31,1 0 –22. Tarlow , E. M., & Haaga, D. A. F. (1996). Negative self-concept:Specificity to depressive symptoms and relation to positive and nega- tive affectivity. Journal of Research in Personality ,30, 120 –127. Taubes , G. (1998, May –June). Telling time by the second hand. Technology Review ,101, 76. Taylor , D. A., & Belgrave, F. Z. (1986). The effects of perceived intimacyand valence on self-disclosure reci-procity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,12, 247 –255. Taylor , M. C., & Hall, J. A. (1982). Psychological androgyny: Theories,methods and conclusions. Psycho- logical Bulletin ,92, 347 –366. Taylor , S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the healthy mind . New York: Basic Books. Tellegen , A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ( “absorption ”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,83, 268 –277. Terracciano , A., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personalityplasticity after age 30. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 999–1009. Terracciano , A., McCrae, R. R., Hagemann, D., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Individual difference vari- ables, affective differentiation, andthe structure of affect. Journal of Personality ,71, 669 –703.Terry , D. J., & Hynes, G. J. (1998). Adjustment to a low-control situa- tion: Reexamining the role of coping responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1078 –1092. Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as predictors of job performance: Ameta-analytic review. Personnel Psychology ,44, 703 –739. Thibodeau , R., Jorgensen, R. S., & Kim, S. (2006). Depression, anxiety, and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,115, 715–729. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development . New York: Brunner/Mazel. Thorndike , E. L. (1911). Animal intelli- gence: Experimental studies . New York: Macmillan. Thrash , T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and self-attributed achieve- ment motives: Concordance and predictive validity. Journal of Personality ,70, 729 –755. Thrash , T. M., Elliot, A. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2007).Methodological and dispositional predictors of congruence between implicit and explicit need forAchievement. Personality and Social Ps ychology Bulletin ,33,9 6 1 –974. Tidwell , M-C. O., Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. R. (1996). Attachment, attractiveness, and social interac- tion: A diary study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 71, 729–745. Timmers , M., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Gender differences in motives for regulating emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,24, 974 –985. Todorov , A., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Automatic sources of aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior , 7,5 3–68. Torges , C. M., Stewart, A. J., & Duncan, L. E. (2009). Appreciatinglife’s complexities: Assessing narra- tive ego integrity in late midlife. Journal of Research in Personality , 43,6 6–74. Tracy , J. L., Robins, R. W., & Sherman, J. W. (2009). The practice of psy- chological science: Searching for Cronbach ’s two streams in social- personality psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 96, 1206 –1225.Triandis , H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review ,96, 506–520. Triandis , H. C. (2001). Individualism- collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality ,69, 907–924. Tripathi , R., & Cervone, D. (2008). Cultural variations in achievement motivation despite equivalent mo- tivational strength: Motivationalconcerns among Indian andAmerican corporate professionals. Journal of Research in Personality , 42, 456–464. Trivers , R. L. (1972). Parental invest- ment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man: 1871 –1971 (pp. 136 –179). Chicago: Aldine. Trull , T. J., Widiger, T. A., & Burr, R. (2001). A structured interview for the assessment of the five-factormodel of personality: Facet-levelrelations to the Axis II personality disorder. Journal of Personality ,69, 175–198. Tucker , J. S., & Anders, S. L. (1999). Attachment style, interpersonalperception accuracy, and relation- ship satisfaction in dating couples. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin ,15, 403 –412. Tuerlinckx , F., De Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). Measuring needs withthe Thematic Apperception Test:A psychometric study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 82, 448–461. Twenge , J. M. (2000). The age of anxiety? Birth cohort change in anxiety and neuroticism, 1952 – 1993. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,79, 1007 –1021. Uchino , B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The relationship between social supportand physiological processes: Areview with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin ,119, 488–531. Ungerer , J. A., Waters, B., & Barnett, B. (1997). Defense style and adjust- ment in interpersonal relationships. Journal of Research in Personality , 31, 375–384. Urbina , S. P., & Grey, A. (1975). Cultural and sex differences in the sex distribution of dream charac-ters. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology ,6, 358 –364.498 REFERENCES
Vaidya, J. G., Gray, E. K., Haig, J., & Watson, D. (2002). On the tempo- ral stability of personality: Evidence for differential stability and the roleof life experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 83, 1469 –1484. Vaillant , G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life . Boston: Little, Brown. Vaillant , G. E. (1992). Ego mechanisms of defense: A guide for clinicians and researchers . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Valentiner , D. P., Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., & Gershuny, B. S. (1996). Coping strategies and posttrau- matic stress disorder in femalevictims of sexual and nonsexual assault. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,105, 455 –458. Valli, K., Revonsuo, A., Palkas, O., & Punamaki, R-L. (2006). The effectof trauma on dream content— A field study of Palestinian children. Dreaming ,16,6 3 –87. van Ijendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M.(2004). Assessing attachmentsecurity with the Attachment Q-Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observerAQS. Child Development ,75, 1188 –1213. Van Yperen , N. W. (2006). A novel approach to assessing achievementgoals in the context of the 2 X 2framework: Identifying distinct profiles and individuals with dif- ferent dominant achievement goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 1432 –1445. Vasquez , E. A., Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., Stenstrom, D. M., & Miller, N. (2005). The moderating effect oftrigger intensity on triggered dis- placed aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 41,6 1–67. Vaughn , L. A., & Weary, G. (2002). Roles of the availability of expla- nations, feelings of ease, and dysphoria in judgments about thefuture. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology ,21, 686 –704. Veroff , J., Depner, C., Kulka, R., & Douvan, E. (1980). Comparison of American motives: 1957 versus 1976. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,39, 1249 –1262. Verona , E., & Sullivan, E. A. (2008). Emotional catharsis and aggressionrevisited: Heart rate reduction following aggressive responding. Emotion ,8, 331 –340. Viglione , D. J. (1999). A review of recent research addressing the utility of the Rorschach. Psychological Assessment ,11, 251 –265. Viglione , D. J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2001). The Rorschach: Facts,fictions, and future. Psychological Assessment ,13, 452 –471. Vignoles , V. L., Manzi, C., Regalia, C., Jemmolo, S., & Scabini, E. (2008).Identity motives underlying desired and feared possible future selves. Journal of Personality ,76, 1165 –1200. Vitaliano , P. P., DeWolfe, D. J., Maiuro, R. D., Russo, J., & Katon, W. (1990). Appraised changeability of a stressor as a modifier ofthe relationship between coping and depression: A test of the hypothesis of fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 59, 582–592. Vitkus, J., & Horowitz, L. M. (1987). Poor social performance of lonely people: Lacking a skill or adoptinga role? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,52, 1266 –1273. Vittengl , J. R., Clark, L. A., Dunn, T. W., & Jarrett, R. B. (2007). Reducing relapse and recurrence in unipolar depression: A comparativemeta-analysis of cognitive-behavioral therapy ’s effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology ,75, 475 –488. Vo, K., & Ostrov, B. F. (2006, February 19). Women seek reliable health advice in vain: Studies keep con- tradicting the conventional wisdom. San Jose Mercury News , pp. 1A, 15A. Todorov , A., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Automatic sources of aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior , 7,5 3–68. Voelz , C. J. (1985). Effects of gender role disparity on couples ’decision- making processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 49, 1532 –1540. Vogel , D. A., Lake, M. A., Evans, S., & Karraker, K. H. (1991). Children ’s and adults ’sex-stereotyped percep- tions of infants. Sex Roles ,24, 605–616. Vogel , G. W. (1975). Review of REM sleep deprivation. Archives of General Psychiatry ,32, 749 –761.Wacker , J., Chavanon, M., Leue, A., & Stemmler, G. (2008). Is running away right? The behavioral activation-behavioral inhibitionmodel of anterior asymmetry. Emotion ,8, 232 –249. Wallston , K. A. (1992). Hocus-pocus, the focus isn ’t strictly on locus: Rotter ’s social learning theory modified for health. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,16, 183–199. Wallston , K. A. (Ed.). (2005). Research with the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control (MHLC) Scales [Special issue]. Journal of Health Psychology ,10(5). Wallston , K. A., Maides, S., & Wallston, B. S. (1976). Health-related information seeking as afunction of health-related locus ofcontrol and health value. Journal of Research in Personality ,10, 215–222. Wallston , K. A., & Smith, M. S. (1994). Issues of control and health: The action is in the interaction. In G. N. Penney, P. Bennett, &M. Herbert (Eds.), Health psychol- ogy: A lifespan perspective (pp. 153 –168). London, England: Harwood. Ward , C. H., & Eisler, R. M. (1987). Type A behavior, achievement striving, and a dysfunctional self- evaluation system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 53, 318–326. Watkins , C. E., Campbell, V. L., Nieberding, R., & Hallmark, R.(1995). Contemporary practice ofpsychological assessment by clinical psychologists. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice , 26,5 4–60. Watson , D. (1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: Their relation to healthcomplaints, perceived stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 1020 –1030. Watson , D., & Clark, L. A. (1991). Self- versus peer-ratings of specific emotional traits: Evidence of con- vergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,60, 927 –940. Watson , D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders.REFERENCES 499
Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 97, 346–353. Watson , D., Clark, L. A., McIntyre, C. W., & Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and socialactivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,63, 1011 –1025. Watson , D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and valida- tion of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANASScales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,54, 1063 –1070. Watson , D., Hubbard, B., & Wiese, D. (2000). Self-other agreement inpersonality and affectivity: The roleof acquaintanceship, trait visibility, and assumed similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 78, 546–558. Watson , D., & Naragon, K. (2009). Positive affectivity: The disposition to experience positive emotional states. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder(Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive ps ychology (2nd ed., pp. 207 –21 5). New York: Oxford University Press. Watson , D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review ,96, 234–254. Watson , D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure ofmood. Psychological Bulletin ,98, 219–235. Watson , J. B. (1936). John Broadus Watson. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiog- raphy (Vol. 3, pp. 271 –281). Worcester, MA: Clark UniversityPress. Watson , J. B. (1924/1970). Behaviorism . New York: Norton. Watson , J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology ,3,1–14. Webb , S. D. (1978). Privacy and psy- chosomatic stress: An empiricalanalysis. Social Behavior and Personality ,6, 227 –234. Weeks , D. G., Michela, J. L., Peplau, L. A., & Bragg, M. E. (1980).Relation between loneliness anddepression: A structural equation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,39, 1238 –1244. Wegner , D. M., Wenzlaff, R. M., & Kozak, M. (2004). Dream rebound:The return of suppressed thoughtsin dreams. Psychological Science , 15, 232–236. Wei, M., Russell, D. W., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, social self-efficacy, self-disclosure,loneliness, and subsequent depres- sion for freshman college students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology ,52, 602–614. Weiner , B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivationand emotion. Psychological Bulletin ,92, 548 –573. Weiner , B. (1990). Attribution in personality psychology. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 465 –485). New York: Guilford. Weiner , B. (2006). Social motivation, justice, and the moral emotions: An attributional approach . Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Weiner , I. B. (1995). Methodological considerations in Rorschach re- search. Psychological Assessment , 7, 330–337. Weiner , I. B. (1996). Some observations on the validity of the Rorschachinkblot method. Psychological Assessment ,8, 206 –213. Weiner , I. B. (2001). Advancing the sci- ence of psychological assessment:The Rorschach inkblot method as exemplar. Psychological Assessment ,13, 423 –432. Weinstein , S. E., & Quigley, K. S. (2006). Locus of control predicts appraisals and cardiovascularreactivity to a novel active copingtask. Journal of Personality ,74, 911–931. Wenzel , A., & Emerson, T. (2009). Mate selection in socially anxiousand nonanxious individuals. Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology ,28, 341 –363. Westen , D. (1996). A model and a method for uncovering the nomo- thetic from the idiographic: An al- ternative to the five-factor model? Journal of Research in Personality , 30, 400–413. Westen , D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psy-chological science. Psychological Bulletin ,124, 333 –371. Wheeler , R. E., Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1993). Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: A biological substrate ofaffective style. Psychophysiology , 30,8 2–89. Whiting , R. (1989). You gotta have Wa . New York: Vintage. Whitley , B. E. (1983). Sex-role orienta- tion and self-esteem: A criticalmeta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 44, 765–778. Whitley , D. (2002, March 31). Former major leaguer confronts painful past. San Jose Mercury News , p. 16D. Whyte , L. L. (1978). The unconscious before Freud . New York: St. Martin ’s. Wichman , A. L., Rodgers, J. L., & MacCallum, R. C. (2006). A mul- tilevel approach to the relationship between birth order and intelli-gence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,32, 117 –127. Wicker , F. W., Barron, W. L., & Willis, A. C. (1980). Disparagementhumor: Dispositions and resolu-tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,39, 701 –709. Widiger , T. A. (2005). Five factor model of personality disorder: Integrating science and practice. Journal of Research in Personality ,39, 67–83. Wilkowski , B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2008). The cognitive basis of trait anger and reactive aggression: An integrative analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,12, 3–21. Williams , J. E., Nieto, F. J., Sanford, C. P., Couper, D. J., & Tyroler,H. A. (2002). The association be-tween trait anger and incident stroke risk: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Stroke ,33,1 3 –20. Williams , J. E., Nieto, F. J., Sanford, C. P., & Tyroler, H. A. (2001).Effects of an angry temperament oncoronary heart disease risk: TheAtherosclerosis Risk in Communities study. American Journal of Epidemiology ,154, 230–235. Williams , J. E., Paton, C. C., Seigler, I. C., Eigenbrodt, M. L., Nieto, F. J., & Tyroler, H. A. (2000). Anger proneness predicts coronaryheart disease risk: Prospective analysis from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Circulation ,101, 2034 –2039. Williams , J. G., & Solano, C. H. (1983). The social reality of feeling lonely:500 REFERENCES
Friendship and reciprocation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,9, 237 –242. Williams , S. L. (1995). Self-efficacy and anxiety and phobic disorders. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), Self-efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment: Theory, research, and application (pp. 69 –108). New York: Plenum. Williams , P. G., Colder, C. R., Lane, J. D., McCaskill, C. C., Feinglos, M. N., & Surwit, R. S. (2002).Examination of the neuroticism-symptom reporting relationship in individuals with Type 2 diabetes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,28, 1015 –1025. Williams-Piehota , P., Schneider, T. R., Pizarro, J., Mowad, L., & Salovey,P. (2004). Matching health mes-sages to health locus of controlbeliefs for promoting mammogra- phy utilization. Psychology and Health ,19, 407 –423. Wilson , T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1982). Improving the academic perfor-mance of college freshmen: Attribution therapy revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,42, 367 –376. Wilson , T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1985). Improving the performance ofcollege freshmen with attributionaltechniques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,49, 287–293. Windle , M., & Windle, R. C. (1996). Coping strategies, drinking motives, and stressful life events among middle adolescents:Associations with emotional andbehavioral problems and with academic functioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,105, 551–560. Winter , D. A. (2003). Repertory grid technique as a psychotherapy re- search measure. Psychotherapy Research ,13,2 5 –42. Wittenberg , M. T., & Reis, H. T. (1986). Loneliness, social skills, and social perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,12, 121–130. Woike , B. A. (2008). A functional framework for the influence of im-plicit and explicit motives on auto-biographical memory. Personality and Social Psychology Review ,12, 99–117. Wolfe , R. N., & Kasmer, J. A. (1988). Type versus trait: Extraversion, impulsivity, sociability, andpreferences for cooperative and competitive activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 864–871. Won-Doornink , M. J. (1985). Self- disclosure and reciprocity in con- versation: A cross-national study. Social Psychology Quarterly ,48, 97–107. Wong , M. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Motivation and academic achievement: The effects of person- ality traits and the quality ofexperience. Journal of Personality , 59, 539–574. Wood , J. M., Garb, H. N., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2002).Clinical assessment. Annual Review of Psychology ,53, 519 –543. Wood , J. M., Nezworski, M. T., & Stejskal, W. J. (1996). Thinking critically about the Comprehensive System for the Rorschach: A reply to Exner. Psychological Science ,7, 14–17. Wood , J. M., Nezworski, M. T., & Stejskal, W. J. (1997). The reliabil- ity of the Comprehensive System for the Rorschach: A comment onMeyer (1997). Psychological Assessment ,9, 490 –494. Wood , J. V., Heimpel, S. A., Manwell, L. A., & Whittington, E. J. (2009). This mood is familiar and I don ’t deserve to feel better anyway: Mechanisms underlying self-esteemdifferences in motivation to repairsad moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,96, 363–380. Wood , J. V., Heimpel, S. A., Newby- Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (2005). Snatching defeat from the jaws of victory: Self-esteem differences inthe experience and anticipation ofsuccess. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,89, 764–780. Wood , W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sexdifferences. Psychological Bulletin , 128, 699–727. Wood , W., Wong, F. Y., & Chachere, J. G. (1991). Effects of media vio-lence on viewers ’aggression in unconstrained social interaction. Psychological Bulletin ,109, 371–383. Woodhill, B. M., & Samuels, C. A. (2004). Desirable and undesirable androgyny: A prescription for thetwenty-first century. Journal of Gender Studies ,13,1 5 –28. Woodward , S. A., McManis, M. H., Kagan, J., Deldin, P., Snidman, N., Lewis, M., et al. (2001). Infanttemperament and the brainstem auditory evoked response in later childhood. Developmental Psychology ,37, 533 –538. Woody , E. Z., & Sadler, P. (2008). Dissociation theories of hypnosis.In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 81 –110). New York: Oxford University Press. Wrosch , C., & Miller, G. E. (2009). Depressive symptoms can be useful: Self-regulatory and emotional ben- efits of dysphoric mood in adoles-cence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,96, 1181 –1190. Wrosch , C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Health stresses and de-pressive symptomatology in the elderly: The importance of health engagement control strategies. Health Psychology ,21, 340–348. Wu, K. D., & Clark, L. A. (2003). Relations between personality traitsand self-reports of daily behavior. Journal of Research in Personality , 37, 231–256. Wyatt , E. (2005, February 24). A Mormon daughter ’s book stirs a storm. New York Times . Yarnold, P. R., Mueser, K. T., & Grimm, L. G. (1985). Interpersonaldominance of Type As in groupdiscussions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,94, 233 –236. Young , J. E. (1982). Loneliness, depres- sion and cognitive therapy: Theory and application. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness (pp. 379 –405). New York: Wiley. Zadra , A., Desjardins, S., & Marcotte, E. (2006). Evolutionary function ofdreams: A test of the threat simu- lation theory in recurrent dreams. Consciousness and Cognition ,15, 450–463. Zadra , A. L., O ’Brien, S. A., & Donderi, D. C. (1998). Dream content, dream recurrence and well-being:A replication with a younger sample. Imagination, Cognition and Personality ,17, 293–311. Zajonc , R. B. (2001). The family dynamics of intellectualREFERENCES 501
development. American Psychologist ,56, 490 –496. Zajonc , R. B., & Sulloway, F. J. (2007). The confluence model: Birth order as a within-family or between-family dynamic? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,33, 1187 –1194. Zammichieli , M. E., Gilroy, F. D., & Sherman, M. F. (1988). Relations between sex-role orientation and marital satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin ,14, 747–754. Ze idner , M. (2007). Anxiety and coping with community disasters: The Israeliexperience. Journal of Research in Per sonality ,41,2 1 3 –220. Zeidner , M., & Hammer, A. L. (1992). Coping with missile attack:Resources, strategies, and out-comes. Journal of Personality ,60, 709–746. Zeigler-Hill , V., Chadha, S., & Osterman, L. (2008). Psychologicaldefense and self-esteem instability: Is defense style associated with unstable self-esteem? Journal of Research in Personality ,42, 348–364. Zernike , K. (2000, August 25). Academic race gap grows again. San Jose Mercury News , p. 21A. Zezima , K., & Carey, B. (2009, September 11). Ex-priest challenges abuse conviction on repressed memories. New York Times , p. A13. Zhou, X., Saucier, G., Gao, D., & Liu, J. (2009). The factor structure ofChinese personality traits. Journal of Personality ,77, 363 –400. Zillmann , D. (1979). Hostility and aggression . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Zillmann , D., Bryant, J., & Cantor, J. R. (1974). Brutality of assault inpolitical cartoons affecting humor appreciation. Journal of Research in Personality ,7, 334–345.Zimbardo, P. G. (1986). The Stanford Shyness Project. In W. H. Jones, J. M. Cheek, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 17 –25). New York: Plenum. Zuckerman , M., & Gagne, M. (2003). The COPE revised: Proposing a 5-factor model of coping strategies. Journal of Research in Personality , 37, 169–204. Zuckerman , M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M.(1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: The Big Three, the Big Five, and thealternative five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 65, 757–768. Zur, O., Williams, M. H., Lehavot, K., & Knapp, S. (2009).Psychotherapist self-disclosure and transparency in the Internet age. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice ,40,2 2 –30.502 REFERENCES
NAME INDEX Abela, J. R. Z., 450 Ablow, J. C., 163Aboud, F., 78 Abramson, L. Y., 424, 446, 449 –450 Accortt, E. E., 246 Adams, N. E., 363Adler, N. E., 129 Affleck, G., 15 Ahadi, S. A., 230 –232 Ainsworth, M. D. S., 140Albino, A., 130, 145 Alden, L. E., 203 Aldwin, C. M., 130Alford, B. A., 445Ali, S., 364 Allard, L. M., 145 –146 Allen, A., 168 Allen, J., 145Allen, J. J. B., 246 Allen, M., 312 Allen, M., 318Alloy, L. B., 446, 450Allport, G. W., 149, 153 –154 Almagor, M., 163Alper, C. M., 320Altman, I., 311, 335Altman, J., 128 Alvarenga, M., 196 Amir, N., 201Amirkhan, J. H., 263 Anderman, E. M., 193 Anderman, L. H., 193 Anders, S. L., 144Andersen, B. L., 419Anderson, C. A., 134, 200, 319, 391, 394 –395, 436 –438, 451Anderson, K. B., 134 Angelo, K. M., 263Angie, A., 262 Angleitner, A., 150, 176, 255 Ansbacher, H. L., 98 Ansbacher, R. R., 98Antill, J. K., 381, 383 Antonen, M., 216 Antoni, M. H., 215Antrobus, J. S., 74Araya, R., 450 Archer, J. A., 316 Archer, J., 248Archibald, F. S., 320Arkin, A. M., 74 Arkin, R. M., 203, 400 Armeli, S., 159Arneson, J. J., 177Arnkoff, D. B., 367 Arnold, M. L., 118 Aron, A., 145Aron, E. N., 145Arroyo, J. A., 130 Arthur, W., 162 Asendorpf, J. B., 161Aserinsky, E., 74Ashton, M. C., 163 –164 Aspinwall, L. G., 214, 216Atherton, S. C., 203Atkins, D. C., 368 Atkinson, G., 91 Atkinson, G. R., 11 Atkinson, J. W., 184Aube, J., 384Auerbach, S. M., 130 Austenfeld, J. L., 130Austin, T., 309 Averill, J. R., 126, 335, 338Aviles, F. E., 136 Avitzur, E., 405 Bach, L., 429 Bacon, P. L., 444 Bagby, R. M., 175 Bailey, J. M., 62, 269Baird, B. M., 262Baker, E. L., 87 Baker, L. A., 259 Baker, S. R., 215Balabanis, M. H., 364Baldwin, A. S., 364 Baldwin, M. W., 202 Baldwin, S. A., 368Bandura, A., 351, 353, 356 –357, 362 –364, 378, 387 Bannister, D., 428Barbaranelli. C., 364Barber, A. M., 240Barber, T. X., 88, 90 –92 Barbo, E., 266Bardos, A. N., 60Bargh, J. A., 438Bar-Haim, Y., 233 Barkin, A., 320 Barlett, C. P., 394Barlow, D. H., 238 Barnes, M., 266, 269 Barnes, R. D., 382 Barnett, B., 80Baron, R. A., 83Barrett, K. C., 142 Barrett, M. S., 309 503
Barrett, P., 227 Barrick, M. R., 171 –172 Barron, K. E., 193 Barron, W. L., 83Barry, J. E., 319Bartholomew, K., 142, 320 Bartholow, B. D., 394 Bartlett, M. Y., 272Barton, J. R., 429 Bartussek, D., 246 Basgall, J. A., 405 Bata. I., 197Bates, J. E., 248Batten, P. G., 324 Batty, G. D., 406 Baucom, D. H., 368, 381Bauder, D., 390Bauer, J. J., 364 Baumeister, R. F., 137 –138, 169, 237 Bauminger, N., 312 Baxter, C. E., 91Beall, S. K., 313 Beard, C., 201 Beaubien, J. M., 192Beaulieu-Prevost, D., 91Bechtle, R., 116 Beck, A. T., 58, 400, 425, 445 –446, 450 Becker, D., 386 Beevers, C. G., 450 Belanger, K. A., 84 Belgrave, F. Z., 310 Bell, N. J., 383Bell, R. C., 428Bem, D. J., 168 Bem, S. L., 379, 381 Ben-Ari, A., 210, 212Benassi, V. A., 404Benet-Martinez, V., 10, 163 Bengston, P. L., 298 Benham, G., 92Ben Hamida, S., 269, 272Benjamin, A. J., 7, 195, 394 Bennett, P., 407 –408 Bera, S., 144 Bereson, K. R.,Berg, J. H., 324 Berkman, L. F., 320 Berkow, I., 317Berkowitz, L., 83, 133 –134, 438 Berman, J. S., 309 Berman, M. E., 7 Berman, M. I., 272Bernston, G. G., 320 –321 Berry, D. S., 206 Berry, J. D., 321 Berry, J. M., 195Bettencourt, B. A., 7, 195, 407 Beyer, J., 363 Bianchi, E. C., 116 Biederman, J., 235Billings, D. W., 209Bissonnette, V., 202 Bhar, S. S., 58Bhatia, A., 83 Blankenship, V., 184 Blanton, H., 213, 423 Blatt, S. J., 76, 142Blehar, M. C., 140Bleidorn, W., 150 Bleske, A. L., 236 Bless, H., 292Block, J., 9, 80, 163, 298 Block, J. H., 9 Bloise, S. M., 441 Bodenlos, J. S., 406Bodner, E., 400Boldizar, J. P., 389 Boldry, J., 145 Bolduc, D., 377Bonacci, A. M., 136Bonanno, G. A., 364 Bond, M., 79 Bono, J. E., 405Booth, R. J., 316Booth-Kewley, S., 165 Borkenau, P., 255 Borod, J. C., 314Bossio, L. M., 215Bouas, H. K., 193 Bouchard, T. J., 252 Boudreaux, E. D., 406Bourbeau, L. S., 142 Bouvrette, A., 329 Bovbjerg, D. H., 360 Bower, E., 201Bowers, S. P., 381Bowker, A., 242 Bowlby, J., 140 Bowman, P. J., 118Boyatzis, R. E., 187Bradbury, T. N., 146, 384 Bragg, M. E., 319 Branch, O., 394Brantley, P. J., 406Brashears, M. E., 317 Brass, D. J., 136 Braverman, J., 270Brebner, J., 262Brenman, M., 87, 89 Brennan, K. A., 142, 144 Bridges, N. A., 214, 309Briggs, S. R., 163 –164, 202 Brilliant, M. A., 78 Brissette, I., 206, 215 Britt, T. W., 168 –169 Brockner, J., 325 –326 Brodkin, J. D., 400 Brody, J. E., 50 Brody, L. R., 78Brook, A. T., 328 Brook, J. S., 391 Brooks, K. P., 206 Brooks-Gunn, J., 382Brower, A. M., 217Brown, J., 398 Brown, J. D., 212, 325, 327Brown, M., 62 Brown, R. J., 73 Brown, S., 390 Bruch, M. A., 201, 320Brunhart, S. M., 216Brunner, R., 233 Brunstein, J. C., 185 Bryant, C. M., 161Bryant, J., 83 Buchholz, E. S., 335 Buckley, M. E., 297 Buhler, C., 318Buigas, R. A., 175Buijsse, B., 216 Buis, T., 175 Bulkeley, K., 74Bullock, W. A., 226Bunde, J., 196 Burge, D., 146 Burger, J. M., 172, 270, 335, 337, 400, 404 Burgess, C. A., 91 Burke, B. W., 163 Burleson, M. H., 320Burns, J. W., 364Burns, L. R., 205 Burr, R., 165 Busch, A. M., 424Bush, A. L., 330 Bushman, B. J., 136 –138, 391 –392, 393 –394, 437 Buss, A. H., 200 –201, 230 Buss, D. M., 236 –238, 265 –267, 269 –270 Bussey, K., 378 Bustamante, V., 74Butcher, J. N., 175Butera, F., 192 Butler, A. C., 425 Butler, J. M., 299Butler, S., 441Buunk, B. P., 213 Byrne, B. M., 382 Byrne, D., 128 Caccioppo, J. T., 196, 205, 319 –321 Cain, D. J., 296, 301, 307 Cairns, R. B., 7Caldwell, D. F., 172Calkins, S. D., 233 Calverley, D. S., 92 Camara, W. J., 62, 173Cameron, C. L., 128Cameron, J., 371 Cameron, L. D., 316 Cameron, R., 425Campbell, J. B., 261Campbell, J. D., 212, 326, 328 Campbell, L., 145 Campbell, S. M., 384Campbell, V. L., 62, 175Campos, J. J., 142 Canevello, A., 330504 NAME INDEX
Canli, T., 226, 260 Cantor, J. R., 83 Cantor, N., 217 Caperchione, C., 320Cappella, E., 404Caprara, G. V., 364 Card, N. A., 439 Carey, B., 50Carey, G., 7, 206, 252 Carlozzi, B. L., 315 Carlson, M., 135, 438 Carnagey, N. L., 394, 438Carnelley, K. B., 146Carnevale, P. J. D., 382 Carosella, G. M., 86 Carr, D. E., 82Carroll, J. M., 205Carter, S. E., 217 Carter-Saltzman, L., 257 Carvajal, S. C., 216Carver, C. S., 129, 142, 185, 205, 212 –215, 316 Caspi, A., 170, 206, 230 –232, 255 –256 Catalano, R., 132Catanzaro, S. J., 320Cattell, H. E. P., 159 Cattell, R. B., 160 Cavallero, C., 71Cepeda-Benito, A., 316 Cervone, D., 188 Chachere, J. G., 391 Chadha, S., 80Chaikin, A. L., 311 –312 Chalmers, A., 213 Chance, J. E., 351 Chang, E. C., 213 –214, 217 Chao,G. T., 171Chaplin, W. F., 80 Chapman, J. E., 425 Charles, S. T., 205Chase, S. K., 330Chason, S., 312 Chastain, R. L., 316 Chatard, S., 443Chavanon, M., 246Cheek, J. M., 200 –201 Chelune, G. J., 324Chen, H., 379Cheng, C., 130, 381Cherry, M., 78 Chess, S., 239 –242 Cheung, M., 130 Chia, R., 405Chipperfield, J. G., 399 Chipuer, H. M., 255 Chitsabesan, P., 429Choi, D-W, 332 Choi, I., 332 Christensen, A., 368 Christensen, K., 9, 252Christensen, P. N., 321Church, A. T., 10, 163 Churchill, S. L., 242Clair, S. D., 216 Clancy, S. M., 444 Clark, C. L., 142 Clark, D. A., 445 –447, 449 Clark, L. A., 165, 168, 204 –206, 230, 425 Clark, M. S., 312Clark, R. A., 184Clearfield, M. W., 377 Cleveland, H. H., 101 Clore, G. L., 210 Coccaro, E. F., 7Coe, W. C., 88 –89, 91 Cohen, D. B., 74 Cohen, L. H., 159, 381 Cohen, P., 379, 391Cohen, S., 197, 206 –207, 263, 320 Cohn, N. B., 310 Colder, C. R., 206, 315 Colder, M., 315Cole, S. G., 202Coleman, M., 384 Collins, A., 144 Collins, N. L., 144 –146, 310 Comer, R., 195Compas, B. E., 129 Comstock, G., 389, 391 Conger, R. D., 161Conley, J. J., 227 Constantinople, A., 379 Conti, D., 50 Conti, R., 293Contrada, R. J., 197Conway, F., 215 Conway, M. A., 119 Cook, A., 330Cook, O., 319Cook, S. W., 128 Coolidge, C., 82 Coolidge, F. L., 80Cooper, C., 262Cooper, H. M., 241, 262, 403 –404 Cooper, M. L., 130, 145, 329Cooper, S. M., 242Cooper, S. R., 177Cooper, T., 195 Coopman, S. J., 429 Coplan, R. J., 240, 242Corbett, S., 429Cordeiro, A. M., 422 Cordova, J. V., 212 Corley, R., 256Cornell, D. G., 248Cornelius, J. S., 266 Cornette, M. M., 424 Cosby, M., 270Cossette, L., 377 Costa, P. T., 129, 159 –160, 163, 177, 196, 205, 255, 270 Costello, C. G., 449 Council, J. R., 91 Couper, D. J., 196 Cournoyer, R. J., 78Cowan, C. P., 163Cowan, P. A., 163 Cowen, E. I., 240 Cowles, A., 408 Cox-Fuenzalida, L., 229, 262Crabbe, J., 258 Crabbe, J. C., 242 Craig, J., 384Craighead, W. E., 449Craik, K. H., 15 Cramer, K. M., 319, 337 Cramer, P., 76, 78, 80Cramer, R. E., 266, 269Crandall, R., 288 Crane, E. C., 212 Crane, F. G., 212Crawford, E., 320Crawford, T., 379 Creighton, A., 92 Crick, F., 75Crick, N. R., 436, 439Crisp, R. J., 444 Crocker, J., 328 –330 Cromwell, R. L., 448 Cronkite, R. C., 129Cropanzano, R. S., 2090 Cross, S. E., 442, 444 Crossen, C., 334Crost, N. W., 246Crowson, J. J., 448 Csikszentmihalyi, I. S., 291 Csikszentmihalyi, M., 291 –293, 334 Cunningham, J. A., 312 Cunningham, J. D., 381 Cunningham, M. R., 206 Curran, J. P., 203Cury, F., 191Cutler, S. E., 209 Cutmore, T. R. H., 360 Cutrona, C. E., 318 –319 Cyranowski, J. M., 419 Dadds, M. R., 360 Da Fonseca, D., 191Daniels, D., 259Daniels, J., 292 Danoff-Burg, S., 386 Dalsky, D. J., 264Danoff-Burg, S., 128Darnon, C., 192 David, D., 74 Davidson, J., 73Davidson, K., 76, 80, 84, 297 Davidson, K. W., 197 Davidson, R. J., 244 –246 Davila, J., 146 Davis, J. D., 310Davis, K. E., 144 Davis, K. L., 423 Davis, P. J., 442Davis, R., 344Davis, S. F., 344 Davison, J., 195NAME INDEX 505
Dawes, R. M., 62 Day, N. E., 399 Deary, I. J., 406 Deaux, K., 382De Boeck, P., 184de Castro, B. O., 394 Deckers, L., 82 Deffenbacher, J., 197, 425DeFranc, W., 78 DeFries, J. C., 252, 256 de Kort, Y. A. W., 337 del Valle, C. H. C., 217Demakis, G. J., 175Dembroski, T. M., 196 DeNeve, K. M., 262, 403 Denson, T. F., 136DePaulo, B. M., 201 –202 De Pelsmacker, P., 209 Depner, C., 187 Depue, R. A., 404, 449de Rivera, J., 50Derlega, V. J., 310 –312 Derr, W. R., 326Derry, P. A., 446de Sales French, R., 320De Schipper, J. C., 242 Desjardins, S., 73 de St. Aubin, E., 118DeSteno, D., 272 Detre, T., 195 Deutsch, C. L., 382 De Vellis, B. M., 398De Vellis, R. F., 398de Vries, M. W., 449 DeWolfe, D. J., 130 Diamond, A., 118Dickens, W. T., 253Di Dio, L., 384 Diefenbach, M., 206 Diehl, M., 142Diener, E., 11, 205, 209 –210, 227, 263 –264, 332 –333, 440 –441 Diener, M., 333Digman, J., 159, 163DiLalla, L. F., 252Dill, K. E., 394 –395 DiMatteo, M. R., 210Dindia, K., 312Dion, K. K., 144Dion, K. L., 144 Di Paula, A., 328 Dobson, K. S., 449Docter, R., 450Dodge, K. A., 129, 436, 438 –439 Dodgson, P. G., 327Dollard, J., 131Dollinger, S. J., 405, 444 Domhoff, B., 71, 75 Dompnier, B., 192 Donderi, D. C., 73Donnellan, M. B., 161Donnelly, D. A., 316 Donnerstein, E., 389 –391Donovan, J. J., 171 Doob, A. N., 131 –132, 135 Dooley, D., 132 Doucet, C., 229Douvan, E., 187Doyle, W. J., 320 Dozois, D. J. A., 449 Draguns, J. G., 11Dubow, E. F., 391 Dubreuil, D. L., 89 du Chene, T., 91 Dufour, C. L., 404Duke, T., 73Dull, W. R., 201 Dunbar, E., 50 Duncan, J. H., 203Duncan, K. E., 117Dunn, T. W., 425 Durante, K. M., 266 Durbin, C. E., 297Dutton, K. A., 325, 327Dwivedi, Y., 400 Dwyer, J., 215 Dwyer, K. A., 207Dyce, J. A., 165Dyrenforth, P. S., 262 Eagly, A. H., 247, 270 Earleywine, M., 136 Eastwick, P. W., 270 Eaves, L., 259 Eby, L. T., 403Eccles, J., 187Eder, D., 81 Edge, K., 128 Eggleston, T., 213Egloff, B., 205Eisenberg, J. G., 391 Eisenberger, R., 371 Eisenbud, L., 377Eisler, R. M., 195Ekman, P., 244 Eley, T. C., 450 Elliot, A. J., 185, 191, 193Elliott, A. J., 365, 432Elliott, R., 296 Ellis, A., 425 –426 Ellman, S. J., 74 Elnick, A. B., 142Else-Quest, N. M., 231 Emde, R. N., 248 Emerson, T., 201Emmons, R. A., 205, 209, 212, 264Endler, N. S., 166 Epstein, J. A., 202 Epstein, S., 167 –168 Erazo-Caicedo, M. I., 429 Erez, A., 405 Ericksen, A. J., 163 Erickson, M. H., 87Erikson, E. H., 105 –106, 111 Ernst, J. M., 319 Eron, L. D., 7, 132, 391Escobar, O., 386 Espindle, D., 419 Etaugh, C., 377 Eubanks, J., 394, 438Evans, D. E., 230Evans, F. B., 62 Evans, J., 450 Evans, R. I., 216, 355 Evans, S., 377 Evans, W. P., 404 Evon, D., 364 Ey, S., 129 Eysenck, H. J., 164, 224 –229, 259 Eysenck, S. B. G., 224, 227 –228 Fairey, P. J., 326 Fancher, R. E., 43Fanning, J. R., 7 Farabee, D. J., 202 Farber, B. A., 308 –309, 312 Farley, F., 228Fassler, O., 89, 92 Fearn, M., 246 Feeney, J. A., 142, 145 –146 Feinglos, M. N., 206Feingold, A., 269 Feist, G. J., 161 Feixas, G., 429Feldman Barrett, L., 310, 441Fenichel, O., 79 Ferguson, S. M., 400 Fernengel, K., 196Fiese, B. H., 422Finch, J. F., 270 Fincham, F. D., 384 Findley, M. J., 404Finkel, D., 145, 258 Finkel, E. J., 270 Finkelstein, L. E., 315 Fins, A., 74Finzi-Dottan, R., 312Fioravanti, M., 299 Fisch, M., 382 Fischer, A. H., 210Fischer, A. R., 11Fischer, P., 438 Fischer, R., 142, 213 Fischetti, M., 203Fisher, M., 40Fivush, R., 441 Flanagan, M., 394 Fletcher, B., 129Fletcher, G. J. O., 269Flinn, J. A., 379 Floderus-Myrhed, B., 259 Flora, J., 322Flory, J. D., 197Flynn, J. R., 253 Foa, E. B., 129 Fodor, E. M., 185Folkman, S., 128 –129 Fonagy, P., 58 Fontaine, R. G., 436, 438506 NAME INDEX
Ford, M. B., 146 Ford, R. Q., 76 Forman, E. M., 425 Foulkes, D., 71Fox, N. A., 233, 245Fraley, R. C., 146, 450 Frankel, A., 322 Frankel, B. S., 325Fransella, F., 411, 428 Frattaroli, J., 314 –316 Frazier, L. D., 422Frazier, P. A., 272Fredickson, B. L., 329Freemon, J. A., 320 Freidrich-Cofer, L., 391 Frensch, K. M., 118Fresco, D. M., 450Freud, A., 79 Freud, S., 47, 53 –55, 57 –58, 81, 91, 115 Friberg, L., 260 Friedman, L. C., 214Friedman, H. S., 162, 210, 212, 216 Friedman, M., 193 Friesen, M., 269Friesen, W. V., 244Frist, W. H., 212 Fritz, H. L., 320, 384 –385 Froming, W. J., 419 Fromm, E., 117 Fry, G., 92 Fujita, F., 263, 441 Fulker, D. W., 256, 259Funder, D. C., 166, 170 Gabbard, G. O., 58 Gabriel, S., 443Gabrielidis, C., 266Gacono, C. B., 62 Gaeddert, W. P., 188 Gagne, M., 128Gale, C. R., 406Gallagher, F. M., 327 Gallagher, K. C., 240 Gallo, L. C., 196Gangestad, S. W., 248Ganiban, J. M., 231 Ganong, L. H., 384 Gao, D., 163Garb, H. N., 62 –63 Garcia, J., 371 Garcia, S., 202 Gardner, W. L., 443Gaschke, Y. N., 205Gasper, K., 217 Gastorf, J. W., 194 Gatz, M., 205Gaudiano, B. A., 425 Geary, D. C., 265 Gee, C. B., 212 Geen, R. G., 137, 262, 391 –392 Geers, A. L., 213Geleijnse, K. M., 216 Gemar, M. C., 449Gendlin, E. T., 280 Gentile, D. A., 395 George, W. H., 368 Geraerts, E., 50Gerrard, M., 213, 423Gershoff, E. T., 389 Gershuny, B. S., 129 Gerald, K., 196Gerstein, R. K., 450 Gersten, J. C., 391 Gersten, M., 239 Geshuri, Y., 387Gest, S. D., 233, 235Geuens, M., 209 Gibbons, F. X., 213, 423 Gibson, H. B., 228Gidron, Y., 197Giesler, R. B., 448 Gilbert, L., 382 Gill, M. M., 87, 89Gillespie, N. A., 258, 260Gilliland, K., 226, 229 Gillum, B., 9 Gillum, R., 9Gilroy, F. D., 384Giltay, E. J., 216 Gjerde, P. F., 9 Glasberg, R., 78Glass, C. R., 203 Glass, D. C., 194 –195, 206 Glazer, K., 196Gleser, G. C., 76Glisky, M. L., 91Gohm, C. L., 210, 264 Gol, A. R., 128 Goldberg, J. F., 450Goldberg, L. R., 159, 163 –164, 170 –171 Goldsmith, H. H., 142, 231 Gonzales-Roma, V., 205 Good, T. E., 241Gooding, R. Z., 171Goodwin, R., 319 Gorassini, D. R., 92 Gordon, A. H., 210Gore, J. S., 444Gorsuch, R. L., 164 Goswick, R. A., 320 –321 Gotay, C. C., 195 Gottesman, I. I., 252Gough, H. G., 299 Gouze, M., 9 Graef, R., 334Graham, J. W., 404Graham, S. M., 312 Granberg, E., 423 Gray, E. K., 163Gray, J. A., 226 Graziano, W. G., 161 –162, 176, 231, 270 Green, B. L., 382Green, J. P., 88, 91Green, L. R., 319 Greenberg, J. M., 201Greenberg, R., 74 Greene, K., 310 Greenier, K. D., Greening, L., 450Greitemeyer, T., 438Greve, W., 327 Grich, J., 145 –146 Grigorenko, E. L., 253 Grimm, L. G., 195 Grob, A., 227 Gronnerod, C., 62 Gross, J. J., 210 Grotevant, H. D., 297 Grothe, K. B., 406 Grove, J. R., 189 Gruber, C., 233Gruzelier, J. H., 91Guerra, N. G., 132, 389 Guichard, A. C., 146 Guimaraes, F. S., 400Guimond, S., 443 Gullone, E., 320 Gunderson, J. G., 58 Gunlicks, M., 145Gunthert, K. C., 159Gur, R. C., 246 Guyll, M., 197 Gwaltney, C. J., 364 Haaga, D. A. F., 206 Haan, B., 313 Haans, A., 337Habashi, M. M., 161Haeffel, C. J., 450 Haemmerlie, F. M., 203 Hagemann, D., 205, 246Hager, J. L., 371Hair, E. C., 176 Haig, J., 163 Hall, C., 405Hall, C. S., 70 –71 Hall, J. A., 380 –381 Hall, M. H., 277, 285 Hallam, J. R., 327Hallmark, R., 62, 175Hallquist, M. N., 88 Hamaker, S., 206 Hamamura, T., 331Hamer, R. J., 201Hammen, C., 146 Hammer, A. L., 213 Handler, L., 60Handley, G. W., 88Hankin, B. L., 450 Hanley-Dunn, P., 321 Hanrahan, S. J., 189Hansell, S., 206Hansen, J. S., 206 Hansen, R. D., 129 Hanson, R. K., 62Harackiewicz, J., 193, 371, 382Hardin, E. E., 424 Har-Even, D., 312NAME INDEX 507
Harmon-Jones, E., 246 Harris, C. R., 272 Harris, M. B., 134 Harris, R., 394Harrison, A. A., 264Harrist, A. W., 315 Harrist, S., 315 Hart, H. M., 118Harter, S. L., 429 Hartman, K. A., 408 Hartshorne, H., 166 Harvey, J. H., 312Harvey, R. J., 200Haselton, M. G., 266 Hatfield, E., 266 Haselton, M. G., 236Hasselbach, P., 406Havermans, R., 449 Hawkley, L. C., 329 –321 Hawks, E., 209 Hawley, C. W., 261,Hayden, E. P., 297 Haydon, K. C., 144 Haynes, S. G., 196Haynes, S. N., 366Hazan, C., 141 –142, 145 Headey, B., 263Heath, A. C., 259Heatherton, T. F., 325 Hebblethwaithe, S., 118 Heckhausen, J., 399 Hedlund, S., 449Heery, E. A., 201Heim, M., 319 Heimberg, R. G., 201 Heimpel, S. A., 326 –327 Heine, S. J., 213, 331 –332, 424 Heinrich, L. M., 320 Heinricks, N., 203 Helbraun, E., 335Helgeson, V. S., 126, 312, 320, 378, 381, 384 –386 Helm, B., 321Helmreich, R. L., 184, 379Helms, J. E., 175Hemenover, S. H., 316 Henderson, H. A., 233 Hendrick, S. S., 312Henriques, J. B., 246Herbst, J. H., 338 Herbst, K. C., 100 Heron, J., 450Herrnstein, R. J., 253Hervig, L. K., 165 Hessling, R. M., 314, 316, 423 Hewig, J., 246Hewitt, E. C., 89 Hibbard, S., 76 Higgins, C. A., 172 Higgins, E. T., 420, 423Higgins, R. L., 406Hilgard, E. R., 88, 91 Hill, C. E., 71, 309Hill, G. J., 203 Hilsenroth, M. J., 62 Hiroto, D. S., 397 Hobbs, S. A., 322Hockenbury, D., 322Hof, J., 196 Hoffman, L. W., 257 Hogan, M. E., 450Hogan, R., 171 Hojnoski, R. L., 62 Holcom, M. L., 202 Holcroft, C., 196Hollon, S. D., 425Holloway, R. A., 419 Holloway, S., 262 Holmberg, D., 444Holmes, A., 119Hoobler, J. M., 136 Hooker, K., 213, 422 Hoover, C. W., 201Hopkins, J. R., 105Hopkinson, P. J., 246 Horley, J., 429 Horn, J., 160Horn, J. M., 256Horney, K. D., 113 –114 Hornstein, G. A., 312Horowitz, L. M., 142, 320, 322Hough, R., 132 House, R. J., 187 Houshi, F., 73 Houston, B. K., 197Howard, M. L., 89Howell, D. C., 129 Hoyt, I. P., 91 Hoyt, M. F., 74Huang, J. Y., 312Hubbard, B., 162 Huesmann, L. R., 7, 132, 389, 391, 436, 438 Huff, L., 325 Huff, M., 197 Hui, C. H., 188 Hunsley, J., 62Hunt, J. M., 166Hurtz, G. M., 171 Huston, A. C., 391 Hutchinson, C., 213Hyde, J. S., 187, 231 Iacono, W., 258 Ialongo, N., 377Ickes, W., 145, 201 –202, 382 –383, 419 Ihilevich, D., 76 Ilardi, S. S., 449 Ilgen, M., M., 364Illingworth, K. S. S., 217 Ingram, R. E., 449 Inouye, D. K., 398 Inouye, J., 163 Jaccard, J., 423 Jackson, B., 197Jackson, C. J., 227 Jackson, D. N., 164, 171 Jackson, L. A., 377, 382 Jackson, S. A., 292Jacobs, D. R., 197Jaffe, K., 146 Jahoda, M., 63, 69 Jang, K. L., 255Janicki, D., 126 Janicki-Devers, D., 320 Janoff-Bulman, R., 316 Jarrett, R. B., 425Jefferson, T., 100Jeffery, R. W., 364 Jeffrey, A., 309 Jemmolo, S., 422Jenkins, C. D., 193, 195Jenkins, S. R., 187 Jensen, A. R., 253 Jensen, M. P., 86Jensen-Campbell, L. A., 161, 176, 231, 270 Jing, H., 331Joca, S. R. L., 400 Johansson, B., 406 John, O. P., 159, 163, 210 Johnson, B. T., 197 Johnson, C., 334Johnson, E. A., 80Johnson, H. H., 195 Johnson, J. G., 391 –392 Johnson, K. A., 202 Johnson, L. M., 266Johnson, M. K., 441 Johnson, P. M., 422 Johnson, T. B., 318Johnson, W., 255, 258 Joiner, T. E., 320, 449 Joireman, J., 163 Jome, L. M., 11Jones, A., 288Jones, E., 40, 64 Jones, M., 320 Jones, R. E., 209Jones, S. C., 215Jones, W. H., 320 –322 Jorgensen, R. S., 197Jorgensen, S. J., 246Josephs, R. A., 444, 448Jourard, S. M., 308, 312 Judge, T. A., 171 –172, 405 Jung, C. G., 102 –104 Justice, L. M., 241 Kaflowitz, N. G., 320 Kagan, J., 232 –235 Kahn, J. H., 314, 316Kahn, M. L., 313 Kaiser, F. G., 337 Kalechstein, A. D., 404Kallio, S., 89Kamen-Siegel, L., 215 Kandler, C., 150508 NAME INDEX
Kang, S-M., 331 Kaplan, A., 191 –193 Kaplan, G. D., 408 Kaprio, J., 259Karney, B. R., 146Karpinski, A., 330 Karraker, K. H., 377 Kasen, S., 279, 391Kashdan, T. B., 201 Kashy, D. A., 145, 321 Kasimatis, M., 263 Kasmer, J. A., 164Kasser, T., 7Kaster-Bundgaard, J., 365 Katon, W., 130 Katsanis, J., 88Katsura, H., 331Katz, I. M., 212 Kaus, C. R., 422 Kawabata, Y., 332Kawachi, I., 196Keefe, R. C., 266 Keelan, J. P. R., 144 –145 Keller, J., 292 Keller, J. W., 175Kelley, K., 240 Kelly, A. E., 316 Kelly, G. A., 411, 413, 431Kemp, A. H., 246 Kendler, K. S., 259 Kendzierski, D., 419 Kennedy, S. J., 449Kenny, C., 316Kenny, D. A., 201 Kenrick, D. T., 266, 270, 382 Kent, G., 359Keogh, B. K., 240 –242 Kern, M. L., 162 Kernis, M. H., 325 Kessler, R. C., 142, 259Ketelaar, T., 264Khaw, K-T., 399 Kidd, K. K., 253 Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., 196, 320Kiefer, A. K., 330Kihlstrom, J. F., 87, 91 Kim, J-H., 447 Kim, S., 246Kimiecik, J. C., 292Kim-Prieto, C., 332 King, D. W., 129 King, L. A., 129, 212, 316Kingsley, D., 15Kirk, S. B., 128 Kirker, W. S., 421 Kirkpatrick, L. A., 144 –145 Kirsch, I., 86, 88 –89, 91 Kirsch, M., 171 Kirschenbaum, H., 280 Kitayama, S., 10 –11, 331 –332 Kivimaki, M., 213Klein, D. C., 401 Klein, D. N., 297Kleinke, C. L., 313 Kleinman, A., 11 Kleitman, N., 74 Klinesmith, J. 7Kling, K. C., 187Klinger, B. I., 92 Klohnen, E. C., 144 Klonowicz, T., 406Klusas, J. A., 316 Knapp, S., 309 Knee, C. R., 330 Knight, P. D., 202Knowles, E. S., 179Knox, J., 89, 92 Knox, S., 309 Kobasa, S. C., 407Koelling, R. A., 371Koester, N. H., 320, 324 Koestner, R., 384 Kohler, S. S., 171Kolodziej, M. E., 197Konecni, V. J., 135 Konold, T. R., 240 Kop, W. J., 320Korabik, K., 382Koriat, A., 126 Korn, J. H., 344 Koskenvuo, M., 259Kowalski, R. M., 202 –203 Kozak, M., 74 Kraft, M., 163 Krahe, B., 395Kram, M. L., 400Kramer, G. L., 400 Krantz, D. S., 196 Kring, A. M., 201, 209 –211 Krokoff, L. J., 84Kromhout, D., 216 Krueger, R. F., 255, 258 Kubzansky, L. D., 197Kuebli, J., 441Kuhlman, D. M., 163 Kuhlman, T. L., 81, 85 Kuiper, N. A., 84, 421, 446Kulick, J. A., 382Kulick, W., 81 Kulka, A., 187 Kuncel, N. R., 170, 177Kunda, Z., 418Kuppens, P. 332 Kurdek, L. A., 383 Kusulas, J. W., 165Kwon, P., 80 Labouvie-Vief, G., 142 LaFraniere, S., 177Lagace-Seguin, D. G., 240 LaGasse, L., 233 Lago, T., 319 Lahey, B. B., 159Lai, J. C. L., 214Laing, W. N., 326 Lake, M. A., 377Lake, R. P., 337 Lakin, J. L., 424 Lamb, M. E., 142 Lamke, L. K., 383Lamnin, A. D., 316Landau, S. F., 133 Landy, F. J., 171 Lane, J. D., 206Lane, R. D., 441 Langens, T. A., 316 Langer, E. J., 391, 398 –399 Langinvainio, H., 259 Langston, C. A., 217Larsen, J. T., 205 Larsen, R. J., 207, 209 –210, 263 –264 Larson, D. G., 316 Larson, R. W., 334 –337 Lassiter, G. D., 213 Lau, J. Y. F., 450 Lau, R. R., 408Lauder, W., 320Laurence, J-R., 91 Laurenceau, J-P, 310 Lavee, Y., 210, 212Lax, E., 293Lazarus, R. S., 126, 128 –129 Lazzari, R., 299Le, K., 262Leak, G. K., 83 Leary, M. R., 200 –203, 237, 338 LeBeau, L. S., 217Ledley, D. R., 201Lee, K., 163Lee, M., 337 Lee, Y-T., 213, 450 Leedham, B., 212Leen-Felder, E. W., 209Lefcourt, H. M., 84, 403 LeFevre, J., 292 Lefkowitz, E. S., 381Lefkowitz, M. M., 391Lehavor, K., 309 Lehman, D. R., 213, 332, 424 Lehman, J. M., 215 Leichsenring, F., 58LeMay, C. S., 202 Lenney, E., 379, 381 Lens, W., 184Lent, R. W., 364Leon, G. R., 9 Lepore, S. J., 315 –316, 381 Lerner, H., 76 Lerner, P., 76Lester, P., 83 Leszczynski, J. P., 379 Leue, A., 246Leventhal, E. A., 206Leventhal, H., 206 Levin, I., 321 Levin, R. A., 175Levine, A., 404Levy, B. R., 129, 315 Levy, K. N., 142NAME INDEX 509
Lewin, K., 411 Lewin, T., 177 Lewinsohn, P. M., 449 –450 Lewis, G., 450 Lewis, M., 118Lewis-Fernandez, R., 11 Li, M. P., 266 Liddy, S. J., 91Lilienfeld, S. O., 62 –63, 89 Lim, S-L., 447 Linde, J. A., 364 Linder, J. R., 395Lindsay, J. L., 134Linville, P. W., 190 Lips, H. M., 423 Lipsitt, L. P., 233Liss, M. B., 377Littig, L. W., 187 Little, T. D., 439 Liu, J., 163Liu, J. H., 142Livesley, W. J., 255 Lloyd, G. K., 400 Lockard, J. S., 178Loehlin, J. C., 255 –256 Long, B. C., 214 Long, C. R., 335, 338 Long, F. Y., 228Lopez, F. G., 364 Lopez, S. J., 301 Lord, C. G., 422 Lowell, E. L., 184Lozinski, R. H., 207Luben, R., 399 Lucas, R. E., 227, 262 Lueck, L., 246Luhtanen, R. K., 329 –330 Lumley, V., 365 Luria, Z., 377 Luthans, F., 364Lynch, P. J., 395Lynch, R., 197 Lyness, S. A., 195 Lynn, R., 227Lynn, S. J., 86, 88 –90, 92 Lytton, H., 256 MacCallum, R. C., 101 MacDonald, D. A., 164MacDonald, M. R., 446 MacGregor, M. W., 76, 80, 297 McAdams, D. P., 4, 117 –119 McAndrew, F. T., 7McCarthy, E. D., 391 McCaskill, C. C., 206 McCauley, Ch., 398McCauley, Cl., 82 McCeney, M. K., 196 McClearn, G. E., 260 McClelland, D. C., 184 –185, 187 McCloskey, M. S., 7McClure, E. B., 210 McConkey, K. M., 91McConnell, A. M., 424 McCrae, R. R., 100, 129, 159 –160, 163 –164, 176 –177, 205, 255, 270 McCrary, F., 338 McGhee, P. E., 85McGovern, A. R., 315 McGrath, M. J., 74 McGraw, A. P., 205McGregor, H. A., 191 McGue, M., 9, 252, 258 McGuire, C. V., 444 McGuire, M. T., 237McGuire, P. A., 84McGuire, W. J., 444 McInman, A., 189 McIntyre, C. W., 206McKellar, J., 364McKenzie, S. D., 84 McKillop, K. J., 315 McLean, K. C., 118McManus, J., 419McNalley, R. J., 50 McNaughton, N., 226 McPherson, M., 317Mackie, M., 444Maddux, J. E., 203 Madson, L., 442 Maehr, M. L., 191 –193 Magai, C., 215 Magnus, K., 263 Mahalik, J. R., 78 Mahoney, M. J., 367Maides, S., 407Maier, G. W., 185 Maier, M. A., 193 Maier, S. F., 395, 398, 400 –401 Main, K. J., 202Maiuro, R. D., 130 Major, B., 382 Malcuit, G., 377Mallon, S. D., 15Manis, M., 175 Mannell, R., 334 Manning-Ryan, B., 266Mansfield, E., 118 –119 Mansi, C., 422 Manstead, A. S. R., 210 Manwell, L. A., 326Marcotte, E., 73Marcus, D-K., 450 Marcus-Newhall, A., 135, 438 Margarett, J., 379Marien, M., 196Markey, C. N., 163 Markey, P. M., 163 Marks, G., 404Markus, H., 10 –11, 331 –332, 418 –420, 422 –423, 444 Marmot, M., 206 Maroto, J. J., 216Marsh, H. W., 381 –382 Marsh, S. C., 404 Marshall, G. N., 165Marshall, M. A., 212, 327 Marshall, T. C., 321 Martin, A., 362 Martin, C. L., 203, 377Martin, L. R., 216Martin, R., 197 Martin, R. A., 84 Martin, T., 227Martinez, J. C., 404Martinot, D., 444 Martyna, W., 381 Maruna, S., 118Marx, R., 320Masi, C.M., 321 Maslow, A. H., 279, 284, 286 –287, 290, 296, 334 Mateos, P. M., 217 Mathersul, d., 246 Matthews, K. A., 196 –197, 216 Matthews, W. J., 62 Matto, H. C., 60Maxwell, S. E., 321 May, M. A., 166 Mayer, J. D., 4, 205Mayman, M., 175Mayne, T. J., 58, 63, 296 Mead, A. D., 159 Meadows, S., 201 Meara, N. M., 423Measelle, J. R., 163 Mednick, M., 187 Meece, J. L., 193Meeker, W. B., 90Meichenbaum, D., 425 Meissner, W. W., 117 Meleshko, K. G. A., 203Melges, F. T., 404Melkman, R., 126 Mellman, T. A., 74 Menzaghi, F., 400Mershon, B., 164Meyer, B., 450 Meyer, G. J., 205 Meyerowitz, B. E., 212Michalski, R. L., 100Michela, J. L., 319 Mickelson, K. D., 142 Mikulincer, M., 145, 364, 400, 405Miles, D. R., 7 Miller, G. E., 248, 320 Miller, I. W., 449 –450 Miller, J. L., 175 Miller, L. C., 310Miller, M. G., 270 Miller, N., 135 –137, 438 Miller, N. E., 131 –132 Miller-Herringer, T., 210 Milling, L. S., 86 Mills, D. E., 84 Miltenberger, R. G., 365Min, K-H., 321Mineka, S., 269, 359 Miner, K. N., 316510 NAME INDEX