page_content
stringlengths 39
150k
|
---|
I went to a social gathering with a friend of mine recently. We talked with
old friends, met some new people, and mingled, sampling conversations, mu-sic, food, and drink. As is our custom, we immediately shared our perceptions
after leaving the party. “Did you notice how casually some people were
dressed? ”my friend asked. Actually, I hadn ’t. I asked him what he thought of
a man we had both met. “Wasn ’t he the most arrogant person? ”I asked. My
friend hadn ’t seen anything to indicate so. As we continued to exchange im-
pressions, I began to wonder if my friend had been at the same party interact-
ing with the same people I had. I couldn’ t believe he hadn ’t noticed how weird
the music was or realized how ill at ease the hostess seemed. My friend didn’ t
understand how I had failed to recognize the architecture of the house or eventhe furniture I sat on. “I guess we learned one lesson, ”I said. “Never go to a
party at their house again. ”My friend stared at me in disbelief. “Are you kid-
ding?”he said. “I had a great time! ”
How can two people participate in the same situation yet leave with very
different impressions of what happened? The answer from the cognitive ap-
proach to personality is that my friend and I have very different ways of pro-cessing information. Whereas I was attending to and processing information
about the weirdness of the music and the arrogance of the guests, my friend
entered the party prepared to notice clothing styles and furniture. Because we
attended to different features of the party, we had very different perceptions
of it and very different experiences. These different perceptions no doubt af-
fected how we acted that night and how we will respond to future invitations.
The cognitive approach explains differences in personality as differences
in the way people process information. Because I have developed relativelystable ways of processing information in social settings, I probably respond
to parties and other social gatherings in a similar way most of the time. Other
people respond differently than I do because they consistently see something
different from what I see.
Cognitive models of personality have become popular in recent years, but
they are not entirely new. An early predecessor can be found in Kurt Lewin ’s
(1938) field theory of behavior. Lewin described the mental representationswe form of the important elements in our lives and how we organize those
cognitive elements within our “life space. ”A more recent and, for the pur-
poses of this book, more important cognitive personality theory was devel-
oped by George Kelly. Since the publication of his book The Psychology of
Personal Constructs in 1955, Kelly ’s work has evolved into a rich source of
ideas for personality researchers and psychotherapists (Fransella, 2003,2005). It is interesting that Kelly did not think of himself as a cognitive psy-
chologist. “I have been so puzzled over the early labeling of [my] theory as
cognitive, ”he wrote, “that several years ago I set out to write another short
book to make it clear that I wanted no part of cognitive theory ”(1969,
p. 216). Despite his protests, Kelly ’s writings have become the starting point
for many of the approaches to personality we now identify as “cognitive.”The Cognitive Approach 411 |
PERSONAL CONSTRUCT THEORY
George Kelly ’s approach to personality begins with a unique conception of hu-
mankind. He called it a man-the-scientist perspective. Like scientists, people
constantly generate and test hypotheses about their world. Just as scientists try
to predict and control the things they study, we all want to predict and control
as many events in our lives as possible. Not knowing why things happen or
how the people around us might act can be unsettling. So to satisfy our need
for predictability, we engage in a process Kelly compared to template matching.
That is, our ideas about the world are similar to transparent templates. Weplace these templates over the events we encounter. If they match, we retain
the templates. If not, we modify them for a better prediction next time. For ex-
ample, based on past observations, you may have generated a few hypothesesabout one of your instructors. One hypothesis is that this man is stuffy and
arrogant. Whenever you see this instructor, you collect more information and
compare the new data with your hypothesis. If it is verified (the instructor actsthe way stuffy people act), you continue using it. If not (outside of the class-
room he is warm and charming), you discard the hypothesis and replace it
with a new one. The process resembles the one used by scientists who retainand reject hypotheses based on empirical findings.
Kelly called the cognitive structures we use to interpret and predict events
personal constructs . No two people use identical personal constructs, and no
two people organize their constructs in an identical manner. What do these
constructs look like? Kelly described them as bipolar. That is, we classify rele-
vant objects in an either or fashion within our constructs. When I meet
someone for the first time, I might apply the personal constructs friendly –
unfriendly, tall –short, intelligent –unintelligent, and masculine –feminine in
constructing an image of this person. I might decide that this person isfriendly, tall, intelligent, and feminine. But this does not mean that we see
the world as black and white with no shades of gray. After applying our first
construct, we often use other bipolar constructs to determine the extent ofthe blackness or whiteness. For example, after determining that this new
acquaintance is intelligent, I might then apply an academically intelligent –
commonsense intelligent construct to get an even clearer picture of what this
person is like.
How can personal constructs be used to explain personality? Kelly main-
tained that differences in personality result largely from differences in the way
people “construe the world. ”If you and I interact with Jacob, I might use
friendly –unfriendly, fun –boring, and outgoing –shyconstructs in forming my
impression. But you might interpret Jacob in terms of refined –gross, sensitive –
insensitive, and intelligent –unintelligent constructs. After we both talk to Jacob
for a while, I might act as if I ’m interacting with a friendly, fun, and outgoing
person. You might respond to Jacob as if dealing with a gross, insensitive, andunintelligent person. We ’re both in the same situation, but because we interpret
that situation very differently, we respond in very different ways. In addition,because I tend to use these same constructs when meeting other individuals, Iprobably have a characteristic way I interact with people that is different from“Id on o tr e g a r dm y
career in psychology
as a ‘calling. ’
Everything aroundus‘calls, ’if we
choose to heed. Itwas I who got myselfinto it and I who have
pursued it. ”
George Kelly412 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
yours. In other words, the relatively stable patterns in our behavior are the re-
sult of the relatively stable way we construe the world.
Personal Construct Systems
To get a rough idea of your own personal constructs, ask yourself what youtend to notice about people when you first meet them. The first few thoughts
that come to mind are probably some of the constructs you typically use to
make sense of other people and their behavior. It is also possible that two
people use the same constructs but construe the world differently. That is, I
might think someone intelligent, and you might see the same person as unin-
telligent. Further, two people ’s constructs might be similar on one pole but
not the other. I might use an outgoing –reserved construct, whereas you use
anoutgoing –melancholy construct. If that were the case, what I see as re-
served behavior you might see as sadness.
One reason you and I act differently from each other is that we use differ-
ent constructs. Another reason is that we organize our constructs differently.After I determine that a new acquaintance appears friendly, I might want toGeorge Kelly
1905 –1967
George Alexander Kelly was
born in a farming commu-nity near Wichita, Kansas, in1905. He attended FriendsUniversity in Wichita for
3 years before graduatingfrom Park College inMissouri in 1926. He was
an active member of the intercollegiate debate teamduring these years and developed a keen ability tochallenge arguments and conventional positions.Although these skills would eventually become anasset, they may have kept him away from the field ofpsychology for many years. Kelly described his first
psychology course as boring and unconvincing. Theinstructor spent considerable time discussing learningtheories, but Kelly was unimpressed. “The most I could
make of it was that the S was what you had to have in
order to account for the R, and the R was put there so
the S would have something to account for, ”he wrote.
“I never did find out what that arrow stood for ”(1969,
pp. 46 –47). He was also skeptical when he first read
Freud. “I don’t remember which one of Freud’ s books I
was trying to read, ”he recalled, “but I do rememberthe mounting feeling of incredulity that anyone could
write such nonsense, much less publish it ”(p. 47).
After graduating with a degree in physics and
mathematics, Kelly went to the University of Kansasto study educational sociology. After a series of odd
jobs, including teaching speech and working as anaeronautical engineer, he went to the University ofEdinburgh to study education in 1929. While there,he developed a growing interest in psychology andreceived his PhD in psychology from the University ofIowa a few years later.
Kelly spent the next 10 years at Fort Hays Kansas
State College. During this time he set up a network ofclinics to provide psychological services to the poor anddestitute Dustbowl victims of the 1930s. “I listened to
people in trouble, ”he wrote, “and tried to help them
figure out what they could do about it” (p. 50). He
soon came to see that what these people needed most
was an explanation for what had happened to them
and the ability to predict what would happen to them
in the future. Personal construct theory evolved fromthis insight. After serving in the Navy in World War II,Kelly spent a year at the University of Maryland andthen 20 years at Ohio State University. He moved toBrandeis University in 1965 and died soon after.
National Library of MedicinePersonal Construct Theory 413 |
know if the person is outgoing or quiet. We could diagram the relation be-
tween my constructs this way:
Friendly –Unfriendly
Outgoing– Quiet
Note that within this construct system, I could not see an unfriendly personas
either outgoing or quiet, just unfriendly. On the other hand, you might
use the same constructs but organize them this way:
Friendly –Unfriendly
Outgoing –Quiet Outgoing –Quiet
In this case, whether you judge someone as friendly or unfriendly, you canstill judge that person as either outgoing or quiet. Of course, it is also possible
to organize these same two constructs this way:
Outgoing –Quiet
Friendly –Unfriendly
Friendly –Unfriendly
In this case, after deciding someone is a quiet person, you might want to
know
if she is a quiet– friendly person or a quiet –unfriendly one. In short,
not only do we use a limitless number of constructs to make sense of ourworld, but the ways we organize and use these constructs also are practically
endless.
Psychological Problems
Like many personality theorists, Kelly was a practicing psychotherapist who
applied his ideas about personality to treating psychological problems.However, unlike many theorists, Kelly rejected the notion that psychological
disorders are caused by past traumatic experiences. Rather, he argued, people
suffer from psychological problems because of defects in their construct sys-
tems. Past experiences with an unloving parent or a tragic incident may ex-
plain why people construe the world as they do, but they are not the cause
of the person’ s problems.
Kelly placed anxiety at the heart of most psychological problems. We be-
come anxious when our personal constructs fail to make sense of the events
in our lives. We have all had this experience on occasion. An upcoming inter-view will cause more anxiety if you have no idea who you will meet or what
kind of questions you will be asked. Similarly, when you can ’t understand414 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
why certain people treat you the way they do or you don ’t know how to be-
have in certain situations, you probably feel confused, disoriented, and anxi-
ous. Relationship problems are particularly unsettling when you don’ t know
why things are going poorly and have no idea how to put the relationship
back on track.
The problem is that construct systems are never perfect. For a variety of
reasons, our constructs occasionally fail us. Most of the time, we simply gener-
ate a new construct to replace the inadequate one. If you anticipate that a con-versation with Anna is going to be boring but then find it interesting, you will
probably alter your expectations for future encounters with Anna. But failure
to consider this new information lessens your ability to predict what will hap-pen the next time you interact with Anna. You may have experienced this frus-
tration when you said to someone, “Ij u s td o n ’t understand you anymore. ”
COGNITIVE PERSONALITY VARIABLES
In the early days of behaviorism (Chapter 13), psychologists sometimes used
a“black box ”metaphor to describe the relationship between stimuli and re-
sponses. In this model, features in the environment (e.g., a loud noise) causebehaviors (e.g., running away). But what happens inside the organism be-
tween the stimulus and response is unknown and unknowable, i.e., the black
box. In contrast, it is exactly the elements between stimulus and response that
are of greatest interest to cognitive personality psychologists. In recent years,these psychologists have introduced a large number of cognitive variables to
account for individual differences (Mischel & Shoda, 1995, 2008; Shoda,
Tiernan, & Mischel, 2002). Some of these cognitive variables, sometimescalled cognitive-affective units, are shown in Table 15.1.
TABLE 15.1
Cognitive-Affective Units
Encodings Categories (constructs) for encoding informa-
tion about one ’s self, other people, events, and
situations
Expectations and Beliefs Expectations for what will happen in certain
situations, for outcomes for certain behaviors,
and for one ’s personal efficacy
Affects Feelings, emotions, and emotional responses
Goals and Values Individual goals and values, and life projects
Competencies and Self-RegulatoryPlansPerceived abilities, plans, and strategies forchanging and maintaining one ’s behavior and
internal states
Source: From Kelly, George, A. A Theory of Personality: The Psychology of Personal Constructs . Copyright
© 1955, 1963 by George A. Kelly, renewed 1983, 1991 by Gladys Kelly. Used by permission of W. W. Norton &
Company, Inc.Cognitive Personality Variables 415 |
These cognitive variables are part of a complex system that links the
situations we encounter with our behavior. An oversimplified illustration of
this process is shown in Figure 15.1. How we react to features in the environ-
ment, and even whether we notice these features, depends on our cognitive
structures. Once perceived, various mental representations, such as expecta-
tions, values and goals, interact with one another to determine how we respond
to the situation. Notice also that, as in some of the social learning models, ourbehavior can then affect the situation.
How do we explain individual differences within this cognitive frame-
work? The answer is that each of us possesses a different set of mental repre-sentations. In addition, how easily we access certain kinds of information
stored in memory varies from individual to individual. As a result, two people
often react to the same situation differently. What one person hears as aclever retort someone else might take as an insult. A Christmas tree will re-
mind one person of religious values, another of family and seasonal joy, and
a third of sad memories from childhood.
Schemas
Let’s now return to the scene at the beginning of this chapter —the one in
which my friend and I came away from the party with completely different
impressions. Although we were exposed to essentially the same people andi
jhgfedcbaFeatures of SituationsCognitive-
Affective
System
Behaviors
FIGURE 15.1 Cognitive Model of Personality
Source: From “A Cognitive-Affective System Theory of Personality: Reconceptualizing Situations, Disposition, Dynamics, and Invariance in
Personality Structure, ”by W. Mischel and Y. Shoda, Psychological Review , 1995, 102, 246 –148. Copyright © 1995 American Psychological
Association.416 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
events, our experiences were quite different. One explanation for our different
reactions is that my friend and I were using different schemas.
Schemas are hypothetical cognitive structures that help us perceive,
organize, process, and use information. Because there are so many stimuli to
attend to in most situations, we need some way to make sense of the mass
confusion around us. Imagine what the world must look like to a baby —
what psychologist William James once referred to as a “buzzing, blooming
confusion. ”The baby does not know what in all this confusion to pay atten-
tion to and what to ignore. Of course, the mass of stimuli doesn ’t go away.
Think about all the sounds and sights bombarding your senses at this verymoment. Fortunately, each of us has developed systems for identifying and at-tending to what is important and ignoring the rest.
One of the main functions of schemas is to help us perceive features in
our environment. Naturally, when something extremely important happensor someone possesses an attention-grabbing feature, everyone notices. If a 7-
foot-tall man attends a party, everyone makes note of his height. But less con-
spicuous features of an environment will probably not be noticed unless weenter the situation with a readiness to process that information. Actually, I
seldom notice how tall people are. However, a friend of mine is very aware
of other people’ s height (she is short). In schema terms, the reason she pays
attention to height is that she has a well-developed schema for processing
this information. Because I use different schemas to process information
about others, she and I often have different impressions of people.
Beyond helping to perceive certain features in our environment, schemas
provide us with a structure within which to organize and process informa-tion. For example, I can incorporate a new piece of information about my
mother into my existing knowledge of her because I have a well-defined
mother schema. I can give you a well-organized description of her because
the information is organized into one well-formed cognitive structure rather
than scattered about as bits of information in various unrelated schemas.
I also should be able to process information about my mother more readily
than information about a woman I have never met. When asked if my motheris sociable, I should be able to answer more quickly than if asked whether the
queen of England is sociable. Without a strong schema for the queen, it will
take me longer to process whatever information I might have about her.Moreover, because my mother schema provides me with a framework within
which to process and organize information, it is easier for me to use this in-formation. I should be able to recall information about my mother morereadily than information stored loosely in my memory.
COGNITIVE REPRESENTATIONS OF THE SELF
Of all the cognitive structures that organize and store memories, the most impor-
tant mental representation is probably the one most unique to you. Beginning at
a very early age, each of us develops a cognitive representation of ourselves.
Psychologists sometimes refer to this representation as our self-concept. As with
other personality constructs, researchers f ind that our self-concepts are relativelyCognitive Representations of the Self 417 |
stable over time (Markus & Kunda, 1986). Moreover, research indicates that
cognitive representations of the self p lay a central role in the way we process in-
formation and thus in how we interact with the world around us.
Self-Schemas
Surveys tell us that most Americans believe exercise is good for their physical
and mental health. The majority of adults periodically take up jogging, swim-
ming, aerobic dancing, or some other type of exercise program. However, alarge number of people rarely, if ever, exercise. And about half of those who
begin an exercise program quit within the first year. Why do some people
succeed in making exercise a part of their lives, whereas others fail? One ex-planation has to do with whether the would-be exerciser incorporates exercise
into his or her self-schema.
Self-schemas are cognitive representations of ourselves that we use to or-
ganize and process self-relevant information (Markus, 1977, 1983). Your self-
schema consists of the behaviors and attributes that are most important to
you. Because each part of your life is not equally important, not everythingyou do becomes part of your self-schema. If both you and I occasionally
play baseball and write poetry, we can ’t assume that these two activities play
an equally important role in our self-schemas. Baseball might be an important
part of how I think of myself, but not poetry, whereas the opposite might be
the case for you.
If you could see your self-schema, what would it look like? An example is
shown in Figure 15.2. Basic information about you makes up the core of yourself-schema. This includes your name, information about your physical ap-
pearance, and information about your relationships with significant people,
such as with a spouse or parents. Although different for each of us, these
SelfMemory
MenParentsWine
Clothing SchoolBasketballGeographyReligion
FIGURE 15.2 Example of a Self-Schema Diagram418 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
basic elements are found in nearly everyone ’s self-schema. More interesting
to personality psychologists are the unique features within your self-schema
(Markus & Sentis, 1982; Markus & Smith, 1981). Returning to the exercise
question, some people include athlete orphysically fit in their self-schemas.
Another way of saying this is that these i ndividuals consider their athletic
activities a part of who they are. Researchers find that people who incorpo-
rate such identities into their self-schemas are more likely to stick with regu-
lar exercise programs than those who do not (Kendzierski, 1988, 1990).When exercising becomes a part of who you are, you are much less likely
to give it up when the weather turns bad or you experience a few aches and
pains.
Trait concepts, such as independence or friendliness, can also be part of
your self-schema. That is, you might think of yourself as a friendly person. Ifthat is the case, you frequently evaluate your behavior by asking yourself,“Was that a friendly thing to do? ”However, it might never occur to me to
evaluate my actions in terms of friendliness. In this example, friendliness is a
feature of your self-schema, but not mine. Because the elements that consti-tute self-schemas vary from person to person, we process information about
ourselves differently. And because of these individual differences in self-
schemas, we behave differently. In one study, elementary school children
with prosocial as a part of their self-schemas were more likely to give valu-
able tokens to others than children who did not include prosocial as part of
their self-concepts (Froming, Nasby, & McManus, 1998). In another investi-gation, men and women whose self-schemas included sexuality reported
higher levels of sexual desire and stronger romantic attachments than those
whose self-schemas did not include sexuality (Andersen, Cyranowski, &
Espindle, 1999; Cyranowski & Andersen, 2000). One team of researchers
found that Latino Americans were more likely than White Americans to in-
clude simpatico (an interpersonal style emphasizing hospitality and gracious-
ness) in their self-schemas (Holloway, Waldrip, & Ickes, 2009). Participants
with simpatico as part of their self-schema were found to interact with others
in a warmer, more engaged style.
At this point, you may be asking how psychologists determine what a
person ’s self-schema looks like. Although examining something as abstract as
self-schemas presents a challenge, cognitive personality researchers have de-
veloped some creative procedures to test their hypotheses. Essentially, these
psychologists look at how people perceive and use information presented to
them. For example, answer the following question yes or no: Are you a com-petitive person? When faced with this question on a personality inventory,
some people answer immediately and decisively, whereas others have to pause
to think about what it means to be competitive and whether they possessthose qualities. In taking the various personality tests in this book, you prob-
ably found some items were easy to answer and some for which you simply
couldn ’t make up your mind. According to a self-schema analysis, the items
that were easy to answer are those for which you have a well-defined schema.
People who say yes immediately when asked if they are competitive have
a strong competitive schema that is part of their self-schema. The schemaCognitive Representations of the Self 419 |
enables them to understand the question and respond immediately. People
without a strong competitive schema are unable to process the information as
quickly.
Much of the early research on self-schemas was based on this reasoning.
Participants in one study were classified as possessing either a strong indepen-
dence schema or a strong dependence schema or as aschematic (Markus,
1977). Later these participants were presented with a series of adjectives ona computer screen. Their task was to press either a ME or a NOT ME buttonto indicate whether the adjective described them. Fifteen of the adjectives
were related to independence (for example, individualistic, outspoken ) and
15 to dependence (for example, conforming, submissive). As Figure 15.3
shows, people with strong independence schemas pressed the ME button
quickly on the independence-related adjectives but took longer to respond on
the dependence-related adjectives. Participants with strong dependence sche-
mas responded in the opposite pattern. Aschematics showed no difference in
making these judgments for any of the words. Researchers find similar results
when they divide participants along other personality dimensions (Shah &Higgins, 2001).
2.02.5Latency in SecondsDependent Adjectives Independent AdjectivesWords Judged Self-Descriptive
2.02.5Latency in SecondsDependent Adjectives Independent AdjectivesWords Judged Not Self-DescriptiveDependentsParticipants:
Aschematics
Independents
FIGURE 15.3 Mean Response Latencies for Adjectives
Source: From “Self-schemata and processing information about the self, ”by H. Markus, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology , 1977, 35, 63 –78. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological
Association.420 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
In addition to allowing for rapid processing of schema-relevant information,
self-schemas provide a framework for organizing and storing this information.Consequently, we would expect people to retrieve information from memory
more readily when they have a strong schema for a topic than when the
information is stored in a less organized manner. To test this hypothesis,
researchers presented college students with a series of 40 questions on a
computer screen (Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977). Participants answered each
question by pressing a YES or a NO button as quickly as possible. Thirty of thequestions were written so that people could answer easily without using their
self-schemas to process the information. For these questions, participants
simply answered whether a word was printed in big letters, whether it rhymedwith another word, or whether it meant the same thing as another word.
However, for 10 questions participants had to decide whether the word
described them. That is, they had to process the information through theirself-schemas.
What the participants were not told was that afterward they would be
asked to recall as many of the 40 words as possible. As shown in Figure 15.4,
when participants answered questions about themselves, they were more
likely to remember the information than when the question was processed
in other ways. The researchers point to this finding as evidence for a self-
schema. When asked whether a word describes them, participants processed
the question through their self-schemas. Because information in our self-
schemas is easy to access, the self-referent words were easier to remember
123Number of Words Recalled
Big
letters?Rhymes
with?Means
same as?Describes
you?
FIGURE 15.4 Mean Number of Words Recalled as a Function of
Cue Question
Source: From Rogers, et al. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology , 35, 677 –688. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological
Association.Cognitive Representations of the Self 421 |
than those not processed through self-schemas. But might this finding be ex-
plained in other ways? Could it be that the self-referent question was simplyharder than the other questions, thus causing participants to think about it
more? Apparently not. When people are asked if a word describes a celebrity,
they don’ t recall the words as well as when they are asked about themselves
(Lord, 1980).
Possible Selves
Suppose two college students, Denise and Carlos, receive an identical poor
grade in a course on deductive logic and argumentation. Neither of them is
pleased with the grade, but Denise quickly dismisses it as a bad semester,whereas Carlos frets about the grade for weeks. Denise turns her attention to
the next term, but Carlos looks over his final exam several times and thinks
about taking another course in this area. Although many explanations canbe suggested to account for the two students ’different reactions, a key piece
of information may be that Carlos is thinking about going to law school andbecoming a trial attorney someday, but Denise is not. A negative evaluationof his deductive logic and argumentation skills means something quite differ-
ent to Carlos than it does to Denise.
Our behavior is directed not only by cognitive representations of the way
we think of ourselves at the moment, but also by representations of what wemight become. You might think about a future self with a lot of friends, with
a medical degree, or with a physically fit body. Psychologists refer to these
images as our possible selves (Markus & Nurius, 1986; Ruvolo & Markus,
1992; Vignoles, Manzi, Regalia, Jemmolo, & Scabini, 2008). Possible selves
are cognitive representations of the kind of person we might become some-
day. These include roles and occupations we aspire to, such as police officer
or community leader, as well as the roles we fear we might fall into, such as
alcoholic or divorced parent. Possible selves also include the attributes we
think we might possess in the future, such as being a warm and loving per-
son, an overworked and underappreciated employee, or a contributor to soci-
ety. In a sense, possible selves represent our dreams and aspirations as well as
our fears and anxieties. Like other personality constructs, possible selves are
fairly stable over time (Frazier, Hooker, Johnson, & Kaus, 2000; Morfei,
Hooker, Fiese, & Cordeiro, 2001).
Possible selves serve two important functions (Markus & Nurius, 1986).
First, they provide incentives for future behavior. When making decisions,
we ask ourselves whether a choice will take us closer to or further awayfrom one of our future selves. A woman might enter an MBA program be-
cause this decision moves her closer to becoming her powerful business exe-
cutive possible self. A man might stop seeing old friends if he thinks the
association could lead him to the criminal self he fears he might become.
The second function of possible selves is to help us interpret the meaning
of our behavior and the events in our lives. A man with a professional base-
ball pitcher possible self will attach a very different meaning to an arm injury
than someone who does not think of himself this way. A woman with a pos-
sible self of cancer patient will react differently to small changes in her health422 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
than someone without this cognitive representation. In other words, we pay
more attention to and have a stronger emotional reaction to events that arerelevant to our possible selves.
Because possible selves guide many of our choices and reactions, they can
be useful in predicting future behavior. Researchers have used measures ofpossible selves to look at such varied behaviors as binge drinking (Quinlan,
Jaccard, & Blanton, 2006), academic performance (Oyserman et al., 2006),
weight loss (Granberg, 2006), and adherence to an exercise program(Ouellette, Hessling, Gibbons, Reis-Bergan, & Gerrard, 2005). Other research
has tied possible selves to problem behaviors. One team of investigators ex-
amined possible selves in juvenile delinquents (Oyserman & Markus, 1990;Oyserman & Saltz, 1993). Significantly, more than one-third of the juvenile
delinquents had developed a criminal possible self. In addition, very few of
these adolescents possessed possible selves for more conventional goals, such
as having a job. Because possible selves are indicative of our goals, fears, and
aspirations, we should not be surprised if many of these youthful offenders
were to become adult criminals.
Researchers also find gender differences in the possible selves of young
men and young women. In particular, female high school and college students
are less likely than males to see themselves in traditionally masculine roles in
the future. University women are less likely than men to have a possible self
that includes a career in a math, science or business (Lips, 2004). Male stu-
dents are less likely than women to see themselves in careers related to arts,
culture, and communication. However, research also points to solutions for
these gender discrepancies. High school girls who have female friends inter-
ested in science and who receive encouragement from their peers are more
likely to develop a scientist possible self (Stake & Nickens, 2005). Same-
gender role models also help. High school girls who see women scientistsand business leaders are more likely to incorporate these occupations intotheir possible selves. People are more optimistic about reaching their hoped-
for occupation when they know someone already in that position (Robinson,
Davis, & Meara, 2003).
Self Discrepancies
While reading the previous section, you may have been struck by the simi-
larities between possible selves and Carl Rogers’ description of the real-self
and ideal-self (Chapter 11). Rogers described the difficulties people face
when the person they are is very different from the person they would like
to be. Cognitive personality psychologists have also explored the relation
between different self-concepts. One approach, called self-discrepancy the-
ory, proposes three different cognitive representations of self (Higgins,
1987, 1989). First, each of us possesses an actual self. The actual self con-
tains all the information you have about the kind of person you are (or be-
lieve you are), similar to the notion of self-concept used by other personalitypsychologists. Second, you also possess an ideal self, w h i c hi sy o u rm e n t a l
image of the kind of person you would like to be. The ideal self includesyour dreams and aspirations and the goals you have set for yourself in life.Cognitive Representations of the Self 423 |
Third, there is the ought self. This is the self you believe you should be, the
kind of person who fulfills all the duties and obligations various sources
(parents, religion) have defined for you. Your ought self might be a devoted
parent, a patriotic citizen, or someone who gets involved in community
activities.
According to the theory, we often compare the way we act (our actual
self) with the way we want to be (ideal self) or the way we should be (ought
self). Not surprisingly, we often fall short in these comparisons.Discrepancies between our actual self and ideal self result in disappointment,
dejection, and sometimes sadness. This is the reaction of a would-be honors
student who becomes lax in his or her study habits and receives low grades.On the other hand, discrepancies between the actual self and the ought self
lead to agitation, anxiety, and guilt. These are the emotions we might expect
when we act selfishly or take advant age of someone, in contrast with the
generous and kind person we think we ought to be. As with other cognitive
processes, this comparison is said to take place outside of conscious aware-
ness. Thus you can experience sadness or guilt without being aware of whyyou feel that way.
Researchers find support for many of the predictions generated from
self-discrepancy theory. In particu lar, individuals made aware of a gap be-
tween their actual and ideal self often experience sadness, whereas those
with discrepant actual and ought selves suffer from anxiety (Cornette,
Strauman, Abramson, & Busch, 2008; Hardin & Lakin, 2009; Petrocelli &
Smith, 2005; Phillips & Silva, 2005; Renaud & McConnell, 2007).
Researchers also find that, like other p ersonality variables, measures of an
adult’s ideal-self and ought-self are fairly consistent over time (Strauman,
1996). Although little is known about how self-discrepancies affect
emotions in other cultures, one study found Japanese undergraduates had
larger discrepancies between their actual and ideal selves than did Canadianstudents (Heine & Lehman, 1999). Interestingly, this increase in self-
criticism by the Japanese students did not translate into higher levels of
depression.
APPLICATION: COGNITIVE ( BEHAVIOR) PSYCHOTHERAPY
The increased attention given to cognitive structures by personality research-
ers in recent years has been paralleled by the growing popularity of cognitive
approaches to psychotherapy. As described in Chapter 13, today many thera-pists combine cognitive approaches to therapy with procedures from traditional
behavioral therapies. But whether they call themselves cognitive therapists or
cognitive-behavior therapists, each identifies inappropriate thoughts as a causeof mood disorders and self-defeating behavior. People become anxious and
depressed because they harbor anxiety-provoking and depressing thoughts.
Consequently, the goal of most cognitive therapies is to help clients recognizeinappropriate thoughts and replace them with more appropriate ones. A cog-
nitive therapist ’s role usually falls somewhere between that of the intrusive
Freudian therapist and the Rogerian therapist who relies on the client for“The best scientist is
one who approaches
his subject [as] inti-
mately as a clinician…and the best clini-
cian is one who in-vites his client to joinhim in a controlled
investigation of life. ”
George Kelly424 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
clinical progress. Although clients must come to see how their cognitions
affect their emotions and behaviors, the therapist plays an active role in theprocess.
In addition to addressing current issues, cognitive psychologists often teach
clients how to deal with future and recurring problems (Meichenbaum &Deffenbacher, 1988). One cause of recurring problems is self-defeating think-
ing(Meichenbaum & Cameron, 1983). A man who suffers from shyness
probably approaches a party telling himself something like this: “I don ’t
know why I ’m going to this dumb party. No one ever wants to talk with me.
And when they do, I usually sound awkward and stupid. ”This man has set
himself up to fail. At the first awkward moment, he will conclude that thingsare going as poorly as anticipated. All the nervousness and embarrassment he
dreaded are likely to follow.
What can be done for this man? A cognitive therapist might try to re-
place these self-defeating thoughts with more appropriate, positive ones. This
is not to say the man should unrealistically expect that everything will go
well. Rather, he should be prepared for some disappointments and failuresand learn to interpret these in appropriate ways. Some psychologists compare
this process to inoculation. Like a medical vaccine that prevents a patient
from becoming ill, the treatment is designed to keep negative thoughts from
creating undue psychological distress.
Like any approach to treatment, cognitive psychotherapy does not
work for everyone and may be limited to psychological problems thatare based in irrational and self-defeat ing thinking. Nonetheless, the suc-
cess many therapists have had with t his approach has been encouraging
(Butler, Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006; Gaudiano, 2005). Cognitive-behavioral therapies have been found t o be especially effective for treat-
ing emotional disorders like depressi on and anxiety (Hollon, Stewart, &
Strunk, 2006; Vittengl, Clark, Dunn, & Jarrett, 2007). Not only do thesetreatments relieve the symptoms of e motional disorders, but people who
have gone through cognitive-behavioral therapies are less likely to experi-ence a relapse in the future. Presumably these individuals have learnedhow to identify unhealthy thoughts and how to replace them with posi-
tive ones.
Rational Emotive Therapy
One of the earliest advocates of cogni tive therapy was Albert Ellis, who de-
veloped rational emotive therapy . Consistent with the tendency to blend
cognitive and behavioral treatments, r ecently Ellis referr ed to his approach
asrational emotive behavior therapy (Ellis, 2003). According to Ellis, peo-
ple become depressed, anxious, upse t, and the like because of faulty rea-
soning and a reliance on irrational be liefs. Ellis described this as an A-B-C
process. For example, suppose your boyfriend/girlfriend calls tonight and
tells you the relationship is over . This is the A, which Ellis calls the
Activating experience . However, when clients seek out psychotherapy,
they usually identify the reason as the C, the emotional Consequence .I n
this case, you are probably depressed, guilty, or angry. But how did youApplication: Cognitive (Behavior) Psychotherapy 425 |
logically get from A to C? Why should a personal setback or loss cause
such strong negative emotions? The answer is that you have used a middlestep in this sequence, B —the irrational Belief. The only way you could
logically conclude from breaking up with your partner that you should be
depressed is that you are also saying to yourself something like “It is nec-
essary for me to be loved and approved by virtually every person in my
life,”or“I can never be happy without this person.” Of course, when iso-
lated like this, the belief is obviously irrational. But these irrational beliefs
are so entrenched in our thoughts that it often takes professional help tosee the flaws in our thinking.
Ellis maintained that each of us harbors and relies on a large number of
these irrational beliefs. Imagine that you fail an important class (A). If youthen fall back on the irrational belief “I need to do well at everything to be
considered worthwhile ”(B), you’ ll lead yourself to the conclusion that this is
a catastrophe and therefore become excessively anxious (C). A rational emo-
tive therapist would point out that, whereas failing is certainly an unfortunate
event—and something you ’d prefer didn ’t happen —it does not warrant ex-
treme anxiety. Expecting everything to work out well all the time will only
lead to disappointment and frustration. Some of the more commonly used ir-
rational beliefs are listed in Table 15.2. Ellis (1987) maintained that some of
these beliefs are blatantly irrational and therefore easily identified and cor-
rected during therapy. However, other beliefs are more subtle or trickier and
thus are more resistant to change.
The goal of rational emotive therapy is twofold. First, clients must see
how they rely on irrational beliefs and thereby identify the fault in their rea-soning. Second, the therapist works with the client to replace irrational beliefs
with rational ones. For example, instead of deciding that your romantic
breakup is a reason to be depressed, you might tell yourself that, although
you enjoy a stable romantic relationship and wish this one could have con-tinued, you know that not all relationships work out. You also know that
this doesn ’t mean no one else can love you or that you are never going
to have a good relationship again. Thus, whereas the A statement is the
same—“I broke up with my partner ”—the B statement is different. Because
the situation is identified as unpleasant but not catastrophic, there is no needto become overly depressed, the old C.
In the following sample, taken from one of Ellis ’therapy sessions
with a young woman (Ellis, 1971), you can see how a rational emotive thera-pist tries to change faulty thoughts:
Client: Well, this is all a part of something that’ s bothered me for a long time. I ’m always
afraid of making a mistake.
Ellis: Why? What ’s the horror?
C: I don’t know.
E: You’re saying that you ’re a bitch, you ’re a louse when you make a mistake.426 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
C: But this is the way I ’ve always been. Every time I make a mistake, I die a thousand
deaths over it.
E: You blame yourself. But why? What ’s the horror? Is it going to make you better next
time? Is it going to make you make fewer mistakes?
C: No.
E: Then why blame yourself? Why are you a louse for making a mistake? Who said so?
C: I guess it ’s one of those feelings I have.
E: One of those beliefs. The belief is: “I am a louse! ”And then you get the feeling: “Oh,
how awful! How shameful! ”But the feeling follows the belief. And again, you ’re say-
ing,“I should be different; I shouldn’ t make mistakes! ”instead of, “Oh, look: I made
a mistake. It ’s undesirable to make mistakes. Now, how am I going to stop making
one next time? ”…
C: It might all go back to, as you said, the need for approval. If I don ’t make mistakes,
then people will look up to me. If I do it all perfectly —TABLE 15.2
Some Common Irrational Beliefs
Obvious Irrational Beliefs
Because I strongly desire to perform important tasks competently and successfully, I
absolutely must perform them well at all times.
Because I strongly desire to be approved by people I find significant, I absolutely
must always have their approval.
Because I strongly desire people to treat me considerately and fairly, they absolutely
must at all times and under all conditions do so.
Because I strongly desire to have a safe, comfortable, and satisfying life, the condi-
tions under which I live absolutely must at all times be easy, convenient, andgratifying.
Subtle and Tricky Irrational Beliefs
Because I strongly desire to perform important tasks competently and successfully,
and because I want to succeed at them only some of the time, I absolutely mustperform these tasks well.
Because I strongly desire to be approved by people I find significant, and because I
only want a little approval from them, I absolutely must have it.
Because I strongly desire people to treat me considerately and fairly, and because I
am almost always considerate and fair to others, they absolutely must treat me well.
Because I strongly desire to have a safe, comfortable, and satisfying life, and be-
cause I am a nice person who tries to help others lead this kind of life, the condi-tions under which I live absolutely must be easy, convenient, and gratifying.
Source: From “The impossibility of achieving consistently good mental health, ”by A. Ellis, American
Psychologist , 1987, 42, 364 –375. Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association.Application: Cognitive (Behavior) Psychotherapy 427 |
E: Yes, that ’s part of it. That is the erroneous belief: that if you never make mistakes ev-
erybody will love you and that it is necessary that they do. …But is it true? Suppose
you never did make mistakes —would people love you? They ’d sometimes hate your
guts, wouldn ’t they?
Rational emotive therapists challenge clients to identify their irrational beliefs
and see how these beliefs lead them to their faulty conclusions. Of course, thisis not easy. Most of us can readily identify what ’s wrong with our friends ’
thinking, but it ’s quite another matter when we ’re the ones with an emotional
problem. Nonetheless, the success of rational emotive therapy with a largenumber of clients has contributed to the increased popularity of cognitive
approaches to psychotherapy in recent years.
ASSESSMENT: THE REPERTORY GRID TECHNIQUE
George Kelly made personal constructs the key concept in his theory of
personality as well as the focus of his approach to psychotherapy. But this
emphasis created a bit of a problem. Specifically, how does one go about
measuring a person’ s personal constructs? Of course, a therapist might obtain
some idea of a client ’s construct system during the course of therapy sessions.
But Kelly and his colleagues needed a more efficient way to examine con-struct systems that could then be communicated fairly easily to the client.Kelly’s answer was the Repertory Grid Technique. Kelly and his followers de-
veloped several variations of this technique (Fransella, Bell, & Bannister,2003), but the essential procedure consists of two steps (Bell, 1990). First,the test taker creates a list of elements. The items on this list can be anything
the individual encounters in life, but most often the list consists of specificpeople the test taker knows. Second, the test taker’ s personal constructs are
elicited by comparing and contrasting various elements on the list.
The most common version of the grid technique is the Role Construct
Repertory Test, or more commonly, the Rep Test. A shortened version of
the basic Rep Test procedure is presented on pages 433 –434. Therapists
begin by asking clients to provide a list of 24 people from various personal
experiences —for example, a teacher they liked, the most interesting person
they know, and so on. The therapist then presents clients with three of the
names from this list and asks, “In what important way are two of these peo-
ple alike but different from the third?” A client might say that two of them
arewarm people and that the third person is cold. In Kelly ’s terms, this client
has used a warm– cold construct to categorize the three people. The process is
repeated with three different names from the list. Perhaps this time the client
will divide the people along an outgoing –shyor a generous –miserly construct.
Kelly suggested that about 20 trials, or “sorts,”provide the therapist with a
useful sample of the client ’s principal constructs.
In one variation of the Rep Test, the therapist takes away one of the three
names and replaces it with a new one. This procedure can be useful in identi-fying clients ’difficulties in applying new constructs to new situations. To ex-
amine self-concepts, therapists sometimes present the client ’s name along with428 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
two names from the list. Again, clients are asked how two of the three are
alike and one is different. Many therapists take the list of constructs gener-
ated from the client ’s initial Rep Test and ask the client to evaluate each per-
son on the list according to the construct. This step creates a grid similar to
the one shown in Table 15.3 and allows the therapist and client to look for
patterns across a broad set of information.
The Repertory Grid Technique has been widely used by therapists and
clinical psychologists to obtain a visual map of how clients and thosesuffering from various psychological disorders construe the world (Feixas,Erazo-Caicedo, Harter, & Bach, 2008; Winter, 2003). But the grid technique
has also been used by researchers when studying such diverse topics as com-
munication within a large organization (Coopman, 1997), teaching effective-ness (Chitsabesan, Corbett, Walker, Spencer, & Barton, 2006), profiles of
specific criminal types (Horley, 1996), and career counseling (Savickas,
1997). One reviewer counted more than 3,000 studies using variations of the
Repertory Grid Technique (Neimeyer, 2001).
Like other assessment procedures, the grid technique also has its limita-
tions. One concern is that, unlike other personality measures, the RepertoryGrid Technique does not generate a simple test score (Horley, 1996).
Although various number-generating systems have been developed, the proce-
dure still allows for a large degree of interpretation on the part of the thera-
pist. Another limitation concerns the many assumptions underlying the test.
One assumption when using the Rep Test is that the constructs clients provide
are not limited to the people on the list but also would apply to new people in
new situations. Another assumption is that the constructs elicited during the
test have some degree of permanence. That is, we assume clients are not using
these constructs for the first time in the testing session and never again. A re-
lated assumption is that the people on the list are representative of the kind of
people clients are likely to deal with in their daily lives. Constructs used only
for unique people that clients rarely encounter are of little use in understand-ing how clients deal with the majority of people with whom they interact.TABLE 15.3
Sample Grid
Mom Dad Sister Brother Boss Neighbor Friend Coworker
Pleasant P U U U U U P U Unpleasant
Trustworthy U U U U U U ? U Untrustworthy
Competitive N N C C ? N C C Not CompetitiveWarm W C C C C C ? C Cold
Intelligent N I I I I ? N I Not IntelligentFun D D D D D D F D DullAssessment: The Repertory Grid Technique 429 |
ASSESSING YOUR OWN PERSONALITY
Personal Constructs
To begin, write down the names of the following 12 people. Although a
person may fit more than one category, you need to compile a list of 12
different people. If there is no one who fits a category, name someone whois similar to the category description. For example, if you do not have a
brother, select someone who is like a brother to you.
1. A teacher you liked
2. A teacher you disliked
3. Your wife (husband) or boyfriend (girlfriend)
4. An employer, supervisor, or officer you found hard to get
along with
5. An employer, supervisor, or officer you liked
6. Your mother
7. Your father
8. Brother nearest your age
9. Sister nearest your age
10. A person with whom you have worked who was easy to get
along with
11. A person with whom you have worked who was hard to
understand
12. A neighbor with whom you get along well
Next, take three of these people at a time, as indicated by the numbers inthe following list. Then describe in what important way two of them are
alike but different from the third. Put a word or phrase describing the
two alike people in the Construct list and a description of the remaining
person in the Contrast list.
Names Construct Contrast
3, 6, 7
1, 4, 10
4, 7, 8
1, 6, 9
4, 5, 8
2, 11, 12
8, 9, 10
(continues)430 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
But the most precarious assumption made by test givers, according to
Kelly, is that people are ableto describe the constructs they use. Unfortunately,
the grid technique is subject to the inherent limits of our language. Although
clients may supply words that come close to what they mean, these words
may be inadequate. Kelly did not assume that words necessarily exist for
describing all constructs. In fact, he described “preverbal ”constructs, those
developed before we learn to speak. And even when clients do use appropriatewords, therapists may interpret those words differently. For example, a client ’s
definition of aggressive may be quite different from a therapist ’s. In this case
the therapist may still end up with a false impression of how the client viewsthe world.
STRENGTHS AND CRITICISMS OF THE COGNITIVE APPROACH
Strengths
One strength of the cognitive approach to personality is that many of the ideasevolved out of and were developed through empirical research findings. Mostof the cognitive structures used to account for individual differences have been
subjected to extensive investigation in controlled laboratory experiments. In
many cases personality psychologists have borrowed ideas and research proce-
dures from social and cognitive psychologists investigating similar phenomena.
Moreover, cognitive models of personality have been modified as investigatorsNames Construct Contrast
2, 3, 5
5, 7, 11
1, 10, 12
This is an abbreviated version of Kelly’ s Rep Test (the Minimum Context
Form). The test provides a quick idea of the constructs you use to organize
information about the people you know and meet. You may want to com-
pare your responses with those of other test takers. No doubt you will find a
few overlapping constructs, but also many you hadn ’t thought of. Of course,
these differences in personal constructs represent differences in personality
that should translate into individual differences in your behavior.
Scale: The Role Construct Repertory (Rep) Test
Source: The Role Construct Repertory (REP) Test from A Theory of Personality: The Psychology of
Personal Constructs by George A. Kelly. Copyright © 1955, 1963 by George A. Kelly, renewed
1983, 1991 by Gladys Kelly. Used by permission of W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.Strengths and Criticisms of the Cognitive Approach 431 |
learn more about cognitive structures and processes through their ongoing
research.
Another strength of the cognitive approach is that it fits well with the
current mood, or Zeitgeist, of psychology. The number of journal articles
and doctoral dissertations examining cognitive concepts has risen dramati-
cally over the past few decades. Researchers in other areas of psychology,
such as developmental and social psychologists, are working on related lines
of research that often complement and extend what is known from the cogni-tive personality perspective.
Related to the preceding point, cognitive approaches to psychotherapy have
become particularly popular in recent years. Even therapists who identify withother approaches to personality sometimes incorporate aspects of cognitive
therapy in their practice. A survey of practitioners in the Association for the
Advancement of Behavior Therapy, a group originally composed of behaviortherapists, found that 67% described their therapy orientation as “cognitive be-
havioral ”(Elliott et al., 1996). Nearly half said they occasionally use rational
emotive therapy with their clients.
Criticisms
A frequent criticism of the cognitive approach is that the concepts are some-times too abstract for empirical research. What exactly is a “personal con-
struct”or a“possible self ”? How do we know if a schema is being used?
How many schemas are there, and how are they related? More important,
how can we study their influence on behavior if we can ’t agree on clear oper-
ational definitions? Some of the answers may come with more research, but
the nature of cognitions probably renders them more nebulous than manyconstructs used by personality theorists.
A related question is whether we need to introduce these concepts to
account for individual differences in be havior. For example, strict behavior-
ists might argue that they can explain the same phenomena with fewer con-
structs. Introducing schemas or possible selves may be unnecessary and
perhaps even an obstacle to understanding personality. Applying the law ofparsimony, it is incumbent upon cognitive theorists to demonstrate how
their approach can explain personality better than other, less complicated
approaches.
Another source of concern about the cognitive approach to personality
is that there is no single model to organize and guide theory and research.Basic questions about how various cognitive structures relate to one another
and to other aspects of information processing, such as memory, remain
unanswered. A related problem concer ns the relationship between the vari-
ous cognitive structures different theorists have introduced. Is a personal
construct different from a schema? A comprehensive model would help re-
searchers understand precisely what these terms mean and how they are
related.432 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
SUMMARY
1. The cognitive approach to personality describes consistent behavior pat-
terns in terms of the way people process information. George Kelly was
an early pioneer in this approach with his personal construct theory.
Kelly maintained that we are motivated to make sense out of our world.
He compared people to scientists, always striving for better predictions
about what will happen to them. Kelly described the cognitive structures
we use in this regard as personal constructs. He maintained that psycho-
logical problems stem from anxiety, which results from a person ’s inabil-
ity to predict events.
2. Psychologists have described a number of cognitive structures to help
explain individual differences and intrapersonal processes. Schemas arecognitive structures that help us perceive, organize, and store
information.
3. Perhaps the most important cognitive structures for personality psy-
chologists are the cognitive representations we have for our selves.
Much research in this area is concerned with self-schemas. Studies
demonstrate that we perceive information more readily and recall itbetter when it is relevant to our self-schemas. Researchers also find that
cognitive representations of future selves guide our behavior, but that
discrepancies between different self-concepts can result in negative
emotions.
4. Cognitive approaches to psychotherapy have become increasingly popu-
lar in the last few decades. These therapies focus on changing the clients ’
thoughts. Albert Ellis, an early advocate of this approach, argued that
people have emotional problems when they use irrational beliefs.
Rational emotive therapy helps clients see how they use these beliefs
and how to replace them with more rational ones.
5. Kelly introduced the Repertory Grid Technique to measure individual
differences in personal constructs. In one example, test takers typicallydevelop a list of people in their lives and then divide these people into
various categories. This procedure helps therapists see the constructs
clients use to make sense of the world. Kelly acknowledged severalassumptions behind this approach, including that people can adequately
communicate the constructs they use.
6. Among the strengths of the cognitive approach is its strong empirical
background. The cognitive approach also fits nicely with the current
trend in psychology toward cogniti ve explanations of behavior. Some
critics of the cognitive approach have complained that many of the
concepts used by cognitive theor ists are too abstract. Others have
questioned whether it is always neces sary to introduce cognitions to
explain behavior. The cognitive approach also suffers from the lack ofa general model to organize all of the work that falls under this
approach.Summary 433 |
KEY TERMS
personal constructs (p. 412)
possible selves (p. 422)
rational emotive therapy (p. 425)
schema (p. 417)
self-schema (p. 418)
MEDIA RESOURCES
Visit our website. Go to www.cengage.com/
psychology/burger to find online resources
directly linked to your book, including quizzing,
glossary, flashcards, and more.434 CHAPTER 15 The Cognitive Approach |
CHAPTER16
The Cognitive Approach
Relevant Research
Cognitions and Aggression
Gender, Memory, and Self-ConstrualCognitions and Depression
Summary
435 |
If you think back to the first chapter, you may recall the story about the blind
men trying to describe an elephant. The point was that obtaining a completeunderstanding of human personality requires that we examine personality
from several different perspectives. Although each perspective offers useful in-
formation, each also provides only a limited view of this complex topic. That
lesson is clearly illustrated in this chapter. Each program of research examines
from a cognitive perspective a topic covered elsewhere in the book. It ’sn o t
that the research covered earlier is wrong or needs updating. Rather, the point
is that we need to examine important topics from more than one perspective if
we want to obtain a complete picture. We ’ll begin with aggression, a topic
we’ve looked at in depth from a psychoanalytic and behavioral/social learning
perspective. Next, we ’ll return to the topic of gender. In addition to the many
gender differences we ’ve touched on throughout the book, researchers find
that men and women also differ in the way they remember information.
Finally, we ’ll examine cognitive explanations for depression, another topic that
has surfaced often in this book. Investigators find that the way we process in-
formation plays a crucial role in this psychological disorder. Psychologists alsolook at cognitive styles to identify those who might be vulnerable to future
bouts of depression.
COGNITIONS AND AGGRESSION
Imagine you are strolling alone through a park. Two teenage boys walking
about 30 feet behind you suddenly quicken their pace and draw closer. What
is your reaction? Perhaps the boys are in a hurry to get somewhere. Perhaps
they are simply more energetic and walk faster than you do. Maybe they are
interested in catching up to you to ask for the time or directions. Or maybe
they want to harm you. This situation, like many we encounter, contains a
fair degree of ambiguity, and people react to it differently.
How you respond to this situation depends on how you interpret it.
Whether you see the circumstances as threatening, annoying, or benign willcause you to run away, prepare to fight, or move out of the way. This ex-ample illustrates a key concept that cognitive researchers rely on when try-
ing to predict aggressive behavior. It ’s not enough to know that a person is
high in aggressiveness, has just witnessed someone model aggression, or has
a history of being rewarded for violence. Although all of these variables play
a role, a full understanding of aggressive behavior requires that we also ex-
amine the cognitions that come into play when people encounter a poten-
tially threatening or dangerous situation (Anderson & Huesmann, 2003;
Crick & Dodge, 1994; Fontaine & Dod ge, 2006; Wilkowski & Robinson,
2008).
General Aggression Model
Whether a potentially violent encounter passes uneventfully or leads to ag-
gression depends on a large number of factors. To explain this process, one
team of psychologists have combined decades of research findings into a436 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
General Aggression Model (Anderson & Bushman, 2002a). As shown in
Figure 16.1, the model begins with a social encounter with the potential for
triggering aggression. This might be an insult, a threat, a shove, or any action
that could be interpreted along these lines. How we respond to the event de-
pends first on the kind of person we are and the kind of situation we are in.
Because of differences in traits, attitudes, past experiences, genetic predisposi-
tions, etc., some people are more prone to aggression than others. Moreover,
some situations lend themselves to violence more than others. Aggression ismore likely when the situation includes provocation, frustration and elements
that we associate with violence (visual cues, words, noises).
But psychologists looking at the process from a cognitive perspective
argue that these personal and situational factors are important only to theextent that they are tied to and activate aggression-relation thoughts and
emotions. Some people are more prone to making these cognitive associa-tions than others. For a highly aggressi ve person, even a mild insult can gen-
erate hostile thoughts and anger. Cog nitive psychologists would say that
these individuals have a well-developed and easily accessible network of
Social Encounters
Person
Hostile Thoughts
and Emotions
Aggression Scripts
Appraisal
and
DecisionSituation
Thoughtful
ActionImpulsive
Action
FIGURE 16.1 General Aggression Model
Source: Adapted from Anderson and Bushman (2002a).Cognitions and Aggression 437 |
hostile cognitions. Aspects of the situation that the person associates with
aggression activate these hostile cogniti ons. These situational variables in-
clude visual aggressive cues, such as weapons, fists, and blood, but also in-
clude any sounds, smells, or sights that the person cognitively links to
hostile thoughts.
People recently exposed to violent images, such as scenes from violent mo-
vies or violent video games, also have highly accessible hostile cognitions.
Similarly, frustrated people are likely to have highly accessible hostile thoughts.Individuals who frequently expose themselves to violent images may have
chronically accessible hostile cognitions. In a sense, these people move through
their worlds ready to think aggressively about whatever they might encounter.
Among the hostile cognitions potentially activated in this process are ag-
gressive behavior scripts . These are potential patterns of behavior that have
been learned and sometimes practiced. In many cases, the aggressive scripthas been learned by watching aggressive models. Even though you may never
have punched someone in the face, you have seen the behavior modeled often
enough in your life that you probably could act out this script if you needed to.Aggressive scripts that have been practiced are even more likely to be acted out.
People can practice aggressive scripts simply by imagining themselves taking
an aggressive action or by reliving in their minds a violent sequence they may
have experienced or seen. People also practice aggressive scripts when they
physically rehearse the behavior, such as during karate drills or target practice.
The more accessible the behavior script, the more likely the individual will act
aggressively.
This cognitive analysis helps us understand why violent movies and tele-
vision programs sometimes lead to aggression. Recall from Chapter 14 thatpeople often imitate acts of aggression. However, researchers find that partici-
pants frequently act in violent ways not modeled in the movie or program.
Psychologists explain this effect in terms of hostile thoughts and emotionsthat are primed by the violent images (Anderson & Huesmann, 2003). When
violent memories and thoughts are primed and therefore highly accessible, the
likelihood that the viewer will act aggressively increases. Consistent with thisanalysis, a large number of studies find exposure to violent movies or violent
video games increases aggressive thoughts (Carlson, Marcus-Newhall, &
Miller, 1990; Todorov & Bargh, 2002). Even exposure to songs with violentlyrics has been found to increase aggressive thoughts and feelings (Anderson,
Carnagey, & Eubanks, 2003; Fischer & Greitemeyer, 2006).
Aggressive cognitions not only trigger aggressive behavior scripts, they also
affect the way we interpret situations. Returning to the example of the teenageboys in the park, if hostile thoughts and emotions were easily accessible (per-
haps you just came from a violent movie), you might very well interpret thesituation as a threat, perhaps even one that called for an aggressive reaction
on your part. However, psychologists are quick to point out that a great deal
of aggression takes place in a more-or-less automatic or impulsive manner(Berkowitz, 2008; Fontaine & Dodge, 2006). Sometimes when someone steps
on our toes or bumps us from behind we respond without thinking. In these in-
stances, highly accessible hostile cognitions can be particularly dangerous.438 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
Reactive Aggression in Boys
Common observation confirms what researchers know about aggression in
adolescents and preadolescents. Boys are much more likely to engage in phys-
ical acts of aggression than girls (Card, Stucky, Sawalani, & Little, 2008).
Needless to say, this aggression creates numerous problems for the aggressive
boys as well as for those around them. Researchers are particularly interested
in boys who exhibit reactive aggression , i.e., angry, hostile aggression in re-
sponse to frustration or provocation. These are the boys who react to a little
teasing by threatening to beat up the teaser and who turn an accidental bump
in the hallway into a fist fight.
To better understand these reactions, some psychologists examine the
way these boys interpret potentially aggression-provoking situations. Oneteam of researchers presented a series of hypothetical situations to boys with
a history of reactive aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1996). For example, in onescenario another student breaks the boy’ s radio while the boy is out of the
room. For each situation, the boys were asked why the other student did
what he did and whether the act was intentional. As shown in Figure 16.2,
the boys with a history of reactive aggression were more likely to see the act
as intentional and hostile than were nonaggressive boys. Another way to say
this is that the aggressive boys had chronically accessible hostile thoughts
that led them to interpret harmless acts as threatening. Not surprisingly,
researchers find that these types of interpretations often lead to aggression
(Dodge, 2006; Dodge et al., 2003).
If the attributions some boys make for unintentional acts are responsible
for reactive aggression, then programs designed to reduce physical aggression
Attri butionMore
Intentional
Less
Intentional Grade Level3/4Aggressive BoysNonaggressive Boys
5/69
87
6
5
FIGURE 16.2 Mean Attribution Score
Source: Adapted from Crick and Dodge (1996).Cognitions and Aggression 439 |
in schools could benefit from focusing on those attributions. This was the ap-
proach taken by one team of investigators (Metropolitan Area Child StudyResearch Group, 2007). The researchers first identified elementary school
children who were at risk for acting violently. Beginning in Grades 2 and 3,
some of these children attended 40 one-hour sessions over a 2-year period
in which they learned, among other things, how to respond in effective, non-
violent ways to conflict and threats. Other children were randomly assigned
to a control group that did not attend the sessions. At the beginning and atthe end of the 2-year study, the researchers presented the children with sce-
narios and asked the children how they would probably react to the situa-
tions. Among the children who came from moderate-income neighborhoods,there was a tendency for those in the control group to become more aggres-
sive as they became older. However, the children in the intervention condi-
tion showed a decrease in aggressive tendencies over the same periodof time.
GENDER, MEMORY, AND SELF-CONSTRUAL
The next time you want to stir things up at a dull social gathering, raise thisquestion: Do men or women have better memories? Inevitably, I find peoplecome to the defense of their own gender. Men complain about times their
spouses forgot to pay bills or fill the gas tank, and women point out the way
their husbands overlook anniversaries and the names of in-laws. Although
these responses reflect more than an ounce of stereotype, they also highlight
observations psychologists make about gender and memory. Research sug-
gests men and women do not differ in their general ability to memorize andrecall information. However, investigators often find differences in what men
and women remember.
Consider a study in which men and women were asked to recall several
different kinds of information (Seidlitz & Diener, 1998). Participants first
were given 3 minutes to list as many positive and negative events as they
could recall from the previous 3 years of their lives. Later the participantswere asked to recall, among other things, emotional events from the previous
year and from a randomly selected 1-hour interval from the previous week.
Participants were also given a limited amount of time to recall events fromAmerican history.
Who had the better recall? As shown in Figure 16.3, the answer depends
on what kind of information the participants were asked to remember. Thewomen recalled significantly more personal events than the men. This was
true for both negative events and positive events. On the other hand, men
did better recalling the impersonal information about American history. In
short, women were better able to remember happy occasions with friends
and times they embarrassed themselves, whereas men recalled better the facts
they had learned in school or read about.
Psychologists explain these differences in memory by pointing to the way
people process self-relevant information. Specifically, investigators identify
two differences in the way men and women organize information in memory.440 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
First, the genders differ in the extent to which self-relevant information is as-
sociated with emotions. Second, men and women differ in the extent to whichinformation about themselves is connected with information about personal
relationships.
Emotional Memories
From an early age, females learn to pay attention to their emotions and the
emotions of others. Consequently, women are more likely than men to en-code information about themselves in terms of emotions (Bloise & Johnson,
2007; Feldman Barrett, Lane, Sechrest, & Schwartz, 2000; Kuebli, Butler, &
Fivush, 1995). And if women organize their memories around emotions, we
should not be surprised to find that they are better able to recall both positive
and negative emotional experiences (Fujita, Diener, & Sandvik, 1991). Memories
for both happy and sad experiences should be more accessible for women.Moreover, the cognitive link between one emotional memory and another
should be stronger for women than men. Thus recalling one sad experience is
likely to trigger another sad memory for women, but perhaps not for men.
Number of Events Recalled
Past
3 Y earsPast
Ye arPast
WeekMenWomen16
1514
13
121110
Personal Experiences 9
87
6
Historical
Facts
FIGURE 16.3 Number of Events Recalled
Source: Adapted from “Sex differences in the recall of affective experience, ”by L. Seidlitz and E. Diener,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 74, 1998, p. 262 –271. Copyright © 1998 by the American
Psychological Association. Adapted with permission of the author.Gender, Memory, and Self-Construal 441 |
These gender differences were demonstrated in a study in which adult
men and women were asked to recall childhood experiences (Davis, 1999).
Participants were cued with a series of emotional words and phrases, such as
“feeling rejected” or“getting something you really wanted. ”As shown in
Figure 16.4, women recalled more emotional memories from childhood than
did men. Moreover, this was true for each emotion examined, whether positive
or negative. When the investigator compared similar recall in male and femalestudents in grades 3, 5, 8, and 11, she found a similar pattern. That is, regard-
less of age, females are better able to recall emotional memories than males.
Interestingly, the researcher found no gender differences when men and womenwere asked to recall nonemotional memories.
This tendency for women to recall more emotional memories could help
explain why women suffer from depression more often than men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001). Not only do women remember sad experiences more often
than men, but recalling one unhappy incident is also likely to activate memo-
ries about other sad events.
Memories About Relationships
Another line of research looks at the extent to which men and women consider
relationships when they organize self-relevant information. Drawing from the
work on individualist and collectivist cultures (Chapter 1), some psychologists
argue that the way men and women are raised in our society causes them to
form different cognitive representations of themselves (Cross & Madson, 1997).
Men are said to develop independent self-construals .T h a ti s ,m e n ’ss e l f -
concepts are relatively unrelated to the cognitive representations they have for510152025
Guilty Self-Conscious Fear Angry Sad HappyNumber of Memories Recalled
Recall ConditionsWomen
Men
FIGURE 16.4 Recall of Emotional Childhood Memories
Source: From “Gender differences in autobiographical memory of childhood emotional experience, ”by
P. J. Davis, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1999, 76, 498 –510. Copyright © 1999 by the
American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission of the American Psychological Association.442 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
other people. On the other hand, women in our society tend to develop inter-
dependent self-construals . Their self-concepts are highly related to the cognitive
representations they have of others and their relationships with those people.
In particular, these cognitive representations are tied to those with whom
women feel close and personal relations (Gabriel & Gardner, 1999).
Put another way, relationships with friends and loved ones are an impor-
tant part of how women think of themselves. It ’s not just that they enjoy their
relationships more than men, but rather that women are more likely to define
themselves in terms of the relationships they share with others. Returning to
the memory data, perhaps one reason women recall certain kinds of experi-
ences more readily than men is that these events may have involved otherpeople. Because of their interdependent self-construal, information involving
relationships is more accessible for women than for men.
Consistent with this analysis, several studies find women are more likely than
men to define themselves in terms of their relationships (Guimond, Chatard,
ASSESSING YOUR OWN PERSONALITY
Self-Construal
Indicate the extent to which you agree with the following statements.
Use a 7-point scale to indicate your response, with 1 ¼Strongly disagree and
7¼Strongly agree.
1. My close relationships are an important reflection of who I am.
2. When I feel very close to someone, it often feels to me like that
person is an important part of who I am.
3. I usually feel a strong sense of pride when someone close to
me has an important accomplishment.
4. I think one of the most important parts of who I am can be
captured by looking at my close friends and understandingwho they are.
5. When I think of myself, I often think of my close friends or
family also.
6. If a person hurts someone close to me, I feel personally hurt
as well.
7. In general, my close relationships are an important part of my
self-image.
8. Overall, my close relationships have very little to do with how
I feel about myself.
9. My close relationships are unimportant to my sense of what
kind of person I am.
10. My sense of pride comes from knowing who I have as close
friends.
11. When I establish a close friendship with someone, I usually
develop a strong sense of identification with that person.
(continues)Gender, Memory, and Self-Construal 443 |
Martinot, Crisp, & Redersdorff, 2006). Participants in one investigation were
asked simply to list as many statements as they could in response to the question
“Who am I? ”(Mackie, 1983). Compared to male participants, the women in
the study included more statements about their roles as parents and family
members. Similar findings were uncovered when elementary and high school
students were asked to “tell us about yourself ”(McGuire & McGuire, 1982).
Another group of researchers gave participants a camera with 12-exposure film
and asked them to take (or have someone else take) photographs that “describe
who you are as you see yourself ”(Clancy & Dollinger, 1993). In other words,
the photographs provided a rough indication of the cognitive representationsthe men and women held of themselves. As shown in Figure 16.5, the women ’s
photographs were more likely to include other people. When the women por-trayed the way they thought of themselves, they chose to include pictures withbest friends and loved ones. In contrast, the men more often portrayed their
self-concept with images of themselves alone.
Other research finds gender differences in the way men and women per-
ceive and recall information about significant people in their lives.
Participants in one study were asked if certain words described them, their
best friend, a group they belong to, or the president of the United States
(Josephs, Markus, & Tafarodi, 1992). As described in the previous chapter,
researchers assume information processed through a strong schema will be
more accessible than information processed through weaker schemas. When
participants were later asked to recall as many of the words as possible, the
women remembered words processed through their best friend and group
schemas better than the men. Married couples in another study were asked
to talk about their first date together, their last vacation together, and a re-
cent argument between the two of them (Ross & Holmberg, 1992). TheTo obtain your score, reverse the answer values for items 8 and 9 (that
is, 1¼7, 2¼6, etc.). Then add all 11 answer values together. High scores
indicate a tendency to think of oneself in terms of your relationships withclose others. That is, those scoring high on the scale have self-concepts
closely tied to the cognitive representations they have of the people they feel
emotionally closest to (Cross, Morris, & Gore, 2002). You can compare
your score with those obtained from a sample of American undergraduates(Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000):
Men Women Total
Mean 52.89 55.11 54.10
Standard Deviation 8.07 10.03 9.29
Scale: The Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale
Source: Copyright © 1998 by the American Psychological Association. Reproduced with permission. Cross, S. E.,
Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology , 78, 791 –808. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.791. No further reproduction
or distribution is permitted without written permission from the American Psychological Association.444 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
stories told by the wives were more vivid and contained more details than
those described by the husbands. In short, evidence from many sources makes
a strong case that men and women differ in the way they store and recall in-
formation about relationships and thus about themselves.
COGNITIONS AND DEPRESSION
For a moment, try to think of a time when you felt a little depressed. One ofthe first things you may notice is that this is relatively easy if you already feel
a little down today and relatively difficult if you feel pretty good. Depressed
people not only remember sad experiences more easily but may also have dif-
ficulty keeping themselves from generating one depressing thought after an-
other. Sad people easily recall times when they felt lonely and unloved. They
tend to dwell on their problems and worry about all the things that might gowrong. They recall embarrassing mishaps, things they wish they had never
said, and experiences they wish they could erase. Even when good things hap-
pen, depressed people look for the gray cloud to go with the silver lining. Justgot accepted into a good school? Think of all that pressure and what happens
if you fail. Been invited to a party? What if you don ’t know anyone or you
embarrass yourself there? In short, when you ’re depressed, your mind fills
with depressing thoughts.
These observations make it clear that depressing thoughts are tied to de-
pressing feelings. This is why psychologists increasingly are turning to cogni-
tive approaches to understand depression. Although negative thoughts are
often considered a symptom of depression, the cognitive perspective argues
that these thoughts can also cause people to become depressed (Clark, Beck,
& Alford, 1999). Psychologists sometimes describe the thoughts of depressed012345Females
Males
Self Alone Self With OthersNumber of Photographs
FIGURE 16.5 Number of Photographs Used to Portray Self
Source: With kind permission from Springer Science+Business Media: “Photographic depictions of the self:
Gender and age differences in social connectedness, ”by S. M. Clancy and S. J. Dollinger, Sex Roles , 29,
477–495 (1993). Copyright © 1993 by Plenum Publishing Corporation. Reprinted by permission of Springer.Cognitions and Depression 445 |
people as a depressive cognitive triad (Beck, 1972). That is, depressed people
typically have negative thoughts about themselves, are pessimistic about the
future, and tend to interpret ongoing experiences in a negative manner.
Many psychologists look for clues about the causes and treatment of de-
pression by examining the way people perceive, organize, and recall emotion-ally laden information (Rusting, 1998). Among other questions, these
investigators want to know if some people are prone to depression because
of the way they process information. We ’ll look at two concepts investigators
use in this research —depressive schemas and negative cognitive style.
Depressive Schemas
Each day we encounter some good events, a few bad events, and an occa-sional incident with ambiguous emotional meaning. Which ones will you
think about today and which will you ignore? From a cognitive perspective,
the happiest people are those who pay attention to the positive information,
dismiss the negative information, and interpret the ambiguous information as
positively as possible. In fact, most of us have an unrealistically positive out-
look on life (Taylor, 1989). We believe we are better than most at almosteverything we do, certain that good things will happen to us, and convinced
unfortunate events happen to other people. Because most of us look at life
through rose-colored glasses, we remain content and in good psychologicalhealth (Alloy & Abramson, 1988).
Unfortunately, many people look at life through glasses that are tinted
blue. Psychologists from a cognitive perspective say that depressed peopleprocess information through an active depressive schema (Clark et al., 1999;
Kuiper & Derry, 1981; Kuiper, MacDonald, & Derry, 1983). A depressive
schema is a cognitive structure containing memories about and associations
with depressing events and thoughts. People processing information through
this schema attend to negative information, ignore positive information, and
interpret ambiguous information in a depressing way. They also recall de-
pressing memories easily and often associate current sad experiences with sad
incidents from their past. In short, depressed people are set to process informa-
tion in a way that keeps negative thoughts prominent and positive thoughts
away. Little wonder, then, that these people remain depressed.
Researchers have developed a number of procedures to study depressive
schemas. Along with clinical observations about how depressed people thinkand act, these experiments provide an impressive body of evidence pointing
to the role of cognitive structures in the development and maintenance of de-
pression. Much of the evidence for depressive schemas comes from studies
employing the self-schema research techniques described in the previous chap-
ter. Researchers sometimes ask depressed and nondepressed individuals to an-
swer questions about a series of words. In one study, depressed patientsresponded to a list of adjectives by pressing a YES or a NO button to indicate
if the word described them (Derry & Kuiper, 1981). Half the words were re-
lated to depression (for example, bleak, dismal, helpless ), and half were not.
The researchers then surprised the participants by giving them 3 minutes to
recall as many of the words as they could.446 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
The results of the study are shown in Figure 16.6. As predicted, depressed
patients remembered the depression-associated words better, whereas two
groups of nondepressed participants recalled the other words better. Thisfinding has been replicated with clinically depressed patients (Lim & Kim,
2005) and mildly depressed college students (Moilanen, 1993). Depressed
people recall words like dismal and helpless better because they process these
words through a depressive schema. They are more likely to attend to the
depression-related words, associate them with aspects of themselves, and
readily recall them later on.
If depressed people process information through a depressive schema, we
would also expect them to recall sad memories more readily than people whoare not depressed. If I ask you to quickly think of something that happened toyou in high school, most likely you will think of a pleasant time. You might
recall a star performance in a play or perhaps just the fun you had hanging
out with friends. But if you are a little depressed today, you might instead re-
call a test you failed or a time you were rejected by friends. This is because
people processing information through a depressive schema have greater ac-
cess to the depressing memories stored there. When you are depressed, it
should not take long to recall times when you were sad, lonely, or embar-
rassed because using a depressive schema makes these memories readily
accessible.
This easy access to sad memories was demonstrated in an experiment
with depressed clients (Clark & Teasdale, 1982). Clients were given a series10203040Percentage of Words Recalled
Depressed
PatientsNondepressed
PatientsNondepressed
NormalsDepression-
Associated Words
Nondepression-
Associated Words
FIGURE 16.6 Proportion of Self-Descriptive Words Recalled with
Self-Referent Processing
Source: From “Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression, ”by P. A. Derry and N. A. Kuiper,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 1981, 90, 286 –297.Cognitions and Depression 447 |
of words (such as train, ice ) and asked to recall a real-life experience each
word brought to mind. For example, a client might describe a train ride to
visit her favorite aunt or a time she missed a train. Clients were tested twice,
once when they were feeling particularly depressed and once when they were
less depressed. As shown in Figure 16.7, most of the memories recalled during
the depressed period were unhappy ones. However, when clients were less de-
pressed, they recalled happier experiences. Presumably the depressive schemas
were activated more when the clients ’depression levels were higher. A similar
pattern is found when depressed people are asked to think about the future.
Depressed individuals in one study could more easily come up with reasons
something bad is likely to happen to them someday than could nondepressedparticipants (Vaughn & Weary, 2002).
Because depressed people filter information through a depressive schema,
they also tend to interpret ambiguous information in the most negative lightpossible. When depressed people consider their performances, they tend to
dwell on what they did wrong and fail to give themselves enough credit for
what they did right (Crowson & Cromwell, 1995; Moretti et al., 1996).Participants in one study were given the choice of looking at either the favor-
able or unfavorable scores from a battery of tests they had taken (Giesler,
Josephs, & Swann, 1996). Eighty-two percent of the depressed participants
chose the unfavorable feedback, significantly more than the nondepressed
participants. Thus, if an instructor tells a depressed student he did well on
five essay answers but was a little weak on one, the student will most likely
30405060Percentage of Recalled Experiences
When More
DepressedWhen Less
DepressedHappy Experiences
Unhappy Experiences
FIGURE 16.7 Percentages of Happy and Unhappy Experiences Recalled
Source: From “Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative
experiences, ”by D. M. Clark and J. D. Teasdale, Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 1982, 91, 87 –95.
Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological Association.448 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
focus his or her attention on the one weak answer and conclude that the per-
formance was poor.
Not surprisingly, depressive thoughts go hand-in-hand with other depres-
sion symptoms, such as sad mood and decreased activity. Cognitive theorists
see the causal arrow between depressive cognitions and these other symptoms
running both ways (Clark et al., 1999). That is, depressing thoughts can cause
depression, and depression can lead to an increase in depressing thoughts.
However, several studies suggest that although negative thoughts decline aspeople recover from an episode of depression, the underlying cognitive network
often remains in place (Dozois & Dobson, 2001; Hedlund & Rude, 1995; Ilardi
& Craighead, 1999; Ingram & Ritter, 2000; Segal, Gemar, & Williams, 1999).If a strong depressive schema stays intact, the individual may be vulnerable
to future bouts of depression (Havermans, Nicolson, & deVries, 2007;
Lewinsohn, Joiner, & Rohde, 2001). In fact, people with strong depressiveschemas may face a daily battle to fend off depression. Formerly depressed
patients in one study showed an increase in negative thoughts simply after lis-
tening to a sad piece of music (Gemar, Segal, Sagrati, & Kennedy, 2001).
Negative Cognitive Style
In Chapter 14 we looked at research on learned helplessness. As you recall,
psychologists initially demonstrated this effect in dogs that failed to escape
from electric shocks after first experiencing inescapable shocks. The dogs
learned they were helpless in one situation and inappropriately generalized
that perception to the new situation. Not long after the demonstrations with
animals, researchers found that people also sometimes generalize helpless feel-
ings to controllable situations. Similarities between learned helplessness parti-cipants and depressed patients led some psychologists to propose learned
helplessness as a model for understanding depression.
However, investigators soon found the simple model used to explain ani-
mal behavior was insufficient for understanding learned helplessness in peo-ple. Human research participants reacted to some uncontrollable situations
with helplessness, but not others. Feelings of helplessness generalized to sometasks, but not every task. People exposed to inescapable noise sometimes be-
came less motivated, but occasionally motivation increased (Costello, 1978;
Depue & Monroe, 1978; Roth, 1980).
The limitations of the original model led some investigators to argue that
negative life experiences alone are not sufficient to produce depression.Rather, how we interpret these events is the key (Abramson, Seligman, &
Teasdale, 1978; Miller & Norman, 1979). If you attribute the loss of a job
to a general lack of skills and aptitude that will keep you from getting a
good job anywhere else, you may be headed for depression. However, if you
fail an algebra class and conclude it ’s because this particular instructor used a
strange and unfair grading system, it is unlikely you’ ll generalize feelings of
helplessness to other math classes or other subjects.
Psychologists soon observed individual differences in the way people ex-
plain the events they encounter. In particular, researchers identified whatCognitions and Depression 449 |
they called a negative cognitive style. Individuals who possess a negative
cognitive style tend to attribute their problems to stable (enduring) and global
(widespread) causes. They also tend to anticipate the most dreadful conse-
quences and often believe the problem is the result of or reflects their own per-
sonal shortcomings. Investigators have developed procedures to measure the
extent to which people rely on this type of thinking (Alloy et al., 2000; Beevers,
Strong, Meyer, Pilkonis, & Miller, 2007; Peterson et al., 1982; Peterson &
Villanova, 1988). Like other personality variables, negative cognitive styletends to be fairly stable over time (Burns & Seligman, 1989; Hankin, 2008).
Not surprisingly, researchers find that negative cognitive style is related to
depression (Goldberg, Gerstein, Wenze, Beck & Welker, 2008; Haeffel et al.,2003; Hankin, Fraley, & Abela, 2005; Lau & Eley, 2008; Otto et al., 2007;
Riso et al., 2003). People who interpret negative events as the result of personal
shortcomings that are enduring and widespread are vulnerable to depressionwhen one of life ’s unfortunate experiences inevitably comes their way. One
pair of investigators looked at the psychological effects of physical and emo-
tional abuse in a group of battered women (Palker-Corell & Marcus, 2004).The researchers contacted the women within 2 weeks after their arrival at a
battered women ’s shelter. They found the women with a negative cognitive
style suffered from depression and other symptoms of trauma more often than
the women who did not rely on this style of thinking. Another study looked at
emotional reactions following a strong earthquake (Greening, Stoppelbein, &
Docter, 2002). Participants with a negative cognitive style were more likely to
be depressed after the earthquake than those without this cognitive style.
Investigators also use negative cognitive style to predict who might be vul-
nerable to future episodes of depression (Evans, Heron, Lewis, Araya, &Wolke, 2005; Fresco, Alloy, & Reilly-Harrington, 2006; Lewinsohn et al.,
2001; Robinson & Alloy, 2003). One team of researchers measured negative
cognitive style in incoming college freshmen (Alloy, Abramson, Whitehouse,Hogan, Panzarella, & Rose, 2006). They divided the students into those who
had previously suffered from an episode of depression and those who had
not. The researchers contacted the students again every 6 weeks for the next2 1/2 years. As shown in Figure 16.8, the students who were identified as high
risk for depression based on their negative cognitive style were much more
likely to experience at least one episode of major depression during this timethan the other students. This was not only the case for those who had bouts
of depression prior to college, but also for those who had never before suffered
from depression. In fact, the high-risk students in the latter group were 6 timesmore likely to experience depression than their low-risk classmates.
We should also note that the link between cognitive style and depression
may be affected by culture. Recall from earlier chapters that people in collec-tivist cultures tend to emphasize their role in the community, whereas people
in individualistic cultures focus on their individual aspirations and accomplish-
ments. One team of researchers found that college students in China (a collec-tivist culture) had a more pessimistic cognitive style than American students
(Lee & Seligman, 1997). Consistent with their individualistic emphasis, the
Americans were more likely to attribute successes to their own efforts and450 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
failures to other people or unfortunate circumstances. However, another inves-
tigation comparing American and Chinese students found that the kinds of ex-planations that predict depression in the United States are also associated with
depression in China (Anderson, 1999). Thus, although the way people in two
cultures typically explain events may be different, the cognitive style that leads
to depression is the same.
SUMMARY
1. Psychologists have looked at the role of hostile cognitions in aggressive
behavior. They find that aggressive responses are more likely when asituation activates hostile thoughts and emotions. Hostile cognitionsinclude aggressive behavior scripts. When hostile cognitions are highly
accessible, the likelihood of acting aggressively increases. Boys with a
history of reacting to minor events aggressively tend to interpret uninten-tional acts as deliberate and hostile.
2. Some psychologists have looked at differences in men ’s and women ’s
abilities to recall certain kinds of information. These researchers findevidence that women are more likely than men to organize self-relevant
information around emotions. Women also have been found to have
strong cognitive connections between mental representations of themselvesand the mental representations they have of close friends and loved ones.
3. The cognitive approach assumes that depressing thoughts are an impor-
tant cause of depression. Depressed people are said to process informa-
tion through a depressive schema. Depressed people recall depressing
information and remember depressing events more readily than non-
depressed people. Other researchers look at negative cognitive style.
People who tend to interpret negative events in terms of enduring and
widespread causes are more prone to depression than those who do not
rely on this cognitive style.25
20
15
10
No Prior
DepressionLow Risk
High Risk
Prior
Depression30
5Percent Experiencing
Major Depression
FIGURE 16.8 Students Experiencing Depression
Source: Alloy et al. (2006).Summary 451 |
KEY TERMS
depressive cognitive triad (p. 446)
depressive schema (p. 446)negative cognitive style (p. 450)
MEDIA RESOURCES
Visit our website. Go to www.cengage.
com/psychology/burger to find online
resources directly linked to your book,
including quizzing, glossary, flashcards,
and more.452 CHAPTER 16 The Cognitive Approach |
GLOSSARY
absorption The ability to become highly
involved in sensory and imaginative
experiences.
achievement goals Targets people aspire
to in achievement situations.
affect intensity The strength or degree to
which people typically experience their
emotions.
anal stage The psychosexual stage of
development in which the anal region is
the primary erogenous zone.
androgyny A personality trait consisting
of masculine as well as feminine
characteristics.
anima/animus The archetype that is the
feminine side of the male (anima) or the
masculine side of the female (animus).
archetypes Primordial images that
predispose us to comprehend the world
in a particular manner.
avoidance coping strategies Coping
strategies designed to distract us from
thinking about the source of anxiety.
behavior modification Therapy
procedures based on operant
conditioning and classical conditioning
principles.
behavioral approach system A
hypothetical biological system that is
focused on seeking out and achievingpleasurable goals.behavioral inhibition system A
hypothetical biological system that is
focused on avoiding dangerous andunpleasant experiences.
behavioral validation A method for
establishing a test ’s validity by
predicting behavior from test scores.
Big Five The five basic dimensions of
personality found in many factor
analytic studies.
case study method An in-depth
examination of one person or one
group.
catharsis A release of tension or anxiety.
central traits The 5 to 10 traits that best
describe a person’ s personality.
cerebral asymmetry Higher levels of
brain activity in one cerebral hemisphere
than the other.
classical conditioning Learning resulting
from pairing a conditioned stimulus
with a new, unconditioned stimulus.
collective unconscious The part of the
unconscious mind containing thoughts,
images, and psychic characteristics
common to all members of a culture.
collectivist culture Culture that
emphasizes the importance of belonging
to a larger group, such as a family, tribe,
or nation.
conditional/unconditional positive
regard Acceptance and respect forpeople either only when they act as we
desire (conditional) or regardless of their
behavior (unconditional).
congruent validity A method for
establishing a test ’s validity by
correlating the test scores with other
measures of the same construct.
conscious In Freud ’s topographic model,
the part of personality that contains the
thoughts we are currently aware of.
construct validity The extent to which a
test measures the hypothetical construct
it is designed to measure.
contingencies of self-worth The domains
of self-concept an individual uses to
evaluate his or her self.
coping strategies Conscious efforts to
reduce
anxiety in the face of a perceived
threat.
correlation coefficient A statistic that
indicates the strength and direction of a
relationship between two variables.
defense mechanisms Devices the ego uses
to keep threatening material out of
awareness and thereby reduce or avoidanxiety.
defensive pessimism The tendency to
attend to and worry about failure on
upcoming tasks in a strategic effort tomotivate oneself to do well.
deficiency motive A need that is reduced
when the object of the need is attained.
453 |
denial A defense mechanism in which a
person denies the existence of a fact.
dependent variable The experimental
variable measured by the experimenter
and used to compare groups.
depressive cognitive triad Three
elements that describe a depressed
person’ s cognitions: negative views of
the self, pessimism, and interpretingevents in a negative manner.
depressive schema A cognitive structure
that allows people to readily make
negative associations.
discriminant validity A method for
establishing a test ’s validity by
demonstrating that its scores do not
correlate with the scores of theoreticallyunrelated measures.
discrimination A learned tendency to
respond only to stimuli that result in
reinforcement and not to similar, but
unrewarded, stimuli.
displacement A defense mechanism in
which a response is directed at a
nonthreatening target instead of the
unconsciously preferred one.
dispositional optimism The extent to
which a person typically adopts an
optimistic or pessimistic approach to
dealing with life ’s challenges.
egoIn Freud ’s structural model, the part
of personality that considers externalreality while mediating between the
demands of the id and the superego.
emotional affectivity The extent to
which people typically experience
positive and negative emotions.
emotional expressiveness The extent to
which people outwardly express their
emotions.
emotion-focused strategies Coping
strategies designed to reduce emotional
distress.
evaluation apprehension A strong
concern about receiving negative
evaluations from others.
face validity A method for establishing a
test ’s validity in which test items appear
to measure what the test was designed to
measure.
factor analysis A statistical procedure
used to determine the number of
dimensions in a data set.
fixation Tying up psychic energy at one
psychosexual stage, which results in
adult behaviors characteristic of that
stage.free association A procedure used in
psychoanalysis in which patients say
whatever comes into their mind.
Freudian slip A seemingly innocent
misstatement that reveals unconscious
associations.
fully functioning person A psycho-
logically healthy individual who is able
to enjoy life as completely as possible.
generalization The tendency to respond
to stimuli similar to the one used in the
initial conditioning.
goodness of fit model A model
proposing that a child performs best
when the demands of the environmentmatch with his or her temperament.
growth need A need that leads to
personal growth and that persists after
the need object is attained.
hierarchy of needs In Maslow ’s theory,
the order in which human needs demand
attention.
Human Figure Drawing test A projective
test
in which test takers are asked simply
to draw a person.
hypothesis A formal prediction about
the relationship between two or more
variables that is logically derived from a
theory.
idIn Freud ’s structural model, the part
of personality concerned with immediate
gratification of needs.
idiographic approach A method of
studying personality through in-depth
analysis of one individual and the
dimensions relevant to that person’ s
personality.
independent variable The experimental
variable used to divide participants into
groups.
individualistic culture Culture that
places great emphasis on individual
needs and accomplishments.
inhibited/uninhibited children Inhibited
children show strong anxiety about
novel and unfamiliar situations;
uninhibited children show very little ofthis anxiety.
intellectualization A defense mechanism
in which the emotional content of
threatening material is removed before itis brought into awareness.
internal consistency The extent to which
test items are interrelated and thus
appear to measure the same construct.
learned helplessness The cognitive,
motivational, and emotional deficits thatfollow a perceived lack of control over
important aversive events.
libido The limited amount of psychic
energy that powers mental activity.
locus of control A personality trait that
divides people along a continuum
according to the extent to which theybelieve what happens to them and others
is controllable.
manipulated independent variable An
independent variable for which
participants have been randomly
assigned to an experimental group.
masculinity –femininity A personality
trait indicating the extent to which a
person possesses sex-typed character-
istics, with masculine characteristics atone end of the trait continuum and
feminine characteristics at the other end.
need for Achievement The motive to
engage in and succeed at entrepreneurial
achievement behavior.
negative cognitive style A style of
processing information in which people
attribute their problems to stable and
global causes, anticipate dreadful
consequences, and believe the problemsreflect their own personal shortcomings.
neodissociation theory Hilgard ’s theory,
which maintains that consciousness is
divided into aware and unaware parts
during hypnosis.
nomothetic approach A method of
understanding personality that
compares many people along the same
personality dimensions.
nonmanipulated independent variable
An independent variable for which
condition assignment is determined by a
characteristic of the participant.
observational learning Learning that
results from watching or hearing about a
person modeling the behavior.
operant conditioning Learning resulting
from the response an organism receives
following a behavior.
optimal experience A state of happiness
and satisfaction characterized by
absorption in a challenging and
personally
rewarding task.
oral stage The psychosexual stage of
development in which the mouth, lips,
and tongue are the primary erogenous
zones.
personal constructs In Kelly ’s theory, the
bipolar cognitive structures through
which people process information.454 GLOSSARY |
personal narratives Assessment
procedure that asks individuals to
provide autobiographical descriptions
of important events in their lives.
personality Consistent behavior patterns
and intrapersonal processes originating
within the individual.
person-by-situation approach An
approach to understanding behavior
that maintains behavior is a function of
the person as well as the situation.
phallic stage The psychosexual stage
of development in which the genital
region is the primary erogenous zone
and in which the Oedipus complexdevelops.
possible selves Cognitive representations
of the kind of people we think we might
become some day.
preconscious In Freud ’s topographic
model, the part of personality that
contains thoughts that can be broughtinto awareness with little difficulty.
primordial images The images that make
up the collective unconscious.problem-focused strategies Coping
strategies directed at taking care of the
problem causing the anxiety.
projection A defense mechanism in
which one ’s own unconscious thoughts
and impulses are attributed to other
people.
projective tests Tests designed to assess
unconscious material by asking test
takers to respond to ambiguous stimuli.
psychoanalysis The system of
psychotherapy developed by Freud that
focuses on uncovering the unconscious
material responsible for a patient ’s
disorder.
psychogenic need In Murray ’s theory, a
relatively stable predisposition toward
a type of action.
psychosexual stages of development The
innate sequence of development made
up of stages characterized by primaryerogenous zones and sexual desires.
Q-Sort An assessment procedure in
which test takers distribute personal
descriptions along a continuum.
rational emotive therapy A
psychotherapy procedure introduced byEllis that examines the irrational
reasoning causing emotional problems.
reaction formation A defense
mechanism in which people act in a
manner opposite to their unconscious
desires.
reciprocal determinism The notion that
external determinants of behavior,
internal determinants of behavior, and
behavior all influence one another.
reliability The extent to which a test
measures consistently.
repression A defense mechanism in
which the ego pushes threatening
material out of awareness and into the
unconscious.
Rorschach inkblot test A projective test
in which test takers are asked to describe
what they see in a series of inkblots.
schema A hypothetical cognitive
structure used to process information.
self-actualization A state of personal
growth in which people fulfill their true
potential.
self-disclosure The act of revealing
intimate information about oneself to
another
person.
self-efficacy A person’ s expectancy that
he or she can successfully perform a
given behavior.
self-esteem Evaluation of one ’s self-
concept, usually measured in terms of a
relatively stable and global assessment
of how a person feels about him- or
herself.
self-regulation The ability to develop
and apply rewards and punishments for
internal standards of behavior.
self-schema A schema consisting of
aspects of a person ’s life most important
to him or her.
shadow The archetype that contains the
evil side of humanity.
social anxiety A trait dimension
indicating the extent to which people
experience anxiety during socialencounters or when anticipating socialencounters.
social desirability The extent to which
test takers tend to respond to items in a
manner that presents them in a positivelight.statistical significance The likelihood
that a research finding represents a
genuine effect rather than a chance
fluctuation of measurement.
striving for superiority The primary
motivational force in Adler ’s theory,
which is the person’ s effort to overcome
feelings of inferiority.structural model Freud ’s model of
personality that divides personality into
the id, the ego, and the superego.
subception The perception of information
at a less-than-conscious level.
sublimation A defense mechanism in
which threatening unconscious impulses
are channeled into socially acceptable
behaviors.
superego In Freud ’s structural model,
the part of personality that represents
society ’s values.
temperaments General behavioral
predispositions present in infancy andassumed to be inherited.
Thanatos The self-destructive (death)
instinct, which is often turned outward
in the form of aggression.
Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) A
projective test in which test takers are
asked to tell stories about a series ofambiguous pictures.
theory A general statement about the
relationship between constructs or
events.
topographic model Freud ’s original
model of personality structure, in which
personality is divided into three differentlevels of awareness.
trait A dimension of personality used to
categorize people according to the
degree to which they manifest aparticular characteristic.
twin-study method A procedure for
examining the role of genetics on
personality in which pairs of
monozygotic and dizygotic twins are
compared.
unconscious In Freud ’s topographic
model, the part of personality that
contains material that cannot easily be
brought into awareness.
validity The
extent to which a test
measures what it is designed to measure.GLOSSARY 455 |
REFERENCES
Abramson , L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., &
Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned
helplessness in humans: Critique
and reformulation.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,87,4 9 –74.
Accortt , E. E., & Allen, J. J. B. (2006).
Frontal EEG asymmetry andpremenstrual dysphoric symptom-
atology.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,115, 179 –184.
Ainsworth , M. D. S. (1989).
Attachments beyond infancy.
American Psychologist
,44,
709–716.
Ainsworth ,
M. D. S., Blehar, M. C.,
Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978).
Patterns of attachment
. Hillsdale,
NJ:
Erlbaum.
Aldwin , C. M., & Revenson, T. A.
(1987). Does coping help? A reex-amination of the relation between
coping and mental health.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,53, 337 –348.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988).
Depressive realism: Four theoretical
perspectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.),
Cognitive processes in depression
(pp. 223 –265). New York:
Guilford.
Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Hogan,
M. E., Whitehouse, W. G., Rose,D. T., Robinson, M. S., et al.
(2000). The Temple-Wisconsin cog-nitive vulnerability to depression
project: Lifetime history of Axis Ipsychopathology in individuals at
high and low cognitive risk fordepression.
Journal of Abnormal
Ps
ychology
,10 9 ,4 0 3 –418.
Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y.,
Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E.,
Panzarella, C., & Rose, D. T.
(2006). Prospective incidence of
first onsets and recurrences ofdepression in individuals at highand low cognitive risk for depres-
sion.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,115, 145 –156.
Allport , G. W. (1961).
Pattern and
growth
in personality
. New York:
Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Allport , G. W. (1965).
Letters from
Jenny
. New York: Harcourt,
Brace
& World.
Allport , G. W. (1967). Gordon W.
Allport. In E. G. Boring &
G. Lindzey (Eds.),
A history of
psychology
in autobiography
(Vol. 5, pp. 3 –25). New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Allport , G. W. (1968).
The person in
psychology:
Selected essays
.
Boston:
Beacon.
Almagor , M., Tellegen, A., & Waller,
N. G. (1995). The Big Seven model:A cross-cultural replication and
further exploration of the basic
dimensions of natural languagetrait descriptors.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
69,
300–307.Altman , I. (1975).
The environment and
social
behavior
. Monterey, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Altman ,
I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973).
Social penetration: The develop-ment
of interpersonal relationships
.
New
York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Amir , N., Beard, C., & Bower, E.
(2005). Interpretation bias andsocial anxiety.
Cognitive Therapy
and
Research
,29, 433 –443.
Amirkhan, J. H., Risinger, R. T., &
Swickert, R. J. (1995).
Extraversion: A “hidden ”person-
ality factor in coping?
Journal of
Personality
,63, 189 –212.
Andersen, B. L., Cyranowski, J. M., &
Espindle, D. (1999). Men ’s sexual
self-schema.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,76,
645–661.
Anderson ,
C. A. (1999). Attributional
style, depression, and loneliness:A cross-cultural comparison of
American and Chinese students.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,15, 482 –499.
Anderson , C. A. (2004). An update on
the effects of playing violent video
games.
Journal of Adolescence
,27,
113–122.
Anderson ,
C. A., & Anderson, K. B.
(1998). Temperature and aggres-
sion: Paradox, controversy, and a
(fairly) clear picture. In R. Geen &
456 |
E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human
aggression:
Theories, research, and
implications for public policy
(pp. 247 –298). New York:
Academic Press.
Anderson , C. A., & Bushman, B. J.
(2001). Effects of violent video
games on aggressive behavior,
aggressive cognition, aggressive af-fect, physiological arousal, and
prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic
review of the scientific literature.
Psychological Science
,12,
353–359.
Anderson ,
C. A., & Bushman, B. J.
(2002a). Human aggression.
Annual Review of Psychology
,53,
27–51.
Anderson ,
C. A., & Bushman, B. J.
(2002b). The effects of media
violence on society.
Science
,295,
2377 –2378.
Anderson ,
C. A., Carnagey, N. L., &
Eubanks, J. (2003). Exposure toviolent media: The effects of songswith violent lyrics on aggressive
thoughts and feelings.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
84,
960–971.
Anderson , C. A., Carnagey, N. L.,
Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J.,
Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C.
(2004). Violent video games:Specific effects of violent content on
aggressive thoughts and behavior.
Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology
,36, 199 –249.
Anderson , C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000).
Video games and aggressive
thoughts, feelings, and behavior inthe laboratory and in life.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,78, 772 –790.
Anderson , C. A., & Harvey, R. J.
(1988). Discriminating between
problems in living: An examination
of measures of depression, loneli-
ness, shyness, and social anxiety.
Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology
,6, 482 –491.
Anderson , C. A., & Huesmann, L. R.
(2003). Human aggression: Asocial-cognitive view. In M.A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.),
Handbook of social psychology
(pp. 296 –323). London: Sage.
Ansbacher , H. L., & Ansbacher, R. R.
(Eds.). (1956).
The individual
psychology
of Alfred Adler
.
New
York: Basic Books.
Antill , J. K. (1983). Sex role comple-
mentarity versus similarity in mar-
ried couples.
Journal of Personalityand
Social Psychology
,45,
145–155.
Antonen,
M. (1993, June 25). Sparky
thrives on high anxiety.
USA
Today
, p. 3C.
Archer ,
J. (1996). Sex differences in
social behavior: Are the social role
and evolutionary explanationscompatible?
American
Psychologist
,51, 909 –917.
Archibald , F. S., Bartholomew, K., &
Marx, R. (1995). Loneliness inearly adolescence: A test of thecognitive discrepancy model of
loneliness.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,21, 296 –301.
Arkin , A. M., Antrobus, J. S., & Ellman,
S. J. (1978).
The mind in sleep:
Psychology
and psychophysiology
.
Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Aron , A., Aron, E. N., & Allen, J.
(1998). Motivations for unrecipro-cated love.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,24, 787 –796.
Arthur , W., & Graziano, W. G. (1996).
The five-factor model, conscien-tiousness, and driving accident in-
volvement.
Journal of Personality
,
64,
593–618.
Asendorpf , J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998).
Personality effects on social rela-tionships.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,74,
1531 –1544.
Aserinsky ,
E., & Kleitman, N. (1953).
Regularly occurring periods of eye
motility and concomitant phenom-ena during sleep.
Science
,118,
273–274.
Ashton ,
M. C., & Lee, K. (2007).
Empirical, theoretical, and practicaladvantages of the HEXACO model
of personality structure.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Review
,11,
150–166.
Ashton ,
M. C., Lee, K., Goldberg, L. R.,
& de Vries, R. E. (2009). Higher
order factors of personality: Do
they exist?
Personality and
Soci
al Psychology Review
,13,
79–91
.
Aspinwall , L. G., & Brunhart, S. M.
(1996). Distinguishing optimismfrom denial: Optimistic beliefs pre-
dict attention to health threats.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,22, 993 –1003.
Aspinwall , L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1992).
Modeling cognitive adaptation: Alongitudinal investigation of theimpact of individual differences andcoping on college adjustment and
performance.
Journal of Personalityand
Social Psychology
,63,
989–1003.
Aube ,
J. (2008). Balancing concern for
other with concern for self: Links
between unmitigated communion,communion, and psychological
well-being.
Journal of Personality
,
76,
101–133.
Aube , J., Norcliffe, H., Craig, J., &
Koestner, R. (1995). Gender char-acteristics and adjustment-related
outcomes: Questioning themasculinity model.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,21,
284–295.
Austenfeld ,
J. L., & Stanton, A. L.
(2004). Coping through emotionalapproach: A new look at emotion,
coping, and health-related out-
comes.
Journal of Personality
,72,
1335 –1363.
Bagby ,
R. M., Rogers, R., Nicholson,
R. A., Buis, T., Seeman, M. V., &
Rector, N. A. (1997). Effectiveness
of the MMPI-2 validity indicatorsin the detection of defensive re-
sponding in clinical and nonclinical
samples.
Psychological Assessment
,
9,
406–413.
Baker , E. L., & Nash, M. R. (2008).
Psychoanalytic approaches to clini-
cal hypnosis. In M. R. Nash &
A. J. Barnier (Eds.),
The Oxford
handbook
of hypnosis: Theory,
research and practice
(pp. 439 –
456).
New York: Oxford University
Press.
Baker , L. A., & Daniels, D. (1990).
Nonshared environmental influ-ences and personality differencesin adult twins.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
58,
103–110.
Baker , S. R. (2007). Dispositional opti-
mism and health status, symptomsand behaviours: Assessing idio-
thetic relationships using a pro-
spective daily diary approach.
Psychology and Health
,22,
431–455.
Baldwin ,
M. W., & Main, K. J. (2001).
Social anxiety and the cued activa-tion of relational knowledge.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,27, 1637 –1647.
Bandura , A. (1965). Influences of mod-
els’reinforcement contingencies on
the acquisition of imitative re-sponses.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,1, 589 –595.
Bandura , A. (1973).
Aggression: A so-
cial
learning analysis
. Englewood
Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.REFERENCES 457 |
Bandura , A. (1977a). Social learning
theory
. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Bandura ,
A. (1977b). Self-efficacy:
Toward a unifying theory of be-
havioral change.
Psychological
Review
,84, 191 –215.
Bandura , A. (1986).
Social foundations
of
thought and action: A social
cognitive theory
. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Bandura , A. (1997).
Self-efficacy: The
exercise
of control
. New York:
Freeman.
Bandura ,
A. (2001). Social cognitive
theory: An agentic perspective.
Annual Review of Psychology
,52,
1–26.
Bandura ,
A. (2006). Toward a psychol-
ogy of human agency.
Perspectives
on
Psychological Science
,1,
164–180.
Bandura ,
A., Adams, N. E., & Beyer, J.
(1977). Cognitive processes medi-ating behavioral change.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
35,
125–139.
Bandura , A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara,
G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996).Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy
beliefs on academic functioning.
Child Development
,67,
1206 –1222.
Bandura ,
A., & Bussey, K. (2004). On
broadening the cognitive, motiva-
tional, and sociostructural scope of
theorizing about gender develop-ment and functioning: Comment on
Martin, Ruble, and Szkrybalo
(2002).
Psychological Bulletin
,130,
691–701.
Bandura ,
A., Pastorelli, C., Barbaranelli,
C., & Caprara, G. V. (1999). Self-
efficacy pathways to childhood de-
pression.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,76, 258 –269.
Barber , T. S., & Calverley, D. S. (1964).
Toward a theory of hypnotic be-havior: Effects on suggestibility ofdefining the situation as hypnosis
and defining responses to sugges-
tion as easy.
Journal of Abnormal
and
Social Psychology
,68,
585–592.
Barber ,
T. X. (1969).
Hypnosis: A
scientific
approach
. New York:
Van
Nostrand Reinhold.
Barber , T. X. (1999). A comprehensive
three-dimensional theory of hypno-
sis. In I. Kirsch, A. Capafons,
E. Cardena-Buelna, & S. Amigo(Eds.),
Clinical hypnosis and self-
regulation:
Cognitive-behavioralperspectives
(pp. 21 –48).
Washington,
DC: American
Psychological Association.
Bardos, A. N., & Powell, S. (2001).
Human figure drawings and theDraw-A-Person: Screening proce-
dures for emotional disturbances.
In W. I. Dorfman & M. Hersen(Eds.),
Understanding psychologi-
cal
assessment
(pp. 275 –294).
New
York: Plenum.
Bar-Haim , Y., Fox, N. A., Benson, B.,
Guyer, A. E., Williams, A., Nelson,E. E., et al. (2009). Neural corre-
lates of reward processing in ado-
lescents with a history of inhibitedtemperament.
Psychological
Science
,20, 1009 –1018.
Barlett , C., Branch, O., Rodeheffer, C.,
& Harris, R. (2009). How long dothe short-term violent video gameeffects last?
Aggressive Behavior
,
35,
225–236.
Barlett , C. P., & Rodeheffer, C. (2009).
Effects of realism on extended vio-lent and nonviolent video game
play on aggressive thoughts, feel-
ings, and physiological arousal.
Aggressive Behavior
,35, 213 –224.
Barlow , D. H. (1988).
Anxiety and its
disorders
. New York: Guilford.
Baron ,
R. A. (1978a). The influence of
hostile and nonhostile humor upon
physical aggression.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,4,
77–80.
Baron ,
R. A. (1978b). Aggression-
inhibiting influence of sexual be-
havior.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,36, 189 –197.
Barrett , M. S., & Berman, J. S. (2001). Is
psychotherapy more effective whentherapists disclose information
about themselves?
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,69, 597 –603.
Barrett , P., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1984).
The assessment of personality
factors across 25 countries.
Personality and Individual
Differences
,5, 615 –632.
Barrick , M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991).
The Big Five personality dimensions
and job performance: A meta-
analysis.
Personnel Psychology
,44,
1–26.
Barrick ,
M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge,
T. A. (2001). Personality and per-formance at the beginning of the
new millennium: What do we knowand where to we go next?
International Journal of Selection
and
Assessment
,91,9–30.Barrick ,
M. R., Mount, M. K., &
Strauss, J. P. (1993).
Conscientiousness and performance
of sales representatives: Test of themediating effects of goal setting.
Journal of Applied Psychology
,78,
715–722.
Barrick ,
M. R., Mount, M. K., &
Strauss, J. P. (1994). Antecedents
of involuntary turnover due to a
reduction in force.
Personnel
Psychology
,47, 515 –535.
Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M.
(2001). Achievement goals andoptimal motivation: Testing multi-
ple goal models.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
80,
706–722.
Bartholomew , K. (1990). Avoidance of
intimacy: An attachment perspec-
tive.
Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships
,7, 147 –178.
Bartholomew , K., & Horowitz, L. M.
(1991). Attachment styles amongyoung adults: A test of a four-category model.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
61,
226–244.
Bartholomew , K., & Shaver, P. R.
(1998). Methods of assessing adult
attachment. In J. A. Simpson &
W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
Attachment
theory
and close relationships
(pp. 25 –45). New York: Guilford.
Bartholow , B. D., Bushman, B. J., &
Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic vio-lent video game exposure and de-sensitization to violence: Behavioral
and event-related brain potential
data.
Journal of Experimental
Social
Psychology
,42, 532 –539.
Basgall , J. A., & Snyder, C. R. (1988).
Excuses in waiting: External locusof control and reactions to success-failure feedback.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
54,
656–662.
Bates, J. E., Wachs, T. D., & Emde,
R. N. (1994). Toward practical usesfor biological concepts of tempera-
ment. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs
(Eds.),
Temperament: Individual
differences
at the interface of biol-
ogy and behavior
(pp. 275 –306).
Washington,
DC: American
Psychological Association.
Bauder , D. (2005, November 29).
Violence surging on networks.
San Jose Mercury News
, p. 2E.
Bauer ,
J. J., & Bonanno, G. A. (2001).
I can, I do, I am: The narrative dif-
ferentiation of self-efficacy and
other self-evaluations while458 REFERENCES |
adapting to bereavement. Journal
of
Research in Personality
,35,
424–448.
Baumeister ,
R. F. (1991). On the stabil-
ity of variability: Retest reliability
of metatraits.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,17,
633–639.
Baumeister ,
R. F., & Leary, M. R.
(1995). The need to belong: Desire
for interpersonal attachments as a
fundamental human motivation.
Psychological Bulletin
,117,
497–529.
Baumeister ,
R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1988).
Metatraits.
Journal of Personality
,
56,
571–598.
Baumeister , R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1990).
Anxiety and social exclusion.
Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology
,9, 165 –195.
Bauminger , N., Finzi-Dottan, R.,
Chason, S., & Har-Even, D. (2008).Intimacy in adolescent friendship:The roles of attachment, coherence,and self-disclosure.
Journal of
Social
and Personal Relationships
,
25,
409–428.
Bechtle , R. (1984). C. G. Jung and the
religion of the unconscious. In
J. Heaney (Ed.),
Psyche and spirit
(pp. 138 –163). New York: Paulist.
Beck , A. T. (1972).
Depression: Causes
and
treatments
. Philadelphia:
University
of Pennsylvania Press.
Beck , A. T., & Bhar, S. S. (2009).
‘Effectiveness of long-term psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy: A meta-analysis ’: Comment.
Journal of the
American
Medical Association
,
301,
931.
Beevers , C. G., Strong, D. R., Meyer, B.,
Pilkonis, P. A., & Miller, I. W.
(2007). Efficiently assessing nega-
tive cognition in depression: Anitem response theory analysis of the
Dysfunctional Attitude Scale.
Psychological Assessment
,19,
199–209.
Bell,
R. C. (1990). Analytic issues in the
use of Repertory Grid Technique.
In G. J. Neimeyer & R. A.
Neimeyer (Eds.),
Advances in per-
sonal
construct psychology
(Vol. 1,
pp.
25–48). Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.
Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On
predicting some of the people some
of the time: The search for cross-
situational consistencies in behavior.
Psychological Review
,81,5 0 6 –520.
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of
psychological androgyny.
Journalof
Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,42, 155 –162.
Bem, S. L. (1977). On the utility of
alternative procedures for assessingpsychological androgyny.
Journal
of
Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,45, 196 –205.
Bem, S. L., & Lenney, E. (1976). Sex-
typing and the avoidance of cross-
sex behavior.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,33,4 8 –54.
Bem, S. L., Martyna, W., & Watson, C.
(1976). Sex typing and androgyny:Further explorations of the expres-
sive domain.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,34,
1016 –1023.
Benassi ,
V. A., Sweeney, P. D., &
Dufour, C. L. (1988). Is there a re-
lationship between locus of control
orientation and depression?
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology
,97,
357–367.
Benet-Martinez ,
V., & Oishi, S. (2008).
Personality and culture. In O.P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A.
Pervin (Eds.),
Handbook of per-
sonality
(3rd ed., pp. 542 –567).
New
York: Guilford.
Benet-Martinez , V., & Waller, N. G.
(1997). Further evidence for the
cross-cultural generality of the Big
Seven factor model: Indigenous andimported Spanish personality con-
structs.
Journal of Personality
,65,
567–598.
Bengston ,
P. L., & Grotevant, H. D.
(1999). The individuality and con-nectedness Q-Sort: A measure for
assessing individuality and con-nectedness in dyadic relationships.
Personal Relationships
,6,
213–225.
Benham,
G., Woody, E. Z., Wilson,
K. S., & Nash, M. R. (2006).Expect the unexpected: Ability, at-
titude, and responsiveness to hyp-
nosis.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,91, 342 –350.
Ben Hamida , S., Mineka, S., & Bailey,
J. M. (1998). Sex differences inperceived controllability of matevalue: An evolutionary perspective.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,75, 953 –966.
Bennett , P., Moore, L., Smith, A.,
Murphy, S., & Smith, C. (1994).
Health locus of control and value
for health as predictors of dietary
behaviour.
Psychology and Health
,
10,4
1–54.
Berg, J. H., & Peplau, L. A. (1982).
Loneliness: The relationship ofself-disclosure and androgyny.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,8, 624 –630.
Berkman , L. F. (1995). The role of social
relations in health promotion.
Psychosomatic Medicine
,57,
245–254.
Berkow ,
I. (2002, March 27). True
heroism outside the lines.
New
York
Times
, p. C15.
Berkowitz ,
L. (1970). Aggressive humor
as a stimulus to aggressive re-
sponses.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,16, 710 –717.
Berkowitz , L. (1989). The frustration-
aggression hypothesis: Anexamination and reformulation.
Psychological Bulletin
,106,5 9 –73.
Berkowitz , L. (1994). Is something
missing? Some observationsprompted by the cognitive-neoassociationist view of anger and
emotional aggression. In L.
R. Huesmann (Ed.),
Aggressive
behavior:
Current perspectives
(pp. 35 –57). New York: Plenum.
Berkowitz , L. (1998). Affective aggres-
sion: The role of stress, pain, andnegative affect. In R. G. Geen &E. Donnerstein (Eds.),
Human ag-
gression:
Theories, research, and
implications for social policy
(pp.
49–72).
San Diego: Academic Press.
Berkowitz , L. (2008). On the consider-
ation of automatic as well as con-
trolled psychological processes in
aggression.
Aggressive Behavior
,
34,
117–129.
Berman , M. E., McCloskey, M. S.,
Fanning, J. R., Schumacher, J. A.,& Coccaro, E. F. (2009). Serotoninaugmentation reduces response to
attack in aggressive individuals.
Psychological Science
,20,
714–720.
Berman ,
M. I., & Frazier, P. A. (2005).
Relationship power and betrayal
experience as predictors of reac-
tions to infidelity.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,31,
1617 –1627.
Berry ,
D. S., & Hansen, J. S. (1996).
Positive affect, negative affect, andsocial interaction.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
71,
796–809.
Berry , D. S., & Willingham, J. K.
(1997). Affective traits, responses to
conflict, and satisfaction in romantic
relationships.
Journal of Research in
Pe
rsonality
,31,5 6 4 –576.
Berry , D. S., Willingham, J. K., &
Thayer, C. A. (2000). Affect andREFERENCES 459 |
personality as predictors of conflict
and closeness in young adults ’
friendships.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,34,8 4 –107.
Bettencourt , B. A., Talley, A. E.,
Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J.
(2006). Personality and aggressive
behavior under provoking and
neutral conditions: A meta-analyticreview.
Psychological Bulletin
,132,
751–777.
Bettencourt ,
B. A., Talley, A. E., Molix,
L., Schlegel, R., & Westgate, S. J.(2008). Rural and urban breast
cancer patients: Health locus of
control and psychological adjust-ment.
Psycho-Oncology
,17,
932–939.
Bianchi ,
E. C. (1988). Jungian psychol-
ogy and religious experience. InR. L. Moore (Ed.),
Carl Jung and
Christian
spirituality
(pp. 16 –37).
New
York: Paulist.
Biederman , J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R.,
Rosenbaum, J. F., Herot, C.,Friedman, D., Snidman, N., et al.
(2001). Further evidence of associ-
ation between behavioral inhibitionand social anxiety in children.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
158,
1673 –1679.
Blankenship , V., Vega, C. M., Ramos,
E., Romero, K., Warren, K.,Keenan, K., et al. (2006). Using the
multifaceted Rasch model to im-
prove the TAT/PSE measure of needfor Achievement.
Journal of
Personality
Assessment
,86,
100–114.
Bleidorn ,
W., Kandler, C., Riemann, R.,
Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M.(2009). Patterns and sources of
adult personality development:
Growth curve analysis of the NEOPI-R Scales in a longitudinal twin
study.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,97, 142 –155.
Block , J. (1978).
The Q-Sort method in
personality
assessment and psychi-
atric research
. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Block , J. (1995). A contrarian view of
the five-factor approach to person-
ality description.
Psychological
Bulletin
,117, 187 –215.
Block , J. (2008).
The Q-Sort in charac-
ter
appraisal
. Washington, DC:
American
Psychological
Association.
Block , J. H., Gjerde, P. F., & Block,
J. H. (1991). Personality antece-dents of depressive tendencies in
18-year-olds: A prospective study.Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,60, 726 –738.
Bloise , S. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2007).
Memory for emotional and neutral
information: Gender and individualdifferences in emotional sensitivity.
Memory
,15, 192 –204.
Bodner , E., & Mikulincer, M. (1998).
Learned helplessness and the oc-
currence of depressive-like and
paranoid-like responses: The role of
attentional focus.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
1010 –1023.
Boldizar , J. P., Perry, D. G., & Perry,
L. C. (1989). Outcome values andaggression.
Child Development
,60,
571–579.
Bond ,
M. (1992). An empirical study of
defensive style: The Defense StyleQuestionnaire. In G. Vaillant (Ed.),
Ego mechanisms of defense: A
guide
for clinicians and researchers
(pp. 127 –158). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
Booth-Kewley , S., & Vickers, R. R.
(1994). Associations between major
domains of personality and health
behavior.
Journal of Personality
,
62,
281–298.
Borkenau , P., Riemann, R., Angleitner,
A., & Spinath, F. M. (2001).Genetic and environmental influ-ences on observed personality:
Evidence from the German obser-
vational study of adult twins.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,80, 655 –668.
Bowers , S. P. (1999). Gender role iden-
tity and the caregiving experience
of widowed men.
Sex Roles
,41,
645–655.
Bowlby ,
J. (1969).
Attachment and loss:
Vol.
1. Attachment
. New York:
Basic
Books.
Bowlby , J. (1973).
Attachment and loss:
Vol.
2. Separation: Anger and
anxiety
. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby ,
J. (1980).
Attachment and loss:
Vol.
3. Loss, sadness, and depres-
sion
. New York: Basic Books.
Bradbury ,
T. N., Campbell, S. M., &
Fincham, F. D. (1995).
Longitudinal and behavioral analy-
sis of masculinity and femininity in
marriage.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,68,
328–341.
Braffman ,
W., & Kirsch, I. (1999).
Imaginative suggestibility and hyp-notizability: An empirical analysis.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,77, 578 –587.Brebner , J., & Cooper, C. (1978).
Stimulus- or response-induced ex-
citation: A comparison of behavior
in introverts and extraverts.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,12,
306–311.
Brennan ,
K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver,
P. R. (1998). Self-report measure-
ment of adult attachment. InJ. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
(Eds.),
Attachment theory and close
relationships
(pp. 46 –76). New
York: Guilford.
Brennan , K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1993).
Attachment styles and parental
divorce.
Journal of Divorce and
Remarriage
,21, 161 –175.
Brennan , K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995).
Dimensions of adult attachment,
affect regulation, and romantic re-lationship functioning.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,21,
267–283.
Bridges ,
N. A. (2001). Therapist ’s self-
disclosure: Expanding the comfortzone.
Psychotherapy
,38,2 1 –30.
Briggs , S. R. (1989). The optimal level of
measurement of personality con-structs. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor(Eds.),
Personality psychology:
Recent
trends and emerging direc-
tions
(pp. 246 –260). New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Brissette , I., & Cohen, S. (2002). The
contribution of individual differ-
ences in hostility to the associations
between daily interpersonal con-flict, affect, and sleep.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,28,
1265 –1274.
Brissette ,
I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver,
C. S. (2002). The role of optimism
in social network development,
coping, and psychological adjust-ment during a life transition.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,82, 102 –111.
Britt, T. W. (1993). Metatraits: Evidence
relevant to the validity of the con-
struct and its implications.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,65, 544 –562.
Britt, T. W., & Shepperd, J. A. (1999).
Trait relevance and trait assess-
ment.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Review
,3, 108 –122.
Brockner , J. (1979). The effects of self-
esteem, success-failure, and self-consciousness on task performance.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,37, 1732 –1741.
Brockner , J., Derr, W. R., & Laing,
W. N. (1987). Self-esteem and460 REFERENCES |
reactions to negative feedback:
Toward greater generalizability.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
21,
318–333.
Brody , J. E. (2000, April 25). Memories
of things that never were.
New
York
Times
, p. D8.
Brody ,
L. R., Rozek, M. K., & Muten,
E. O. (1985). Age, sex, and indi-
vidual differences in children ’s de-
fensive styles.
Journal of Clinical
Child
Psychology
,14, 132 –138.
Brooks-Gunn , J., & Fisch, M. (1980).
Psychological androgyny and col-
lege students ’judgments of mental
health.
Sex Roles
,6, 575 –580.
Brown , J., & Inouye, D. K. (1978).
Learned helplessness throughmodeling: The role of perceived
similarity in competence.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
36,
900–908.
Brown , J. D., & Dutton, K. A. (1995).
The thrill of victory, the complexity
of defeat: Self-esteem and people ’s
emotional reactions to success and
failure.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,68, 712 –722.
Brown , J. D., & Gallagher, F. M.
(1992). Coming to terms with fail-ure: Private self-enhancement and
public self-effacement.
Journal of
Experimental
Social Psychology
,
28,3–22.
Brown ,
J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001).
Great expectations: Optimism and
pessimism in achievement settings.In E. C. Chang (Ed.),
Optimism and
pe
ssimism: Implications for theory,
research, and practice
(pp. 239 –25 5).
Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Brown , J. D., & Smart, S. A. (1991).
The self and social conduct: Linking
self-representations to prosocial be-havior.
Journal of Personality and
So
cial Psychology
,60,3 6 8 –375.
Brown , R. J., & Donderi, D. C. (1986).
Dream content and self-reported
well-being among recurrent drea-
mers, past-recurrent dreamers, and
nonrecurrent dreamers.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
50,
612–623.
Brown , S. (1998, April 20). Television
violence stays constant.
Broadcasting and Cable
,128, 20.
Bruch , M. A., Hamer, R. J., &
Heimberg, R. G. (1995). Shynessand public self-consciousness:Additive or interactive relation withsocial interaction?
Journal of
Personality
,63,4 7 –63.Bruch , M. A., Kaflowitz, N. G., & Pearl,
L. (1988). Mediated and nonme-
diated relationships of personality
components to loneliness.
Journal
of
Social and Clinical Psychology
,
6,
346–355.
Brunstein , J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005).
Implicit and self-attributed motives
to achieve: Two separate but inter-acting needs.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,89,
205–222.
Brunstein ,
J. C., & Schmitt, C. H.
(2004). Assessing individual differ-
ences in achievement motivation
with the Implicit Association Test.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
38,
536–555.
Buchholz, E. S., & Helbraun, E. (1999).
A psychobiological developmentalmodel for an “alonetime ”need in
infancy.
Bulletin of the Menninger
Clinic
,63, 143 –158.
Buckley , M. E., Klein, D. N., Durbin,
C. E., Hayden, E. P., & Moerk,
K. C. (2002). Development and
validation of a Q-Sort procedure to
assess temperament and behavior inpreschool-age children.
Journal of
Clinical
Child and Adolescent
Psychology
,31, 525 –539.
Buhler , C., & Allen, M. (1972).
Introduction to humanistic psy-
chology
. Monterey, CA: Brooks/
Cole.
Bulkeley ,
K. (1997).
An introduction to
the
psychology of dreaming
.
Westport,
CT: Praeger.
Bullock , W. A., & Gilliland, K. (1993).
Eysenck ’s arousal theory of
introversion-extraversion: A con-
verging measures investigation.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,64, 113 –123.
Bunde , J., & Suls, J. (2006). A quanti-
tative analysis of the relationship
between the Cook-Medley Hostility
Scale and traditional coronary
artery disease risk factors.
Health
Psychology
,25, 493 –500.
Burger , J. M. (1984). Desire for control,
locus of control, and proneness todepression.
Journal of Personality
,
52,7
1–89.
Burger , J. M. (1995). Individual differ-
ences in preference for solitude.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
29,8
5–108.
Burger , J. M. (1998). Solitude. In
H. S. Friedman (Ed.),
The ency-
clopedia
of mental health
(pp. 563 –569). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.Burger , J. M., & Arkin, R. M. (1980).
Prediction, control and learned
helplessness.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,38,
482–491.
Burger ,
J. M., & Cosby, M. (1999). Do
women prefer dominant men? The
case of the missing control condi-
tion.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,33, 358 –368.
Burns , J. W., & Evon, D. (2007).
Common and specific process fac-tors in cardiac rehabilitation:Independent and interactive effects
of the working alliance and self-
efficacy.
Health Psychology
,26,
684–692.
Burns ,
M. O., & Seligman, M. E. P.
(1989). Explanatory style across the
life span: Evidence for stability over
52 years.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,56, 471 –477.
Bushman , B. J. (1995). Moderating role
of trait aggressiveness in the effectsof violent media on aggression.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,69, 950 –960.
Bushman , B. J. (2002). Does venting
anger feed or extinguish the flame?
Catharsis, rumination, distraction,
anger, and aggressive responding.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,28, 724 –731.
Bushman , B. J., Baumeister, R. F., &
Phillips, C. M. (2001). Do peopleaggress to improve their mood?Catharsis beliefs, affect regulationopportunity, and aggressive re-
sponding.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,81,1 7 –32.
Bushman , B. J., Baumeister, R. F., &
Stack, A. D. (1999). Catharsis, ag-gression, and persuasive influence:
Self-fulfilling or self-defeating
prophecies?
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,76,
367–376.
Bushman ,
B. J., Bonacci, A. M.,
Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., &Miller, N. (2005). Chewing on it
can chew you up: Effects of rumi-
nation on triggered displaced ag-gression.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,88, 969 –983.
Bushman , B. J., & Huesmann, L. R.
(2001). Effects of televised violenceon aggression. In D. G. Singer &J. L. Singer (Eds.),
Handbook of
children
and the media
(pp. 223 –
254).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Buss, A. H. (1980).
Self-consciousness
and
social anxiety
. San Francisco:
W.
H. Freeman.REFERENCES 461 |
Buss, A. H. (1991). The EAS theory of
temperament. In J. Strelau &
A. Angleitner (Eds.),
Explorations
in
temperament
(pp. 43 –60).
London:
Plenum.
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984).
Temperament: Early developing
personality
traits
. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Buss,
A. H., & Plomin, R. (1986). The
EAS approach to temperament. InR. Plomin & J. Dunn (Eds.),
The
study
of temperament: Changes,
continuities and challenges
(pp.
67–79).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of
human intrasexual competition:
Tactics of mate attraction.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,54, 616 –628.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in
human mate preferences:
Evolutionary hypotheses tested in
37 cultures.
Behavioral and Brain
Sciences
,12,1–49.
Buss,
D. M. (1990). The evolution of
anxiety and social exclusion.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,9, 196 –201.
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary per-
sonality psychology.
Annual
Review
of Psychology
,42,
459–491.
Buss,
D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psy-
chology: A new paradigm for
psychological science.
Psychological Inquiry
,6,1–30.
Buss,
D. M. (1997). Evolutionary foun-
dations of personality. In R. Hogan,
J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.),
Handbook of personality psychol-ogy
(pp. 317 –344). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Buss, D. M. (2009). The great struggles
of life: Darwin and the emergenceof evolutionary psychology.
American Psychologist
,64,
140–148.
Buss,
D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986).
Preferences in human mate selec-
tion.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,50, 559 –570.
Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G.,
Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., &Wakefield, J. C. (1998).
Adaptations, exaptations, and
spandrels.
American Psychologist
,
53,
533–548.
Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K.
(1997). From vigilance to violence:Mate retention tactics in marriedcouples.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,72, 346 –361.Bussey , K., & Bandura, A. (1999).
Social cognitive theory of gender
development and differentiation.
Psychological Review
,106,
676–713.
Butcher ,
J. N. (Ed.). (2006).
MMPI-2:
A
practitioner ’s guide
. Washington,
DC:
American Psychological
Association.
Butler , A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman,
E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The
empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review ofmeta-analyses.
Clinical Psychology
Review
,26,1 7 –31.
Butler , J. M. (1968). Self-ideal congru-
ence in psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research
and
Practice
,5,1 3 –17.
Byrne , D. (1964). Repression-
sensitization as a dimension of
personality. In B. A. Maher (Ed.),
Progress in experimental personal-
ity
research
(Vol. 1, pp. 169 –220).
New York: Academic Press.
Cacioppo , J. T., Ernest, J. M., Burleson,
M. H., McClintock, M. K.,
Malarkey, W. B., Hawkley, L. C.,
et al. (2000). Lonely traits andconcomitant physiological pro-
cesses: The MacArthur Social
Neuroscience Studies.
International
Journal
of Psychophysiology
,35,
143–154.
Cacioppo ,
J. T., Hawkley, L. C.,
Berntson, G. G., Ernst, J. M.,
Gibbs, A. C., Stickgold, R., &Hobson, J. A. (2002). Do lonely
days invade the night? Potential
social modulation of sleep effi-
ciency.
Psychological Science
,13,
384–387.
Cacioppo ,
J. T., Hawkley, L. C.,
Crawford, E., Ernst, J. M.,
Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski,R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and
health: Potential mechanisms.
Psychosomatic Medicine
,64,
407–417.
Cacioppo ,
J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Ernst,
J. M., Burleson, M., Berntson,
G. G., Nouriani, B., et al. (2006).
Loneliness within a nomologicalnet: An evolutionary perspective.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
40,
1054 –1085.
Cacioppo , J. T., & Patrick, B. (2008).
Loneliness: Human nature and the
need
for social connection
. New
York:
Norton.
Cain, D. J., & Seeman, J. (2002).
Humanistic psychotherapies:Handbook
of research and practice
.Washington,
DC: American
Psychological Association.
Cairns , R. B. (1986). An evolutionary
and developmental perspective on
aggressive patterns. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, &
R. Iannotti (Eds.),
Altruism and
aggression:
Biological and social
origins
(pp. 58 –87). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Caldwell , D. F., & Burger, J. M. (1998).
Personality characteristics of job
applicants and success in screeninginterviews.
Personnel Psychology
,
51,
119–136.
Camara , W. J., Nathan, J. S., & Puente,
A. E. (2000). Psychological test us-age: Implications in professional
psychology.
Professional
Psychology:
Research and Practice
,
31,
141–154.
Cameron , L. D., & Nicholls, G. (1998).
Expression of stressful experiences
through writing: Effects of a self-
regulation manipulation for pessi-mists and optimists.
Health
Psychology
,17,8 4 –92.
Campbell , J. B. (1983). Differential re-
lationships of extraversion, impul-
sivity, and sociability to study
habits.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,17, 308 –314.
Campbell , J. B., & Hawley, C. W.
(1982). Study habits and Eysenck ’s
theory of extraversion-introversion.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
16,
139–146.
Campbell , J. D., & Fairey, P. J. (1985).
Effects of self-esteem, hypothetical
explanations, and verbalization
of expectancies on future perfor-mance.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,48,
1097 –1111.
Campbell ,
L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J.,
& Kashy, D. A. (2005). Perceptions
of conflict and support in romantic
relationships: The role of attach-ment anxiety.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
88,
510–531.
Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M.
E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg,C. (1983). Socioemotional devel-
opment. In M. M. Haith &
J. J. Campos (Eds.),
Handbook of
child
psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy
and psychobiology
(pp. 783 –915).
New
York: Wiley.
Canli, T. (2004). Functional brain
mapping of extraversion and neu-roticism: Learning from individual
differences in emotion processing.462 REFERENCES |
Journal of Personality ,72,
1105 –1132.
Canli ,
T. (2006). Genomic imaging of
extraversion. In T. Canli (Ed.),
Biology of personality and individ-
ual
differences
(pp. 93 –115). New
York: Guilford.
Cantor , N., Norem, J. K., Niedenthal,
P. M., Langston, C. A., & Brower,A. M. (1987). Life tasks, self-
concept ideals, and cognitive strat-
egies in a life transition.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
53,
1178 –1191.
Cappella , E., & Weinstein, R. S. (2001).
Turning around reading achieve-ment: Predictors of high schoolstudents ’academic resilience.
Journal of Educational Psychology
,
93,
758–771.
Card , N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani,
G. M., & Little, T. D. (2008).
Direct and indirect aggression dur-
ing childhood and adolescence:A meta-analytic review of genderdifferences, intercorrelations, and
relations to maladjustment.
Child
Development
,79, 1185 –1229.
Carlson , M., Marcus-Newhall, A., &
Miller, N. (1990). Effects of
situational aggression cues: A
quantitative review.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
58,
622–633.
Carnagey , N. L., & Anderson, C. A.
(2005). The effects of reward and
punishment in violent video gameson aggressive affect, cognition, and
behavior.
Psychological Science
,
16,
882–889.
Carnelley , K. B., Pietromonaco, P. R., &
Jaffe, K. (1994). Depression, work-ing models of others, and relation-
ship functioning.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
66,
127–140.
Carvajal , S. C., Clair, S. D., Nash, S. G.,
& Evans, R. I. (1998). Relatingoptimism, hope, and self-esteem tosocial influences in deterring sub-
stance use in adolescents.
Journal of
Social
and Clinical Psychology
,17,
443–465.
Carver ,
C. S. (1997). Adult attachment
and personality: Converging evi-
dence and a new measure.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,23, 865 –883.
Carver , C. S. (2001). Affect and the
functional bases of behavior: On thedimensional structure of affectiveexperience.
Personality and Social
Ps
ychology Review
,5,3 4 5 –356.Carver , C. S., Lehman, J. M., & Antoni,
M. H. (2003). Dispositional pessi-
mism predicts illness-related dis-
ruption of social and recreationalactivities among breast cancer pa-tients.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,84, 813 –821.
Carver , C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D.,
Noriega, V., Scheier, M. F.,
Robinson, D. S., Ketcham, A. S.,
Moffat, F. L., & Clark, K. C.
(1993). How coping mediates theeffect of optimism on distress: Astudy of women with early stage
breast cancer.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
65,
375–390.
Carver , C. S., Smith, R. G., Antoni,
M. H., Petronis, V. M., Weiss, S., &
Derhagopian, R. P. (2005).
Optimistic personality and psycho-logical well-being during treatment
predict psychosocial well-being
among long-term survivors ofbreast cancer.
Health Psychology
,
24,
508–516.
Caspi , A. (1998). Personality develop-
ment across the life course. In
N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
Handbook of
child
psychology: Vol. 3, Social,
emotional and personality develop-ment
(5th ed., pp. 311 –388).
New
York: Wiley.
Caspi , A. (2000). The child is father of
the man: Personality continuities
from childhood to adulthood.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,78, 158 –172.
Caspi , A., Harrington, H., Milne, B.,
Amell, J. W., Theodore, R. F., &Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Children ’s
behavioral styles at age 3 are linked
to their adult personality traits at
age 26.
Journal of Personality
,71,
495–513.
Catalano ,
R., Dooley, D., Novaco,
R. W., Wilson, G., & Hough, R.
(1993). Using ECA survey data to
examine the effect of job layoffs onviolent behavior.
Hospital and
Community
Psychiatry
,44,
874–879.
Cattell ,
H. E. P. (2004). The Sixteen
Personality Factor (16PF)
Questionnaire. In M. J. Hilsenroth
& M. Hersen (Eds.),
Comprehensive handbook of psy-chological
assessment
(Vol. 2)
(pp.
39–49). New York: Wiley.
Cattell , H. E. P., & Mead, A. D. (2008).
The Sixteen Personality FactorQuestionnaire (16PF). In
G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, &D. H. Saklofske (Eds.).
The Sage
handbook
of personality theory and
assessment
(Vol. 2) (pp. 135 –159).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cattell , R. B. (1974). Raymond B.
Cattell. In G. Lindzey (Ed.),
A
history
of psychology in autobiog-
raphy
(Vol. 6, pp. 61 –100).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.
Cepeda-Benito , A., & Short, P. (1998).
Self-concealment, avoidance of
psychological services, and per-ceived likelihood of seeking profes-
sional help.
Journal of Counseling
Psychology
,45,5 8 –64.
Chang , E. C. (1996). Cultural differ-
ences in optimism, pessimism andcoping: Predictors of subsequent
adjustment in Asian American and
Caucasian American collegestudents.
Journal of Counseling
Psychology
,43, 113 –123.
Chang , E. C. (1998). Dispositional op-
timism and primary and secondary
appraisal of a stressor: Controlling
for confounding influences and re-
lations to coping and psychologicaland physical adjustment.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
1109 –1120.
Chang , E. C. (2001). Cultural influences
on optimism and pessimism:Differences in Western and Eastern
construals of the self. In E.
C. Chang (Ed.),
Optimism and pes-
si
mism: Implications for theory, re-
search, and practice
(pp. 257 –280).
Washington,
DC: American
Psychological Association.
Charles , S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz,
M. (2001). Age-related differences
and change in positive and negative
affect over 23 years.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
80,
136–151.
Cheek , J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981).
Shyness and sociability.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
41,
330–339.
Chelune , G. J., Sultan, F. E., &
Williams, C. L. (1980). Loneliness,self-disclosure, and interpersonaleffectiveness.
Journal of Counseling
Psychology
,27, 462 –468.
Cheng , C. (1999). Gender-role differ-
ences in susceptibility to the influ-
ence of support availability and
depression.
Journal of Personality
,
67,
439–467.
Cheng , C. (2001). Assessing coping
flexibility in real-life and laboratorysettings: A multi-method approach.REFERENCES 463 |
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,80, 814 –833.
Cheng , C. (2005). Processes underlying
gender-role flexibility: Do andro-
gynous individuals know more orknow how to cope?
Journal of
Personality
,73, 645 –673.
Cheng , C. (2009). Dialectical thinking
and coping flexibility: A multi-
method approach.
Journal of
Personality
,77, 471 –493.
Cheng , C., & Cheung, M. W. L. (2005).
Cognitive processes underlyingcoping flexibility: Differentiation
and integration.
Journal of
Personality
,73, 859 –886.
Chess , S., & Thomas, A. (1986).
Temperament in clinical practice
.
New
York: Guilford.
Chess , S., & Thomas, A. (1991).
Temperament and the concept of
goodness of fit. In J. Strelau &
A. Angleitner (Eds.),
Explorations
in
temperament
(pp. 15 –28).
London:
Plenum.
Chess , S., & Thomas, A. (1996).
Temperament: Theory and practice
.
New
York: Brunner/Mazel.
Chipperfield , J. G., & Perry, R. P.
(2006). Primary- and secondary-control strategies in later life:
Predicting hospital outcomes inmen and women.
Health
Psychology
,25, 226 –236.
Chitsabesan , P., Corbett, S., Walker, L.,
Spencer, J., & Barton, J. R. (2006).Describing clinical teachers ’charac-
teristics and behaviours using critical
incidents and repertory grids.
Medical Education
,40,6 4 5 –653.
Christensen , A., Atkins, D. C., Yi, J.,
Baucom, D. H., & George, W. H.(2009). Couple and individual
adjustment for 2 years following a
randomized clinical trial comparingtraditional versus integrative be-
havioral couple therapy.
Journal of
Clinical
and Consulting
Psychology
,74, 1180 –1191.
Christensen , P. N., & Kashy, D. A.
(1998). Perceptions of and by
lonely people in initial social inter-action.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,24, 322 –329.
Church, A. T. (Ed.). (2001). Culture and
personality [Special issue].
Journal
of
Personality
,69(6).
Church,
A. T., & Burke, P. J. (1994).
Exploratory and confirmatory testsof the Big Five and Tellegen ’s three-
and four-dimensional models.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,66,9 3 –114.Churchill , S. L. (2003). Goodness-of-fit
in early childhood settings.
Early
Childhood
Education Journal
,31,
113–118.
Clancy ,
S. M., & Dollinger, S. J. (1993).
Photographic depictions of theself: Gender and age differences in
social connectedness.
Sex Roles
,29,
477–495.
Clark ,
D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B.
A. (1999).
Scientific foundations of
cognitive
theory and therapy for
depression
. New York: Wiley.
Clark ,
D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1982).
Diurnal variations in clinicaldepression and accessibility ofmemories of positive and negativeexperiences.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,91,8 7 –95.
Clark , L. A. (2005). Temperament as a
unifying basis for personality and
psychopathology.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,114,
505–521.
Clark ,
L. A., & Watson, D. (1988).
Mood and the mundane: Relations
between daily life events and self-
reported mood.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
54,
296–308.
Clark , L. A., & Watson, D. (1999).
Temperament: A new paradigm fortrait psychology. In L. A. Pervin &O. P. John (Eds.),
Handbook of
personality:
Theory and research
(pp. 399 –423). New York:
Guilford.
Clearfield , M. W., & Nelson, N. M.
(2006). Sex differences in mothers ’
speech and play behavior with 6-9-and 14-month infants.
Sex Roles
,
54,
127–137.
Coe, W. C. (1989). Posthypnotic amne-
sia: Theory and research. InN. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.),
Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral
perspective
(pp. 110 –148). Buffalo,
NY: Prometheus.
Coe, W. C., & Sarbin, T. R. (1991).
Role theory: Hypnosis from adramaturgical and narrational per-
spective. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue(Eds.),
Theories of hypnosis:
Current
models and perspectives
(pp. 303 –323). New York:
Guilford.
Cohen , D. B. (1979).
Sleep and dream-
ing:
Origins, nature and function
.
New
York: Pergamon.
Cohen , S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P.,
Fireman, P., Gwaltney, J. M., Jr., &Newsom, J. T. (1995). State and
trait affect as predictors of objectiveand subjective symptoms of respi-
ratory viral infections.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
68,
159–169.
Cohen , S., Doyle, W., Turner, R., Alper,
C. M., & Skoner, D. P. (2003).
Sociability and susceptibility to the
common cold.
Psychological
Science
,14, 389 –395.
Cohen , S., & Janicki-Deverts, D. (2009).
Can we improve our physical healthby altering our social networks?
Perspectives on PsychologicalScience
,4, 375 –378.
Cohen , S., & Pressman, S. D. (2006).
Positive affect and health.
Current
Directions
in Psychological Science
,
15,
122–125.
Cohen , S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress,
social support, and the bufferinghypothesis.
Psychological Bulletin
,
98,
310–357.
Cohn, N. B., & Strassberg, D. S. (1983).
Self-disclosure reciprocity amongpreadolescents.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,9,
97–102.
Coleman ,
M., & Ganong, L. H. (1985).
Love and sex-role stereotypes: Do“macho” men and “feminine ”
women make better lovers?
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,49, 170 –176.
Collins , N. L., Cooper, M. L., Albino,
A., & Allard, L. (2002).Psychosocial vulnerability fromadolescence to adulthood: A pro-spective study of attachment style
differences in relationship func-
tioning and partner choice.
Journal
of
Personality
,70, 965 –1008.
Collins , N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2000).
A safe haven: An attachment theoryperspective on support seeking andcaregiving in intimate relationships.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,78, 1053 –1073.
Collins , N. L., Ford, M. B., Guichard,
A. C., & Allard, L. M. (2006).
Working models of attachment
and attribution process in intimate
relationships.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,32,
201–219.
Collins ,
N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994).
Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review.
Psychological
Bulletin
,116, 457 –475.
Collins , N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990).
Adult attachment, working models,and relationship quality in datingcouples.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,58, 644 –663.464 REFERENCES |
Compas , B. E., Worsham, N. L.,
Ey, S., & Howell, D. C. (1996).
When Mom or Dad has cancer: II.
Coping, cognitive appraisals, andpsychological distress in children ofcancer patients.
Health Psychology
,
15,
167–175.
Conley , J. J. (1984). Longitudinal
consistency of adult personality:Self-reported psychological charac-
teristics across 45 years.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
47,
1325 –1333.
Conley , J. J. (1985). Longitudinal
stability of personality traits: A
multitrait-multimethod-multi-
occasion analysis.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
49,
1266 –1282.
Constantinople , A. (1973). Masculinity-
femininity: An exception to afamous dictum.
Psychological
Bulletin
,80, 389 –407.
Conti , D. (2005, February 1). Convicted
murderer will get new trial.
Pittsburgh Tribune Review
.
Conti ,
R. (2001). Time flies:
Investigating the connection be-tween intrinsic motivation and theexperience of time.
Journal of
Personality
,69,1–26.
Conway ,
F., Magai, C., Springer, C., &
Jones, S. C. (2008). Optimism and
pessimism as predictors of physical
and psychological health among
grandmothers raising their grand-children.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,42, 1352 –1357.
Conway , M. A., & Holmes, A. (2004).
Psychosocial stages and the acces-sibility of autobiographical memo-ries across the life cycle.
Journal of
Personality
,72, 461 –480.
Cooper , H. M., & Good, T. E. (1983).
Pygmalion grows up: Studies in the
expectation
communication pro-
cess
. New York: Longman.
Cooper ,
M. L., Wood, P. K., Orcutt,
H. K., & Albino, A. (2003).Personality and the predisposition
to engage in risky or problem be-
haviors during adolescence.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,84, 390 –410.
Cooper , T., Detre, T., & Weiss, S. M.
(1981). Coronary-prone behaviorand coronary heart disease: Acritical review.
Circulation
,63,
1199 –1215.
Coopman ,
S. J. (1997). Personal
constructs and communication ininterpersonal and organizational
contexts. In G. J. Neimeyer &R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.),
Advances
in
personal construct psychology
(Vol. 4, pp. 101 –147). Greenwich,
CT: JAI Press.
Coplan , R. J., Barber, A. M., & Lagace-
Seguin, D. G. (1999). The role ofchild temperament as a predictor of
early literacy and numeracy skills inpreschoolers.
Early Childhood
Research
Quarterly
,14, 537 –553.
Coplan , R. J., Bowker, A., & Cooper,
S. M. (2003). Parenting daily has-sles, child temperament, and socialadjustment in preschool.
Early
Childhood
Research Quarterly
,18,
376–395.
Cordova ,
J. V., Gee, C. B., & Warren,
L. Z. (2005). Emotional skillfulness
in marriage: Intimacy as a mediator
of the relationship between emo-tional skillfulness and marital sat-isfaction.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,24, 218 –235.
Cornell , D. G. (1997). Post hoc expla-
nation is not prediction.
American
Psychologist
,52, 1380.
Cornette , M. M., Strauman, T. J.,
Abramson, L. Y., & Busch, A. M.(2008). Self-discrepancy and sui-cidal ideation.
Cognition and
Emotion
,23, 504 –527.
Costa , P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992).
Professional manual for the NEO
PI-R
. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment
Resources.
Costa , P. T., Terracciano, A., &
McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender
differences in personality traits
across cultures: Robust and sur-
prising findings.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
81,
322–331.
Costello , C. G. (1978). A critical review
of Seligman ’s laboratory experi-
ments on learned helplessness anddepression in humans.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,87,2 1 –31.
Cowen , E. I., Wyman, P. A., & Work,
W. C. (1992). The relationship be-
tween retrospective reports of early
child temperament and adjustment
at ages 10 –12.
Journal of Abnormal
Child
Psychology
,20,3 9 –50.
Cox-Fuenzalida , L., Angie, A.,
Holloway, S., & Sohl, L. (2006).Extraversion and task performance:A fresh look through the workloadhistory lens.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,40, 432 –439.
Cox-Fuenzalida , L., Gilliland, K., &
Swickert, R. J. (2001). Congruency
of the relationship between extra-
version and the brainstem auditoryevoked response based on the EPI
versus the EPQ.
Journal of Research
in
Personality
,35, 117 –126.
Crabbe , J. C. (2002). Genetic contribu-
tions to addiction.
Annual Review
of
Psychology
,53, 435 –462.
Craik , K. H. (1986). Personality re-
search methods: An historical
perspective.
Journal of Personality
,
54,1
8–51.
Cramer , K. M., & Lake, R. P. (1998).
The Preference for Solitude Scale:Psychometric properties and factorstructure.
Personality and Individual
Di
fferences
,24,1 9 3 –199.
Cramer , K. M., Ofosu, H. B., & Barry,
J. E. (2000). An abbreviated form
of the social and emotional loneli-
ness scale for adults (SELSA).
Personality and Individual
Differences
,28, 1125 –1131.
Cramer , P. (1991).
The development of
defense
mechanisms: Theory, re-
search, and assessment
. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Cramer ,
P. (1997). Evidence for change
in children ’s use of defense me-
chanisms.
Journal of Personality
,
65,
233–247.
Cramer , P. (1998a). Freshman to senior
year: A follow-up study of identity,
narcissism, and defense mechan-
isms.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,32, 156 –172.
Cramer , P. (1998b). Threat to gender
representation: Identity and identi-fication.
Journal of Personality
,66,
335–357.
Cramer ,
P. (1999). Personality, person-
ality disorders, and defense me-chanisms.
Journal of Personality
,
67,
535–554.
Cramer , P. (2000). Defense mechanisms
in psychology today: Further pro-cesses for adaptation.
American
Psychologist
,55, 637 –646.
Cramer , P. (2002). Defense mechanisms,
behavior, and affect in youngadulthood.
Journal of Personality
,
70,
103–126.
Cramer , P. (2006).
Protecting the self:
Defense
mechanisms in action
. New
York:
Guilford.
Cramer , P. (2007). Longitudinal study
of defense mechanisms: Late child-
hood to late adolescence.
Journal of
Personality
,75,1–23.
Cramer ,
P., & Blatt, S. J. (1990). Use
of the TAT to measure change in
defense mechanisms followingintensive psychotherapy.
Journal of
Personality
Assessment
,54,
236–251.REFERENCES 465 |
Cramer , P., Blatt, S. J., & Ford, R. Q.
(1988). Defense mechanisms in the
anaclitic and introjective personal-
ity configuration.
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,56, 610 –616.
Cramer , P., & Block, J. (1998).
Preschool antecedents of defensemechanism use in young adults: Alongitudinal study.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
159–169.
Cramer , P., & Brilliant, M. A. (2001).
Defense use and defense under-
standing in children.
Journal of
Personality
,69, 297 –322.
Cramer , R. E., Manning-Ryan, B.,
Johnson, L. M., & Barbo, E.(2000). Sex differences in subjective
distress to violations of trust:
Extending an evolutionary per-spective.
Basic and Applied Social
Psychology
,22, 101 –109.
Crane , F. G., & Crane, E. C. (2007).
Dispositional optimism and en-
trepreneurial success.
Psychologist-
Manager
Journal
,10,1 3 –25.
Crick , F., & Mitchison, G. (1983). The
function of dream sleep.
Nature
,
304,
111–114.
Crick , N. R., & Dodge, K, A. (1996).
Social information-processing me-
chanisms in reactive and proactiveaggression.
Child Development
,67,
993–1002.
Crocker ,
J., Brook, A. T., Niiya, Y., &
Villacorta, M. (2006). The pursuitof self-esteem: Contingencies of
self-wroth and self-regulation.
Journal of Personality
,74,
1749 –177.
Crocker ,
J., Karpinski, A., Quinn,
D. M., & Chase, S. K. (2003).
When grades determine self-worth:
Consequences of contingent self-worth for male and female engi-
neering and psychology majors.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,85, 507 –516.
Crocker , J., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2003).
Level of self-esteem and contingen-
cies of self-worth: Unique effects onacademic, social, and financialproblems in college students.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,29, 701 –712.
Crocker , J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper,
M. L., & Bouvrette, A. (2003).
Contingencies of self-worth in
college students: Theory and mea-
surement.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,85,
894–908.Crocker ,
J., & Park, L. E. (2003).
Seeking self-esteem: Construction,
maintenance, and protection of
self-worth. In M. R. Leary &J. P. Tangney (Eds.),
Handbook of
self
and identity
(pp. 291 –313).
New
York: Guilford.
Crocker , J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The
costly pursuit of self-esteem.
Psychological Bulletin
,130,
392–414.
Crocker ,
J., Sommers, S. R., &
Luhtanen, R. K. (2002). Hopes
dashed and dreams fulfilled:
Contingencies of self-worth and
graduate school admissions.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,28, 1275 –1286.
Crocker , J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001).
Contingencies of self-worth.
Psychological Review
,108,
593–623.
Cross ,
S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris,
M. L. (2000). The relational-
interdependent self-construaland relationships.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
78,
791–808.
Cross , S. E., & Madson, L. (1997).
Models of the self: Self-construals
and gender.
Psychological Bulletin
,
122,5–37.
Cross ,
S. E., Morris, M. L., & Gore, J. S.
(2002). Thinking about oneselfand others: The relational-
interdependent self-construal andsocial cognition.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
82,
399–418.
Crossen , C. (1996, March 8). Solitude is
a casualty of the war with time.
Wall Street Journal
, p. R4.
Crost ,
N. W., Pauls, C. A., & Wacker, J.
(2008). Defensiveness and anxietypredict frontal EEG asymmetryonly in specific situational contexts.
Biological Psychology
,78,4 3 –52.
Crowson , J. J., & Cromwell, R. L.
(1995). Depressed and normal in-
dividuals differ both in selection
and in perceived tonal quality of
positive-negative messages.
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology
,104,
305–311.
Csikszentmihalyi ,
M. (1990).
Flow: The
psychology
of optimal experience
.
New
York: Harper & Row.
Csikszentmihalyi , M. (1999). If we are
so rich, why aren ’t we happy?
American Psychologist
,54,
821–827.
Csikszentmihalyi ,
M., &
Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988).Optimal experience: Psychological
studies
of flow in consciousness
.
New
York: Cambridge.
Csikszentmihalyi ,M . ,&L e F e v r e ,J .
(1989). Optimal experience in work
and leisure.
Journal of Personality
an
d Social Psychology
,56,8 1 5 –822.
Cunningham, J. A., Strassberg, D. S., &
Haan, B. (1986). Effects of intimacy
and sex-role congruency of self-
disclosure.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,4, 393 –401.
Cunningham, M. R. (1988). Does hap-
piness mean friendliness? Inducedmood and heterosexual self-
disclosure.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,14, 283 –297.
Curran , J. P., Wallander, J. L., &
Fischetti, M. (1980). The impor-
tance of behavioral and cognitivefactors in heterosexual-social anxi-ety.
Journal of Personality
,48,
285–292.
Cury ,
F., Elliot, A. J., Da Fonseca, D., &
Moller, A. C. (2006). The social-cognitive model of achievement
motivation and the 2 X 2 achieve-
ment goal framework.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
90,
666–679.
Cutrona , C. E. (1982). Transition to
college: Loneliness and the processof social adjustment. In L. A.Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.),
Loneliness
(pp. 291 –309). New
York: Wiley.
Cyranowski , J. M., & Andersen, B. L.
(2000). Evidence of self-schematic
cognitive processing of women with
differing sexual self-views.
Journal
of
Social and Clinical Psychology
,
19,
519–543.
Dadds , M. R., Bovbjerg, D. H., Redd,
W. H., & Cutmore, T. R. H.(1997). Imagery in human classicalconditioning.
Psychological
Bulletin
,122,8 9 –103.
Danoff-Burg , S., Revenson, T. A.,
Trudeau, K. J., & Paget, S. A.
(2004). Unmitigated communion,
social constraints, and psychologi-
cal distress among women withrheumatoid arthritis.
Journal of
Personality
,72,2 9 –46.
Darnon , C., Harackiewicz, J. M.,
Butera, F., Mugny, G., &Quiamzade, A. (2007).Performance-approach and
performance-avoidance goals:
When uncertainty makes a differ-ence.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,33,
813–827.466 REFERENCES |
Davidson , K., & MacGregor, M. W.
(1996). Reliability of an idiographic
Q-Sort measure of defense me-
chanisms.
Journal of Personality
Assessment
,66, 624 –639.
Davidson , K., & MacGregor, M. W.
(1998). A critical appraisal ofself-report defense mechanismmeasures.
Journal of Personality
,
66,
965–992.
Davidson , K., MacGregor, M. W.,
Johnson, E. A., Woody, E. Z., &Chaplin, W. F. (2004). The relationbetween defense use and adaptive
behavior.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,38, 105 –129.
Davidson , K. W., Gidron, Y.,
Mostofsky, E., & Trudeau, K. J.(2007). Hospitalization cost offset
of a hostility intervention for coro-
nary heart disease patients.
Journal
of
Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,75, 657 –662.
Davidson , R. J. (1991). Biological ap-
proaches to the study of personality.
In V. J. Derlega, B. A. Winstead, &
W. H. Jones (Eds.),
Personality:
Co
ntemporary theory and research
(pp. 87 –11 2). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Davidson , R. J., Ekman, P., Saron,
C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Friesen,
W. V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymme-try: Emotional expression and brain
physiology I.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,58,
330–341.
Davidson ,
R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1982).
Asymmetrical brain activity discri-
minates between positive versus
negative affective stimuli in humaninfants.
Science
,218, 1235 –1237.
Davidson , R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1989).
Frontal brain asymmetry predictsinfants ’response to maternal
separation.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,98, 127 –131.
Davidson , R. J., & Tomarken, A. J.
(1989). Laterality and emotion: An
electrophysiological approach. In
F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.),
Handbook of neuropsychology
(Vol. 3, pp. 419 –441). New York:
Elsevier Science.
Davila , J., Burge, D., & Hammen, C.
(1997). Why does attachment style
change?
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,73, 826 –838.
Davila , J., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury,
T. N. (1999). Attachment changeprocesses in the early years of mar-riage.
Journal of Personality and
So
cial Psychology
,76, 783 –802.Davis , J. D. (1976). Self-disclosure
in an acquaintance exercise:
Responsibility for level of intimacy.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,33, 787 –792.
Davis , J. D. (1977). Effects of commu-
nication about interpersonal
process on the evolution of self-disclosure in dyads.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
35,3
1–37.
Davis , P. J. (1999). Gender differences in
autobiographical memory forchildhood emotional experiences.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,76, 498 –510.
Dawes , R. M. (1994).
House of cards:
Psychology
and psychotherapy
built on myth
. New York: Free
Press.
Deckers ,
L., & Carr, D. E. (1986).
Cartoons varying in low-level pain
ratings, not aggression ratings,
correlate positively with funninessratings.
Motivation and Emotion
,
10,
207–216.
Deffenbacher , J., Huff, M., Lynch, R.,
Oetting, E., & Salvatore, N. (2000).Characteristics and treatments ofhigh-anger drivers.
Journal of
Counseling
Psychology
,47,5–17.
del
Valle , C. H. C., & Mateos, P. M.
(2008). Dispositional pessimism,
defensive pessimism and optimism:
The effect of induced mood on
prefactual and counterfactualthinking and performance.
Cognition and Emotion
,22,
1600 –1612.
Dembroski ,
T. M., & Costa, P. T.
(1987). Coronary-prone behavior:Components of the Type A pattern
and hostility.
Journal of
Personality
,55, 211 –235.
DeNeve , K. M. (1999). Happy as an
extraverted clam? The role of per-sonality for subjective well-being.
Current Directions in Psychological
Science
,8, 141 –144.
DeNeve , K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998).
The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits andsubjective well-being.
Psychological
Bu
lletin
,12 4 ,1 9 7 –229.
Denson , T. F., Aviles, F. E., Pollock,
V. E., Earleywine, M., Vasquez,E. A., & Miller, N. (2008). Theeffects of alcohol and the salience of
aggressive cues on triggered dis-
placed aggression.
Aggressive
Behavior
,34,2 5 –33.
DePaulo , B. M., Dull, W. R., Greenberg,
J. M., & Swaim, G. W. (1989). Areshy people reluctant to ask for help?
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,56, 834 –844.
DePaulo , B. M., Epstein, J. A., &
LeMay, C. S. (1990). Responses of
the socially anxious to the prospect
of interpersonal evaluation.
Journal
of
Personality
,58, 623 –640.
DePaulo , B. M., Kenny, D. A., Hoover,
C. W., Webb, W., & Oliver, P. V.(1987). Accuracy of person percep-
tion: Do people know what kinds
of impressions they convey?
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,52, 303 –315.
Depue , R. A., & Monroe, S. M. (1978).
Learned helplessness in the per-
spective of the depressive disorders:
Conceptual and definitional issues.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
87,3–20.
de
Rivera , J. (1997). The construction of
false memory syndrome: The expe-
rience of retractors.
Psychological
Inquiry
,8, 271 –292.
Derlega , V. J., & Chaikin, A. L. (1976).
Norms affecting self-disclosure in
men and women.
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,44, 376 –380.
Derlega , V. J., Wilson, M., & Chaikin,
A. L. (1976). Friendship anddisclosure reciprocity.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
34,
578–582.
Derlega , V. J., Winstead, B. A., &
Greene, K. (2008). Self-disclosureand starting a close relationship. In
S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel & J. Harvey
(Eds.),
Handbook of relationship
initiation
(pp. 153 –174).
New
York: Psychology Press.
Derry , P. A., & Kuiper, N. A. (1981).
Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
90,
286–297.
De Schipper, J. C., Tavecchio, L. W. C.,
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & VanZeijl, J. (2004). Goodness-of-fit in
center day care: Relations of tem-
perament, stability, and quality ofcare with the child ’s adjustment.
Early Childhood Research
Quarterly
,19, 257 –272.
DeSteno , D., Bartlett, M. Y., Braverman,
J., & Salovey, P. (2002). Sex dif-
ferences in jealousy: Evolutionary
mechanism or artifact of measure-
ment?
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,83, 1103 –1116.
DeVellis , R. F., DeVellis, B. M., &
McCauley, C. (1978). VicariousREFERENCES 467 |
acquisition of learned helplessness.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,36, 894 –899.
Dickens , W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2006).
Black Americans reduce the racial
IQ gap: Evidence from standardi-
zation samples.
Psychological
Science
,17, 913 –920.
Diehl , M., Elnick, A. B., Bourbeau, L. S.,
& Labouvie-Vief, G. (1998). Adultattachment styles: Their relations to
family context and personality.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,74, 1656 –1669.
Diener , E. (1984). Subjective well-being.
Psychological Bulletin
,95,
542–575.
Diener ,
E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-
cultural correlates of life satisfac-
tion and self-esteem.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
68,
653–663.
Diener , E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984).
The independence of positive and
negative affect.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
47,
1105 –1117.
Diener , E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002).
Very happy people.
Psychological
Science
,13,8 1 –84.
Digman , J. M. (1989). Five robust trait
dimensions: Development, stability,and utility.
Journal of Personality
,
57,
195–214.
Digman , J. M. (1990). Personality
structure: Emergence of the five-factor model.
Annual Review of
Psychology
,41, 417 –440.
Digman , J. M., & Inouye, J. (1986).
Further specification of the fiverobust factors of personality.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,50, 116 –123.
DiLalla , D. L., Carey, G., Gottesman,
I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (1996).
Heritability of MMPI personality
indicators of psychopathology in
twins reared apart.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,105,
491–499.
Dindia ,
K., & Allen, M. (1992). Sex
differences in self-disclosure: Ameta-analysis.
Psychological
Bulletin
,112, 106 –124.
Di Paula , A., & Campbell, J. D. (2002).
Self-esteem and persistence in theface of failure.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
83,
711–724.
Dodge , K. A. (2006). Translational sci-
ence in action: Hostile attributionalstyle and the development of
aggressive behavior problems.Development and Psychopathol-
ogy
,18, 791 –814.
Dodge , K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks,
V. S., Pettit, G. S., Price, J. M.,
Fontaine, R., et al. (2003). Peerrejection and social information-
processing factors in the develop-
ment of aggressive behaviorproblems in children.
Child
Development
,74, 374 –393.
Dodgson , P. G., & Wood, J. V. (1998).
Self-esteem and the cognitive ac-cessibility of strengths and weak-nesses after failure.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
75,
178–197.
Dollard , J., Doob, L., Miller, N. E.,
Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R.
(1939).
Frustration and aggression
.
New
Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Dollinger , S. J. (2000). Locus of control
and incidental learning: An appli-
cation to college student success.
College Student Journal
,34,
537–540.
Domhoff ,
G. W. (1996).
Finding
meaning
in dreams: A quantitative
approach
. New York: Plenum.
Domhoff ,
G. W. (1999). New directions
in the study of dream content usingthe Hall and Van de Castle codingsystem.
Dreaming
,9, 115 –137.
Domhoff , G. W. (2001). A new neuro-
cognitive theory of dreams.
Dreaming
,11,1 3 –33.
Domhoff , G. W. (2004). Why did em-
pirical dream researchers reject
Freud? A critique of historical
claims by Mark Solms.
Dreaming
,
14,3–17.
Dompnier ,
B., Darnon, C., & Butera, F.
(2009). Faking the desire to learn:A clarification of the link between
mastery goals and academicachievement.
Psychological Science
,
20,
939–943.
Donnellan , M. B., Conger, R. D., &
Bryant, C. M. (2004). The Big Fiveand enduring marriages.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,38,
481–504.
Donnelly ,
D. A., & Murray, E. J.
(1991). Cognitive and emotional
changes in written essays and ther-
apy interviews.
Journal of Social
and
Clinical Psychology
,10,
334–350.
Doob ,
L. W., & Sears, R. R. (1939).
Factors determining substitute be-havior and the overt expression ofaggression.
Journal of Abnormal and
So
cial Psychology
,34,2 9 3 –313.Doucet , C., & Stelmack, R. M. (2000).
An event-related potential analysis
of extraversion and individual dif-
ferences in cognitive processingspeed and response execution.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,78, 956 –964.
Dozois , D. J. A., & Dobson, K. S.
(2001). A longitudinal investigation
of information processing and
cognitive organization in clinical
depression: Stability of schematicinterconnectedness.
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,69, 914 –925.
Draguns , J. G. (2008). What have we
learned about the interplay of cul-
ture with counseling and psycho-
therapy? In U. P. Gielen, J.
G. Draguns, & J. M. Fish (Eds.),
Principles of multicultural counsel-ing
and therapy
(pp. 393 –417).
New
York: Routledge/Taylor &
Francis.
Duke , T., & Davidson, J. (2002).
Ordinary and recurrent dream
recall of active, past and non-
recurrent dreamers during and afteracademic stress.
Dreaming
,12,
185–197.
Dunbar ,
E. (2006, April 1). Bizarre
death four years after trial leads totossed conviction.
Associated Press
State
and Local Wire
. Retrieved
November
27, 2006, from http://
www.lexis-nexis.com
Durante , K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton,
M. G. (2008). Changes in women ’s
choice of dress across the ovulatorycycle: Naturalistic and laboratorytask-based evidence.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,34,
1451 –1460.
Dutton ,
K. A., & Brown, J. D. (1997).
Global self-esteem and specific self-views as determinants of people ’s
reactions to success and failure.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,73, 139 –148.
Dwivedi , Y., Mondal, A. C.,
Payappagoudar, G. V., & Rizavi,H. S. (2005). Differential regulationof serotonin (5HT)-sub(2A) recep-tor mRNA and protein levels after
single and repeated stress in rat
brain: Role in learned helplessnessbehavior.
Neuropharmacology
,48,
204–214.
Eagly,
A. H. (1997). Sex differences in
social behavior: Comparing socialrole theory and evolutionary psy-chology.
American Psychologist
,
52,
1380 –1382.468 REFERENCES |
Eastwick , P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008).
Sex differences in mate preferences
revisted: Do people know what
they initially desire in a romanticpartner?
Journal of Personality
an
d Social Psychology
,94,
24
5–264.
Eaves , L., & Eysenck, H. (1975).
Utilization of self-schemas as amechanism of interpretational bias
in children.
Social Cognition
,5,
280–300.
Eccles ,
J. (1985). Sex differences in
achievement patterns. In T.B. Sonderegger (Ed.),
Nebraska
symposium
on motivation
(Vol. 32 ,
pp. 97 –132). Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press.
Eccles , J. (2005). Subjective task value
and the Eccles et al. model of
achievement-related choices. InA. J. Elliott & C. W. Dweck (Eds.),
Handbook of competence and
motivation
(pp. 105 –121). New
York: Guilford Press.
Eisenberger , R., & Cameron, J. (1996).
Detrimental effects of reward:
Reality or myth?
American
Psychologist
,51, 1153 –1166.
Elliot , A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001).
A 2 X 2 achievement goal frame-
work.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,80, 501 –519.
Elliot , A. J., Shell, M. M., Bouas, H. K.,
& Maier, M. A. (2005).
Achievement goals, performancecontingencies, and performanceattainment: An experimental test.
Journal of Educational Psychology
,
97,
630–640.
Elliott , A. J., Miltenberger, R. G.,
Kaster-Bundgaard, J., & Lumley,
V. (1996). A national survey of
assessment and therapy techniquesused by behavior therapists.
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice
,
3,
107–125.
Elliott , R. (2002). The effectiveness of
humanistic therapies: A meta-analysis. In D. J. Cain & J. Seeman
(Eds.),
Humanistic psychotherapies:
Handbook
of research and practice
(pp. 57 –81). Washington, DC:
American Psychological
Association.
Ellis, A. E. (1971).
Growth through
reason:
Verbatim cases in rational-
emotive therapy
. North
Hollywood,
CA: Wilshire.
Ellis, A. E. (1987). The impossibility of
achieving consistently good mental
health.
American Psychologist
,42,
364–375.Ellis,
A. E. (2003). Early theories and
practices of rational emotive be-
havior therapy and how they have
been augmented and revised duringthe last three decades.
Journal of
Rational-Emotive
& Cognitive-
Behavior Therapy
,21, 219 –243.
Else-Quest , N. M., Hyde, J. S.,
Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle,
C. A. (2006). Gender differences in
temperament: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin
,132,3 3 –72.
Emmons , R. A., & Diener, E. (1986).
Influence of impulsivity and socia-bility on subjective well-being.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,50, 1211 –1215.
Endler , N. S., & Hunt, J. M. (1966).
Sources of behavioral variance asmeasured by the S-R inventory ofanxiousness.
Psychological
Bulletin
,65, 336 –346.
Endler , N. S., & Hunt, J. M. (1968). S-R
inventories of hostility and com-parisons of the proportions of var-iance from persons, responses, and
situations for hostility and anx-
iousness.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,9, 309 –315.
Epstein , S. (1979). The stability of be-
havior: I. On predicting most of thepeople much of the time.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
37,
1097 –1126.
Epstein , S. (1980). The stability of
behavior: II. Implications for psy-chological research.
American
Psychologist
,35, 790 –806.
Epstein , S. (1983). Aggregation and
beyond: Some basic issues on theprediction of behavior.
Journal of
Personality
,51, 360 –392.
Erickson , M. H. (1967).
Advanced
techniques
of hypnosis and therapy:
Selected papers of Milton H.
Erickson
. New York: Grune &
Stratton.
Erikson ,
E. H. (1950/1963).
Childhood
and
society
(2nd ed.). New York:
Norton.
Erikson ,
E. H. (1968).
Identity: Youth
and
crisis
. New York: Norton.
Erikson ,
E. H. (1975).
Life history and
the
historical moment
. New York:
Norton.
Ernst ,
J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1999).
Lonely hearts: Psychological per-
spectives on loneliness.
Applied
Preventive
Psychology
,8,1–22.
Eron ,
L. D. (1987). The development of
aggressive behavior from the per-
spective of a developing behaviorism.
American Psychologist
,42,4 3 5 –442.Etaugh , C., & Liss, M. B. (1992). Home,
school, and playroom: Training
grounds for adult gender roles.
Sex Roles
,26, 129 –147.
Evans , D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007).
Developing a model for adult tem-
perament.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,41, 868 –888.
Evans , J., Heron, J., Lewis, G., Araya,
R., & Wolke, D. (2005). Negativeself-schemas and the onset of de-
pression in women: Longitudinal
study.
British Journal of Psychiatry
,
186,
302–307.
Evans , K. K., & Singer, J. A. (1995).
Studying intimacy through dreamnarratives: The relationship ofdreams to self-report and projective
measures of personality.
Imagination, Cognition and
Personality
,14, 211 –226.
Evans , R. I. (1976).
The making of
psychology
. New York: Knopf.
Evans ,
W. P., Owens, P., & Marsh, S. C.
(2005). Environmental factors,
locus of control, and adolescent
suicide risk.
Child & Adolescent
Social
Work Journal
,22, 301 –319.
Eysenck , H. J. (1967).
The biological
basis
of personality
. Springfield, IL:
Charles
C. Thomas.
Eysenck , H. J. (1982). Development of a
theory. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.),
Personality, genetics and behavior:Selected
papers
(pp. 1– 38).
New
York: Praeger.
Eysenck , H. J. (1990). Biological
dimensions of personality. InL. Pervin (Ed.),
Handbook of
personality
theory and research
(pp. 244 –27 6). New York: Guilford.
Eysenck , H. J. (1997). Personality and
experimental psychology: The uni-
fication of psychology and the pos-sibility of a paradigm.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
73,
1224 –1237.
Eysenck , H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G.
(1968).
Manual for the Eysenck
Personality
Inventory
. San Diego,
CA:
Educational and Industrial
Testing Service.
Eysenck , S. B. G., & Long, F. Y. (1986).
A cross-cultural comparison of
personality in adults and children:
Singapore and England.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
50,
124–130.
Factbox : China ’s dreaded “gaokao, ”the
world ’s largest exam (2008, June
6).
Reuters
. Retrieved from http://
www.reuters.com/articlePrint?
articleId=USPEK26587420080606REFERENCES 469 |
Fancher , R. E. (2000). Snapshots of
Freud in America, 1899 –1999.
American Psychologist
,55,
1025 –1028.
Farabee ,
D. J., Holcom, M. L., Ramsey,
S. L., & Cole, S. G. (1993). Social
anxiety and speaker gaze in a
persuasive atmosphere.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,27,
365–376.
Farber ,
B. A. (2006).
Self-disclosure in
psychotherapy
. New York:
Guilford.
Farber ,
B. A., & Sohn, A. E. (2007).
Patterns of self-disclosure in
psychotherapy and marriage.
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
Practice,
Training
,44, 226 –231.
Farley , F. (2000). Hans J. Eysenck
(1916 –1997).
American
Psychologist
,55, 674 –675.
Fassler , O., Lynn, S. J., & Knox, J.
(2008). Is hypnotic suggestibility astable trait?
Consciousness and
Cognition
,17, 240 –253.
Feeney , B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001).
Predictors of caregiving in adultintimate relationships: An attach-ment theoretical perspective.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,80, 972 –994.
Feeney , B. C., & Kirkpatrick, L. A.
(1996). Effects of adult attachment
and presence of romantic partners
on physiological responses to stress.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,70, 255 –270.
Feeney , J. A., & Noller, P. (1990).
Attachment style as a predictor ofadult romantic relationships.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,58, 281 –291.
Feeney , J. A., Noller, P., & Patty, J.
(1993). Adolescents ’interactions
with the opposite sex: Influence ofattachment style and gender.
Journal of Adolescence
,16,
169–186.
Feingold ,
A. (1990). Gender differences
in effects of physical attractiveness
on romantic attraction: A compari-
son across five research paradigms.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,59, 981 –993.
Feingold , A. (1992). Gender differences
in mate selection preferences: A testof the parental investment model.
Psychological Bulletin
,112,
125–139.
Feist,
G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of
personality in scientific and artisticcreativity.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Review
,2, 290 –309.Feixas , G., Erazo-Caicedo, M. I., Harter,
S. L., & Bach, L. (2008).
Construction of self and others in
unipolar depressive disorders: Astudy using repertory grid tech-nique.
Cognitive Therapy and
Research
,32, 386 –400.
Feldman Barrett , L., Lane, R. D.,
Sechrest, L., & Schwartz, G. E.
(2000). Sex differences in emotional
awareness.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,26, 1027 –
1035.
Fenichel , O. (1945).
The psychoanalytic
theory
of neurosis
. New York:
Norton.
Ferguson ,
S. M., Brodkin, J. D., Lloyd,
G. K., & Menzaghi, F. (2000).Antidepressant-like effects of the
subtype selective nicotinic acetylcho-line receptor agonist, SIB-1508Y, inthe learned helplessness rat model of
depression.
Psychopharmacology
,
15
2,2 9 5 –303.
Findley , M. J., & Cooper, H. M. (1983).
Locus of control and academic
achievement: A literature review.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,44, 419 –427.
Finkel , D., & McGue, M. (1997). Sex
differences and nonadditivity inheritability of the multidimensionalpersonality questionnaire scales.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,72, 929 –938.
Finkelstein , L. E., & Levy, B. R. (2006).
Disclosure of Holocaust experi-
ences: Reasons, attributions and
health implications.
Journal of
Social
and Clinical Psychology
,25,
117–140.
Fischer ,
A. R., Jome, L. M., & Atkinson,
D. R. (1998). Reconceptualizing
multicultural counseling: Universal
healing conditions in a culturallyspecific context.
Counseling
Psychologist
,26, 525 –588.
Fischer , P., & Greitemeyer, T. (2006).
Music and aggression: The impact
of sexual-aggressive song lyrics on
aggression-related thoughts, emo-
tions, and behavior toward thesame and the opposite sex.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,32, 1165 –1176.
Fischer , R., & Chalmers, A. (2008). Is
optimism universal? A meta-
analytical investigation of optimism
across 22 nations.
Personality and
In
dividual Differences
,45,3 7 8 –382.
Fisher , M. (1995, December 6).
Freudian slip.
San Jose Mercury
News
, p. 25A.Fletcher ,
G. J. O., Tither, J. M.,
O’Loughlin, C., Friesen, M., &
Overall, N. (2004). Warm and
homely or cold and beautiful? Sexdifferences in trading off traits inmate selection.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,30,
659–672.
Floderus-Myrhed ,
B., Pedersen, N., &
Rasmuson, I. (1980). Assessment
of heritability for personality, based
on a short-form of the EysenckPersonality Inventory: A study of12,898 twin pairs.
Behavior
Genetics
,10, 153 –162.
Flynn , J. R. (1999). Searching for justice:
The discovery of IQ gains over
time.
American Psychologist
,54,
5–20.
Fodor ,
E. M., & Carver, R. A. (2000).
Achievement and power motives,
performance feedback, and creativ-
ity.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,34, 380 –396.
Folkman , S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980).
An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample.
Journal of
Health
and Social Behavior
,21,
219–239.
Fontaine ,
R. G., & Dodge, K. A. (2006).
Real-time decision making and ag-
gressive behavior in youth: A heu-
ristic model of response evaluationand decision (RED).
Aggressive
Behavior
,32, 604 –624.
Foulkes , D., & Cavallero, C. (1993).
Dreaming as cognition
. New York:
Harvester
Wheatsheaf.
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1986).
Taste-elicited changes in facial signsof emotion and the asymmetry ofbrain electrical activity in human
newborns.
Neuropsychologia
,24,
417–422.
Fox,
N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1987).
Electroencephalogram asymmetryin response to the approach of a
stranger and maternal separation of10-month-old infants.
Develop-
mental
Psychology
,23, 233 –240.
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1988).
Patterns of electrical activity duringfacial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants.
Developmental
Psychology
,24, 230 –236.
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K.
H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A.
(2001). Continuity and discontinu-
ity of behavioral inhibition and
exuberance: Psychophysiologicaland behavioral influences across thefirst four years of life.
Child
Development
,72,1–21.470 REFERENCES |
Fraley , R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1998).
Airport separation: A naturalistic
study of adult attachment dynamics
in separating couples.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
75,
1198 –1212.
Frankel , A., & Prentice-Dunn, S. (1990).
Loneliness and the processing ofself-relevant information.
Journal
of
Social and Clinical Psychology
,
9,
303–315.
Fransella , F. (2003).
International
handbook
of personal construct
psychology
. London: Wiley.
Fransella ,
F. (2005).
The essential prac-
titioner ’s
handbook of personal
construct psychology
. London:
Wiley.
Fransella ,
F., Bell, R., & Bannister, D.
(2003).
A manual for repertory grid
technique
(2nd ed.). London:
Wiley.
Frattaroli ,
J. (2006). Experimental
disclosure and its moderators:A meta-analysis.
Psychological
Bulletin
,132, 823 –865.
Frazier , L. D., Hooker, K., Johnson,
P. M., & Kaus, C. R. (2000).Continuity and change in possibleselves in later life: A 5-year longi-
tudinal study.
Basic and Applied
Social
Psychology
,22, 237 –243.
Fredrickson , B. L., & Roberts, T. A.
(1997). Objectification theory:Toward understanding women ’s
lived experience and mental health
risks.
Psychology of Women
Quarterly
,21, 173 –206.
Frensch , K. M., Pratt, M. W., & Norris,
J. E. (2007). Foundations of gener-ativity: Personal and family corre-lates of emerging adults ’generative
life-story themes.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,41,4 5 –62.
Fresco , D. M., Alloy, L. B., & Reilly-
Harrington, N. (2006). Association
of attributional style for negative
and positive events and the occur-
rence of life events with depressionand anxiety.
Journal of Social
and
Clinical Psychology
,25,
1140 –1159.
Freud ,
A. (1965).
Normality and
pathology
in childhood
. New York:
International
Universities Press.
Freud , S. (1886 –1936/1964).
The com-
plete
psychological works of
Sigmund Freud
(Vols. 1 –24).
London:
Hogarth.
Friedman , H. S., & Miller-Herringer, T.
(1991). Nonverbal display of emo-tion in public and private: Self-
monitoring, personality, andexpressive cues.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
61,
766–775.
Friedman , H. S., Prince, L. M., Riggio,
R. E., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1980).
Understanding and assessing non-
verbal expressiveness: The Affective
Communication Test.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
39,
333–351.
Friedman , L. C., Nelson, D. V., Baer,
P. E., Lane, M., Smith, F. E., &Dworkin, R. J. (1992). The rela-tionship of dispositional optimism,
daily life stress, and domestic envi-
ronment to coping methods used bycancer patients.
Journal of
Behavioral
Medicine
,15, 127 –141.
Friedman , M., & Rosenman, R. (1974).
Type A behavior and your heart
.
New
York: Knopf.
Friedrich-Cofer , L., & Huston, A. C.
(1986). Television violence and
aggression: The debate continues.
Psychological Bulletin
,100,
364–371.
Frisina ,
P. G., Borod, J. C., & Lepore,
S. J. (2004). A meta-analysis of the
effects of written emotional disclo-sure on the health outcomes in
clinical populations.
Journal of
Nervous
and Mental Disease
,192,
629–634.
Fritz,
H. L., & Helgeson, V. S. (1998).
Distinctions of unmitigated com-
munion from communion: Self-
neglect and overinvolvement withothers.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,75, 121 –140.
Froming , W. J., Nasby, W., &
McManus, J. (1998). Pro-social
self-schemas, self-awareness, and
children ’s prosocial behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,75, 766 –777.
Fromm , E. (1950).
Psychoanalysis and
religion
. New Haven, CT: Yale
University
Press.
Fromm , E. (1966).
You shall be as gods
.
Greenwich,
CT: Fawcett.
Fromm , E., & Nash, M. R. (1997).
Psychoanalysis and hypnosis
.
Madison,
CT: International
Universities Press.
Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E.
(1991). Gender differences in nega-tive affect and well-being: The casefor emotional intensity.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
61,
427–434.
Funder , D. C. (2009). Persons, beha-
viors and situations: An agenda for
personality psychology in thepostwar era.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,43, 120 –126.
Funder , D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (1983).
Behavior as a function of the situa-
tion.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,44, 107 –112.
Gabbard , G. O., Gunderson, J. G., &
Fonagy, P. (2002). The place ofpsychoanalytic treatments withinpsychiatry.
Archives of General
Psychiatry
,59, 505 –510.
Gabriel , S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999).
Are there “his”and“her”types of
interdependence? The implications
of gender differences in collective
versus relational interdependencefor affect, behavior, and cognition.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,77, 642 –655.
Gacono , C. B., & Evans, F. B. (Eds.)
(2008).
The handbook of forensic
Rorschach
assessment
. New York:
Routledge.
Gaeddert ,
W. P. (1985). Sex and sex role
effects on achievement strivings:
Dimensions of similarity and dif-
ference.
Journal of Personality
,53,
286–305.
Gale,
C. R., Batty, G. D., & Deary, I. J.
(2008). Locus of control at age10 years and health outcomes and
behaviors at age 30 years: The 1970British Cohort Study.
Psychoso-
matic
Medicine
,70, 397 –403.
Gangestad , S. W., & Thornhill, R.
(1997). Human sexual selectionand developmental stability. In J.A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.),
Evolutionary social psychology
(pp.
169–195).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ganiban , J. M., Saudino, K. J., Ulbricht,
J., Neiderhiser, J. M., & Reiss, D.
(2008). Stability and change in
temperament during adolescence.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,95, 222 –236.
Garb , H. N., Wood, J. M., Lilienfeld,
S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2005).Roots of the Rorschach contro-versy.
Clinical Psychology Review
,
25,9
7–118.
Garcia , J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966).
Relation of cue to consequence inavoidance learning.
Psychometric
Science
,4, 123 –124.
Garcia , S., Stinson, L., Ickes, W.,
Bissonnette, V., & Briggs, S. R.
(1991). Shyness and physical
attractiveness in mixed-sex dyads.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,61,3 5 –49.
Gasper , K., Lozinski, R. H., & LeBeau,
L. S. (2009). If you plan, then youREFERENCES 471 |
can: How reflection helps defensive
pessimists pursue their goals.
Motivation and Emotion
,33,
203–216.
Gastorf ,
J. W. (1980). Time urgency of
the Type A behavior pattern.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,48, 299.
Gaudiano , B. A. (2005). Cognitive
behavior therapies for psychotic
disorders: Current empirical status
and future directions.
Clinical
Psychology:
Science and Practice
,
12,3
3–50.
Geary , D. C. (2000). Evolution and
proximate expression of human
paternal investment.
Psychological
Bulletin
,126,5 5 –77.
Geen , R. G. (1983). The psychophysi-
ology of extraversion-introversion.In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty(Eds.),
Social psychophysiology:
A
sourcebook
(pp. 391 –416).
New
York: Guilford.
Geen , R. G. (1984). Preferred stimula-
tion levels in introverts and extra-
verts: Effects on arousal and
performance.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,46,
1303 –1312.
Geen ,
R. G. (1998). Aggression and an-
tisocial behavior. in D. T. Gilbert,S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.),
The handbook of social psychology
(Vol. 2, 4th ed., pp. 317 –356).
Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Geen , R. G., & Quanty, M. B. (1977).
The catharsis of aggression: An
evaluation of a hypothesis. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in
experimental
social psychology
(Vol. 10, pp. 1 –37). New York:
Academic Press.
Geen , R. G., Stonner, D., & Shope, G. L.
(1975). The facilitation of aggres-sion by aggression: Evidence
against the catharsis hypothesis.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,31, 721 –726.
Geen , R. G., & Thomas, S. L. (1986).
The immediate effects of mediaviolence on behavior.
Journal of
Social
Issues
,42,7–27.
Geers ,
A. L., Wellman, J. A., & Lassiter,
G. D. (2009). Dispositional opti-mism and engagement: The moder-ating influence of goal prioritization.
Journal of Personality and Social
Ps
ychology
,96,9 1 3 –932.
Gemar , M. C., Segal, Z. V., Sagrati, S.,
& Kennedy, S. J. (2001). Mood-
induced changes on the implicit as-
sociation test in recovered depressedpatients.
Journal of Abnormal
Ps
ychology
,11 0 ,2 8 2 –289.
Gendlin , E. T. (1988). Carl Rogers
(1902– 1987).
American
Psychologist
,43, 127 –128.
Gentile , D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R.,
& Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects
of violent video game habits onadolescent hostility, aggressivebehaviors, and school performance.
Journal of Adolescence
,27,5–22.
Geraerts ,
E., Lindsay, D. S.,
Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M.,
Raymaekers, L., Arnold, M. M.,
et al. (2009). Cognitive mechanisms
underlying recovered memoryexperiences of childhood sexualabuse.
Psychological Science
,20,
92–98.
Gershoff ,
E. T. (2002). Corporal pun-
ishment by parents and associatedchild behaviors and experiences: A
meta-analytic and theoretical re-
view.
Psychological Bulletin
,128,
539–579.
Gersten,
M. (1989). Behavioral inhibi-
tion in the classroom. In
J. S. Reznick (Ed.),
Perspectives on
behavioral
inhibition
(pp. 71 –91).
Chicago:
University of Chicago
Press.
Gest, S. D. (1997). Behavioral inhibition:
Stability and associations with adap-
tation from childhood to early
adulthood.
Journal of Personality
an
d Social Psychology
,72,4 6 7 –475.
Geuens , M., & De Pelsmacker, P.
(1999). Affect intensity revisited:Individual differences and the com-
munication effects of emotional sti-
muli.
Psychology and Marketing
,
16,
195–209.
Gibbons , F. X., Blanton, H., Gerrard,
M., Buunk, B., & Eggleston, T.(2000). Does social comparisonmake a difference? Optimism as a
moderator of the relation between
comparison level and academicperformance.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,26,
637–648.
Gibson ,
H. B. (1981).
Hans Eysenck:
The
man and his work
. London:
Peter
Owen.
Gidron , Y., Davidson, K., & Bata, I.
(1999). The short-term effects of ahostility-reduction intervention onmale coronary heart disease patients.
Health Psychology
,18,4 1 6 –420.
Giesler , R. B., Josephs, R. A., & Swann,
W. B. (1996). Self-verification in
clinical depression: The desire for
negative evaluation.
Journal ofAbnormal
Psychology
,105,
358–368.
Gilbert ,
L., Deutsch, C. L., & Strahan,
R. F. (1978). Feminine and mascu-
line dimensions of the typical,desirable and ideal woman and
man.
Sex Roles
,4, 767 –778.
Gill, M. M., & Brenman, M. (1967).
The metapsychology of regression
and hypnosis. In J. E. Gordon (Ed.),
The handbook of clinical and ex-
perimental
hypnosis
(pp. 281 –318).
New
York: Macmillan.
Gillespie , N. A., Zhu, G., Evans, D. M.,
Medland, S. E., Wright, M. J., &
Marti, N. G. (2008). A genome-
wide scan for Eysenckian personal-ity dimensions in adolescent twin
sibships: Psychoticism, extraver-
sion, neuroticism and lie.
Journal of
Personality
,76, 1415 –1445.
Giltay , E. J., Geleijnse, K. M., Zitman,
F. G., Buijsse, B., & Kromhout, D.
(2007). Lifestyle and dietary corre-lates of dispositional optimism inmen: The Zutphen Elderly Study.
Journal of PsychosomaticMedicine
,63, 483 –490.
Glasberg , R., & Aboud, F. (1982).
Keeping one ’s distance from
sadness: Children ’s self-reports of
emotional experience.
Develop-
mental
Psychology
,18, 287 –293.
Glass, C. R., & Shea, C. A. (1986).
Cognitive therapy for shyness andsocial anxiety. In W. H. Jones,J. M. Cheek, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.),
Shyness: Perspectives on research
and
treatment
(pp. 315 –327).
New
York: Plenum.
Glass, D. C. (1977).
Behavior patterns,
stress,
and coronary disease
.
Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Glisky , M. L., Tataryn, D. J., Tobias,
B. A., Kihlstrom, J. F., &
McConkey, K. M. (1991).
Absorption, openness to experi-
ence, and hypnotizability.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,60, 263 –272.
Gohm , C. L., & Clore, G. L. (2000).
Individual differences in emotionalexperience: Mapping availablescales to processes.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,26,
679–697.
Gol,
A. R., & Cook, S. W. (2004).
Exploring the underlying dimen-
sions of coping: A concept mapping
approach.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,23, 155 –171.
Goldberg, J. F., Gerstein, R. K., Wenze,
S. J., Beck, A. T., & Welker, T. A.472 REFERENCES |
(2008). Dysfunctional attitudes and
cognitive schemas in bipolar manic
and unipolar depressed outpatients:
Implications for cognitively basedpsychotherapeutics.
Journal of
Nervous
and Mental Disease
,196,
207–210.
Goldberg ,
L. R. (1992). The develop-
ment of markers for the Big-Fivefactor structure.
Psychological
Assessment
,4,2 6 –42.
Goldberg , L. R. (1993). The structure of
phenotypic personality traits.
American Psychologist
,48,2 6 –34.
Goldberg , L. R. (2001). Analyses of
Digman ’s child-personality data:
Derivation of Big-Five factor scoresfrom each of six samples.
Journal of
Personality
,69, 709 –743.
Goode, E. (1999, April 23).
Homosexuality-gene study re-
leased: Research fails to support
report of chromosomal link.
San
Jose
Mercury News
, p. 8A.
Goodwin ,
R., Cook, O., & Yung, Y.
(2001). Loneliness and life satisfac-tion among three cultural groups.
Personal Relationships
,8,2 2 5 –230.
Gorassini , D. R., Sowerby, D.,
Creighton, A., & Fry, G. (1991).Hypnotic susceptibility enhance-
ment through brief cognitive skill
training.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,61, 289 –297.
Gorassini , D. R., & Spanos, N. P.
(1986). A social-cognitive skillsapproach to the successful modifi-cation of hypnotic susceptibility.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,50, 1004 –1012.
Goswick , R. A., & Jones, W. H. (1981).
Loneliness, self-concept, and
adjustment.
Journal of Psychology
,
88,
258–261.
Gotay , C. C. (1981). Cooperation and
competition as a function of Type Abehavior.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,7, 386 –392.
Gough , H. G., Fioravanti, M., &
Lazzari, R. (1983). Some implica-tions of self versus ideal-self con-
gruence on the Revised Adjective
Check List.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,44,
1214 –1220.
Gough ,
H. G., Lazzari, R., &
Fioravanti, M. (1978). Self versusideal self: A comparison of five
adjective check list indices.
Journal
of
Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,46, 1085 –1091.
Graham , S. M., Huang, J. Y., Clark,
M. S., & Helgeson, V. S. (2008).The positives of negative emotions:
Willingness to express negative
emotions promotes relationships.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,34, 394 –406.
Granberg , E. (2006). “Is that all there
is?”Possible selves, self-change,
and weight loss.
Social Psychology
Quarterly
,69, 109 –126.
Gray , J. A. (1982).
The neuropsychol-
ogy
of anxiety: An inquiry of the
septo-hippocampal system
. Oxford,
England:
Oxford University Press.
Gray , J. A. (1987). Perspectives on
anxiety and impulsivity: A com-
mentary.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,21, 493 –509.
Gray , J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000).
The neuropsychology of anxiety
.
Oxford,
England: Oxford
University Press.
Graziano , W. G., Jensen-Campbell,
L. A., Steele, R. G., & Hair, E. C.
(1998). Unknown words in self-reported personality: Lethargic andprovincial in Texas.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,24,
893–905.
Graziano ,
W. G., Jensen-Campbell,
L. A., & Sullivan-Logan, G. M.
(1998). Temperament, activity, and
expectations for later personalitydevelopment.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
1266 –1277.
Graziano , W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L.
A., Todd, M., & Finch, J. F. (1997).Interpersonal attraction from an
evolutionary psychology perspec-
tive: Women ’s reactions to domi-
nant and prosocial men. InJ. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick
(Eds.),
Evolutionary social psy-
chology
(pp. 141 –167). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Graziano , W. G., Habashi, M. M.,
Sheese, B. E., & Tobin, R. M.
(2007). Agreeableness, empathy,
and helping: A person X situationperspective.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,93,
583–599.
Green ,
B. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (1994).
The attractiveness of gender-typed
traits at different relationship levels:
Androgynous characteristics maybe desirable after all.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,20,
244–253.
Green ,
J. P. (2004). The five factor model
of personality and hypnotizability:Little variance in common.
Con-
te
mporary Hypnosis
,21,1 6 1 –168.Green , J. P., Page, R. A., Handley,
G. W., & Rasekhy, R. (2005). The
“hidden observer ”and ideomotor
responding: A real-simulator com-
parison.
Contemporary Hypnosis
,
22,
123–137.
Green , L. R., Richardson, D. S., Lago,
T., & Schatten- Jones, E. C. (2001).
Network correlates of social andemotional loneliness in young and
older adults.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,27, 281 –288.
Greenberg , M. A., Wortman, C. B., &
Stone, A. A. (1996). Emotionalexpression and physical health:
Revising traumatic memories or
fostering self-regulation?
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
71,
588–602.
Greenberg , R., Pillard, R., & Pearlman,
C. (1978). The effect of dream(stage REM) deprivation on adap-
tation to stress. In S. Fisher &
R. P. Greenberg (Eds.),
The scien-
tific
evaluation of Freud ’s theories
and therapy
(pp. 40 –48). New
York: Basic Books.
Greening , L., Stoppelbein, L., & Docter,
R. (2002). The mediating effects ofattributional style and event-
specific attributions on postdisaster
adjustment.
Cognitive Therapy and
Research
,26, 261 –274.
Greve , W., & Wentura, D. (2003).
Immunizing the self: Self-conceptstabilization through reality-adaptive self-definitions.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,29,3 9 –50.
Gronnerod , C. (2004). Rorschach
assessment of changes following
psychotherapy: A meta-analytic
review.
Journal of Personality
Assessment
,83, 256 –276.
Gross , J. J., & John, O. P. (1998).
Mapping the domain of expressiv-ity: Multimethod evidence for a
hierarchical model.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
170–191.
Grove , J. R., Hanrahan, S. J., &
McInman, A. (1991). Success/
failure bias in attributions acrossinvolvement categories in sport.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,17,9 3 –97.
Gruzelier , J. H. (2006). Frontal func-
tions, connectivity and neural effi-
ciency underpinning hypnosis and
hypnotic susceptibility.
Contempo-
rary
Hypnosis
,23,1 5 –23.
Guerra , N. G., Huesmann, L. R., Tolan,
P. H., Van Acker, R., & Eron, L. D.REFERENCES 473 |
(1995). Stressful events and indi-
vidual beliefs as correlates of eco-
nomic disadvantage and aggression
among urban children.
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,63, 518 –528.
Guimond , S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D.,
Crisp, R. J., & Redersdorff, S.(2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences
in self-construal.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
90,
221–242.
Gunthert , K. C., Cohen, L. H., &
Armeli, S. (1999). The role of
Neuroticism in daily stress and
coping.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,77, 1087 –1100.
Gur, R. C., & Reivich, M. (1980).
Cognitive task effects on hemi-spheric blood flow in humans:Evidence for individual differences
in hemispheric activation.
Brain
and
Language
,9,7 8 –92.
Guyll , M., & Contrada, R. J. (1998).
Trait hostility and ambulatorycardiovascular activity: Responses
to social interaction.
Health
Psychology
,17,3 0 –39.
Gwaltney , C. J., Shiffman, S., Balabanis,
M. H., & Paty, J. A. (2005).
Dynamic self-efficacy and outcomeexpectancies: Prediction of smokinglapse and relapse.
Journal of Ab-
normal
Psychology
,114, 661 –675.
Haans , A., Kaiser, F. G., & de Kort,
Y. A. W. (2007). Privacy needs in
office environments: Development
of two behavior-based scales.
European Psychologist
,12,9 3 –102.
Haeffel , G. J., Abramson, L. Y., Voelz,
Z. R., Metalsky, G. I., Halberstadt,L., Dykman, B. M., et al. (2003).
Cognitive vulnerability to depres-
sion and lifetime history of Axis Ipsychopathology: A comparison of
negative cognitive styles (CSQ) and
dysfunctional attitudes (DAS).
Journal of CognitivePsychotherapy
,17,3–22.
Haemmerlie ,
F. M., & Montgomery,
R. L. (1986). Self-perception theory
and the treatment of shyness. InW. H. Jones, J. M. Cheek, &
S. R. Briggs (Eds.),
Shyness:
Perspectives
on research and treat-
ment
(pp. 329 –342). New York:
Plenum.
Hall, C., Smith, K., & Chia, R. (2008).
Cognitive and personality factors in
relation to timely completion of acollege degree.
College Student
Journal
,42, 1087 –1098.Hall, C. S. (1953). A cognitive theory of
dream symbols.
Journal of General
Psychology
,48, 169 –186.
Hall, C. S. (1984). “A ubiquitous sex
difference in dreams ”revisited.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,46, 1109 –1117.
Hall, C. S., & Domhoff, B. (1963). A
ubiquitous sex difference in dreams.
Journal of Abnormal and SocialPsychology
,66, 278 –280.
Hall, M. H. (1968, August). A conver-
sation with the president of the
American Psychological
Association: The psychology of
universality.
Psychology Today
,
pp.
35–37, 54 –57.
Handler , L. (1996). The clinical use of
drawings: Draw-a-person, house-
tree-person, and kinetic family
drawings. In C. S. Newmark (Ed.),
Major psychological assessment in-
struments
(2nd ed., pp. 206 –293).
Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn &
Bacon.
Hankin , B. L. (2008). Stability of cog-
nitive vulnerabilities to depression:
A short-term prospective multiwavestudy.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,117, 324 –233.
Hankin , B. L., Fraley, R. C., & Abela,
J. R. Z. (2005). Daily depressionand cognitions about stress:Evidence for a traitlike depresso-
genic cognitive style and the pre-
diction of depressive symptoms in aprospective daily diary study.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,88, 673 –685.
Hanley-Dunn , P., Maxwell, S. E., &
Santos, J. F. (1985). Interpretation
of interpersonal interaction: The
influence of loneliness.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,11,
445–456.
Harackiewicz ,
J. M., Barron, K. E.,
Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., &
Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of
achievement goal theory: Necessaryand illuminating.
Journal of Edu-
cational
Psychology
,94, 638 –645.
Hardin , E. E., & Lakin, J. L. (2009). The
Integrated Self-Discrepancy Index:
A reliable and valid measure of
self-discrepancies.
Journal of Per-
so
nality Assessment
,91,2 4 5 –253.
Harmon-Jones , E., & Allen, J. J. B.
(1997). Behavioral activation sensi-tivity and resting frontal EEG
asymmetry: Covariation of putative
indicators related to risk for mooddisorders.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,106, 159 –163.Harmon-Jones , E., Lueck, L., Fearn, M.,
& Harmon-Jones, C. (2006). The
effect of personal relevance and
approach-related action expecta-tion on relative left frontal corticalactivity.
Psychological Science
,17,
434–440.
Harmon-Jones ,
E., & Sigelman, J.
(2001). State anger and prefrontal
brain activity: Evidence that insult-
related relative left-prefrontal
activation is associated with expe-rienced anger and aggression.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,80, 797 –803.
Harris , C. R. (2003). A review of sex
differences in sexual jealousy,
including self-report data, psycho-
physiological responses, interper-
sonal violence, and morbidjealousy.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Review
,7, 102 –128.
Harris , M. B. (1974). Mediators be-
tween frustration and aggression ina field experiment.
Journal of
Experimental
Social Psychology
,
10,
561–571.
Harrison , A. A., & Saeed, L. (1977).
Let’s make a deal: An analysis of
revelations and stipulations in
lonely hearts advertisements.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,35, 257 –264.
Harrist , S., Carlozzi, B. L., McGovern,
A. R., & Harrist, A. W. (2007).Benefits of expressive writing andexpressive talking about life goals.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
41,
923–930.
Hartshorne , H., & May, M. A. (1928).
Studies in the nature of character:Studies
in deceit
. New York:
Macmillan.
Harvey ,
J. H., & Omarzu, J. (1997).
Minding the close relationship.
Personality and Social Psychology
Review
,1, 224 –240.
Havermans , R., Nicolson, N. A., &
deVries, M. W. (2007). Daily
hassles, uplifts, and time use in
individuals with bipolar disorder in
remission.
Journal of Nervous and
Mental
Disease
,195, 745 –751.
Hawkley , L. C., Burleson, M. H.,
Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T.(2003). Loneliness in everyday life:Cardiovascular activity, psychoso-cial context, and health behaviors.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,85, 105 –120.
Hawkley , L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T.
(2007). Aging and loneliness:
Downhill quickly?
Current474 REFERENCES |
Directions in Psychological Science ,
16,
187–191.
Hawkley , L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry,
J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006).
Loneliness is a unique predictor ofage-related differences in systolic
blood pressure.
Psychology and
Aging
,21, 152 –164.
Hawkley , L. C., Thisted, R. A., &
Cacioppo, J. T. (2009). Lonelinesspredicts reduced physical activity:
Cross-sectional & longitudinal
analyses.
Health Psychology
,28,
354–363.
Haynes ,
S. N. (2001). Introduction to
the special section on clinical ap-plications of analogue behavioralobservation.
Psychological
Assessment
,13,3–4.
Hazan ,
C., & Shaver, P. (1987).
Romantic love conceptualized as an
attachment process.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
52,
511–524.
Headey , B., & Wearing, A. (1989).
Personality, life events, and subjec-
tive well-being: Toward a dynamic
equilibrium model.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
57,
731–739.
Heath , A. C., Neale, M. C., Kessler,
R. C., Eaves, L. J., & Kendler, K. S.(1992). Evidence for genetic influ-ences on personality from self-
reports and informant ratings.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,63,8 5 –96.
Heatherton , T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991).
Development and validation of ascale for measuring state self-esteem.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,60, 895 –910.
Hedlund , S., & Rude, S. S. (1995).
Evidence of latent depressive sche-mas in formerly depressed indivi-duals.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,104, 517 –525.
Heery , E. A., & Kring, A. M. (2007).
Interpersonal consequences of so-
cial anxiety.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,116, 125 –134.
Heimpel , S. A., Wood, J. V., Marshall,
M. A., & Brown, J. D. (2002). Dopeople with low self-esteem really
want to feel better? Self-esteem dif-
ferences in motivation to repairnegative moods.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
82,
128–147.
Heine , S. J. (2001). Self as cultural
product: An examination of EastAsian and North American selves.
Journal of Personality
,69,8 8 1 –906.Heine , S. J., & Hamamura, T. (2007).
In search of East Asian self-
enhancement.
Personality and
So
cial Psychology Review
,11,4–27.
Heine ,
S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1995).
Cultural variation in unrealistic
optimism: Does the West feel more
invulnerable than the East?
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,68, 595 –607.
Heine , S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1999).
Culture, self-discrepancies, and self-
satisfaction.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,25, 915 –925.
Heine , S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus,
H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Isthere a universal need for positiveself-regard?
Psychological Review
,
106,
766–794.
Heinrich , L. M., & Gullone, E. (2006).
The clinical significance of loneli-ness: A literature review.
Clinical
Psychology
Review
,26, 695 –718.
Heinrichs , N., Rapee, R. M., Alden,
L. A., Bogels, S., Hofmann, S. G.,
Oh, K. J., & Sakano, Y. (2005).
Cultural differences in perceived
social norms and social anxiety.
Behaviour Research and Therapy
,
44,
1187 –1197.
Helgeson , V. S. (1994). Relation of
agency and communion to well-being: Evidence and potential ex-planations.
Psychological Bulletin
,
116,
412–428.
Helgeson , V. S. (2003). Unmitigated
communion and adjustment tobreast cancer: Associations and ex-
planations.
Journal of Applied
Social
Psychology
,33, 1643 –1661.
Helgeson , V. S., Cohen, S., & Fritz,
H. L. (1998). Social ties and cancer.In J. C. Holland & W. Breitbart
(Eds.),
Psycho-oncology
(pp. 99 –
109).
New York: Oxford University
Press.
Helgeson , V. S., Escobar, O., Siminerio,
L., & Becker, D. (2007).
Unmitigated communion andhealth among adolescents with and
without diabetes: The mediating
role of eating disturbances.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,33, 519 –536.
Helgeson , V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (1999).
Unmitigated agency and unmiti-
gated communion: Distinctionsfrom agency and communion.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
33,
131–158.
Helgeson , V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (2000).
The implications of unmitigated
agency and unmitigated communionfor domains of problem behavior.
Journal of Personality
,68,
1031 –1057.
Helgeson ,
V. S., & Lepore, S. J. (1997).
Men ’s adjustment to prostate can-
cer: The role of agency andunmitigated agency.
Sex Roles
,37,
251–267.
Helgeson ,
V. S., & Lepore, S. J. (2004).
Quality of life following prostatecancer: The role of agency and
unmitigated agency.
Journal of
Applied
Social Psychology
,34,
2559 –2585.
Helmes ,
E., & Reddon, J. R. (1993). A
perspective on developments in as-sessing psychopathology: A criticalreview of the MMPI and MMPI-2.
Psychological Bulletin
,113,
453–471.
Hemenover ,
S. H. (2003). The good, the
bad, and the healthy: Impacts of
emotional disclosure of trauma on
resilient self-concept and psycho-logical distress.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,29,
1236 –1244.
Henriques ,
J. B., & Davidson, R. J.
(1990). Regional brain electricalasymmetries discriminate between
previously depressed and healthy
control subjects.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,99,2 2 –31.
Herrnstein , R. J., & Murray, C. (1994).
The bell curve: Intelligence andclass
structure in American life
.
New
York: Free Press.
Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J.,
Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D.
(2004). On the selective relation of
frontal cortical asymmetry andanger-out versus anger-control.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,87, 926 –939.
Hibbard , S., Farmer, L., Wells, C.,
Difillipo, E., Barry, W., Korman,
R., & Sloan, P. (1994). Validation
of Cramer ’s Defense Mechanism
Manual for the TAT.
Journal of
Personality
Assessment
,63,
197–210.
Higgins ,
E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy:
A theory relating self and affect.
Psychological Review
,94, 319 –340.
Higgins , E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy
theory: What patterns of self-beliefs
cause people to suffer? In L. Berko-witz (Ed.),
Advances in experimen-
tal
social psychology
(Vol. 22,
pp.
93–136). San Diego: Academic
Press.
Hilgard , E. R. (1973). A neodissociation
interpretation of pain reduction inREFERENCES 475 |
hypnosis. Psychological Review ,
80,
396–411.
Hilgard , E. R. (1977).
Divided con-
sciousness:
Multiple controls in
human thought and action
. New
York:
Wiley.
Hilgard , E. R. (1992). Dissociation and
theories of hypnosis. In E. Fromm
& M. R. Nash (Eds.),
Contemporary hypnosis research
(pp. 69 –101). New York: Guilford.
Hilgard , E. R. (1994). Neodissociation
theory. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue
(Eds.),
Dissociation: Clinical, theo-
retical
and research perspectives
(pp. 32 –51). New York: Guilford.
Hill, C. E. (1996).
Working with dreams
in
psychotherapy
. New York:
Guilford.
Hill,
C. E., & Knox, S. (2001). Self-
disclosure.
Psychotherapy
,38,
413–417.
Hill,
G. J. (1989). An unwillingness to
act: Behavioral appropriateness,
situational constraint, and self-efficacy in shyness.
Journal of
Personality
,57, 871 –890.
Hiroto , D. S. (1974). Locus of control
and learned helplessness.
Journal of
Experimental
Psychology
,102,
187–193.
Hiroto ,
D. S., & Seligman, M. E. P.
(1975). Generality of learned help-lessness in man.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
31,
311–327.
Hoffman , L. W. (1985). The changing
genetics/socialization balance.
Journal of Social Issues
,41,
127–148.
Hoffman ,
L. W. (1991). The influence of
the family environment on person-
ality: Accounting for sibling differ-
ences.
Psychological Bulletin
,110,
187–203.
Hogan ,
R. (1991). Personality and
personality measurement. In
M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough
(Eds.),
Handbook of industrial and
organizational
psychology
(2nd ed.,
Vol.2,
pp. 873 –919). Palo Alto,
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Hojnoski , R. L., Morrison, R., Brown,
M., & Matthews, W. J. (2009).
Projective test use among school
psychologists.
Journal of Psychoe-
du
cational Assessment
,24,1 4 5 –159.
Hollon , S. D., Stewart, M. O., & Strunk,
D. (2006). Enduring effects forcognitive behavior therapy in thetreatment of depression and anxi-ety.
Annual Review of Psychology
,
57,
285–315.Holloway , R. A., Waldrip, A. M., &
Ickes, W. (2009). Evidence that a
simpatico self-schema accounts for
differences in the self-concepts and
social behavior of Latinos versus
Whites (and Blacks).
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
96,
1012 –1028.
Hoobler , J. M., & Brass, D. J. (2006).
Abusive supervision and family
undermining as displaced aggres-
sion.
Journal of Applied
Psychology
,91, 1125 –1133.
Hooker , K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K.,
& Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mentaland physical health of spouse care-givers: The role of personality.
Psychology and Aging
,7, 367 –375.
Hopkins, J. R. (1995). Erik Homburger
Erikson (1902– 1994).
American
Psychologist
,50, 796 –797.
Horley , J. (1996). Content stability in
the repertory grid: An examinationusing a forensic sample.
International Journal of OffenderTherapy
and Comparative
Criminology
,40,2 6 –31.
Horn , J. (2001). Raymond Bernard
Cattell (1905– 1998).
American
Psychologist
,56,7 1 –72.
Horney , K. (1945/1966).
Our inner
conflicts:
A constructive theory of
neurosis
. New York: Norton.
Horney ,
K. (1967).
Feminine psychol-
ogy
. New York: Norton.
Hornstein ,
G. A. (1985). Intimacy in
conversational style as a function of
the degree of closeness between
members of a dyad.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
49,
671–681.
Hornstein , G. A., & Truesdell, S. E.
(1988). Development of intimate
conversation in close relationships.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,7,4 9 –64.
Horowitz , L. M., & de Sales French, R.
(1979). Interpersonal problems ofpeople who describe themselves aslonely.
Journal of Consulting and
Clinical
Psychology
,47, 762 –764.
Howard , M. L., & Coe, W. C. (1980).
The effects of context and subjects ’
perceived control in breaching
posthypnotic amnesia.
Journal of
Personality
,48, 342 –359.
Hoyt , M. F., & Singer, J. L. (1978).
Psychological effects of REM(“dream ”) deprivation upon wak-
ing mentation. In A. M. Arkin,
J. S. Antrobus, & S. J. Ellman(Eds.),
The mind in sleep:
Psychology
and psychophysiology(pp. 487 –510). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Huesmann , L. R. (1988). An
information-processing model forthe development of aggression.
Aggressive Behavior
,14,1 3 –24.
Huesmann , L. R., Eron, L. D., Dubow,
E. F., & Seebauer, E. (1987).Television viewing habits in child-hood and adult aggression.
Child
Development
,58, 357 –367.
Huesmann , L. R., Eron, L. D., &
Yarmel, P. W. (1987). Intellectual
functioning and aggression.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,52, 232 –240.
Huesmann , L. R., & Guerra, N. G.
(1997). Children ’s normative beliefs
about aggression and aggressivebehavior.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,72,
408–419.
Hui,
C. H. (1988). Measurement of
individualism-collectivism.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,22,
17–36.
Hunsley ,
J., & Bailey, J. M. (1999). The
clinical utility of the Rorschach:
Unfulfilled promises and an uncer-tain future.
Psychological
Assessment
,11, 266 –277.
Hurtz , G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000).
Personality and job performance:
The Big Five revisited.
Journal of
Applied
Psychology
,85, 869 –879.
Hyde , J. S., & Kling, K. C. (2001).
Women, motivation and achieve-ment.
Psychology of Women
Quarterly
,25, 364 –378.
Ickes, W. (1993). Traditional gender
roles: Do they make, and thenbreak, our relationships?
Journal of
Social
Issues
,49,7 1 –85.
Ickes, W., & Barnes, R. D. (1978). Boys
and girls together and alienated: Onenacting stereotyped sex roles in
mixed-sex dyads.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
36,
669–683.
Ickes, W., Robertson, E., Tooke, W., &
Teng, G. (1986). Naturalistic social
cognition: Methodology, assess-
ment, and validation.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
51,6
6–82.
Ickes, W., Schermer, B., & Steeno, J.
(1979). Sex and sex-role influencein same-sex dyads.
Social
Psychology
Quarterly
,42,
373–385.
Ihilevich ,
D., & Gleser, G. C. (1993).
Defense mechanisms: Their classi-
fication,
correlates, and476 REFERENCES |
measurement with the Defense
Mechanism Inventory
. Odessa, FL:
Psychological
Assessment
Resources.
Ilardi , S. S., & Craighead, W. E. (1999).
The relationship between personal-
ity pathology and dysfunctional
cognitions in previously depressedadults.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,108,5 1 –57.
Ilgen, M., McKellar, J., & Tiet, Q.
(2005). Abstinence self-efficacy andabstinence 1 year after substanceuse disorder treatment.
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,73, 1175 –1180.
Ingram , R. E., & Ritter, J. (2000).
Vulnerability to depression:
Cognitive reactivity and parental
bonding in high-risk individuals.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
109,
588–596.
Jackson , B., Kubzansky, L. D., Cohen,
S., Jacobs, D. R., & Wright, R. J.
(2007). Does harboring hostilityhurt? Associations between hostility
and pulmonary function in the
Coronary Artery Risk Developmentin (young) Adults (CARDIA) study.
Health Psychology
,26, 333 –340.
Jackson , C. J. (2009). Jackson-5 scales
of revised Reinforcement SensitivityTheory (r-RST) and their applica-tion to dysfunctional real world
outcomes.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,43, 556 –569.
Jackson , L. A. (1983). The perception of
androgyny and physical attractive-ness: Two is better than one.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,9, 405 –413.
Jackson , L. A., Ialongo, N., & Stollak,
G. E. (1986). Parental correlates ofgender role: The relations betweenparents ’masculinity, femininity,
and child-rearing behaviors and
their children ’s gender roles.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,4, 204 –224.
Jahoda , M. (1977).
Freud and the di-
lemmas
of psychology
. New York:
Basic
Books.
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon,
P. A. (1996). Heritability of the Big
Five personality dimensions and
their facets: A twin study.
Journal
of
Personality
,64, 577 –591.
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner,
A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J.(1998). Heritability of facet-leveltraits in a cross-cultural twin sam-ple: Support for a hierarchical
model of personality.
Journal ofPersonality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
1556 –1565.
Janoff-Bulman , R. (1992).
Shattered
assumptions:
Towards a new psy-
chology of trauma
. New York: Free
Press.
Jefferson ,
T., Herbst, J. H., & McCrae,
R. R. (1998). Associations between
birth order and personality traits:Evidence from self-reports and ob-
server ratings.
Journal of Research
in
Personality
,32, 498 –509.
Jeffrey , A., & Austin, T. (2007).
Perspectives and practices of clini-cian self-disclosure to clients: A
pilot comparison study of two
disciplines.
American Journal of
Family
Therapy
,35,9 5 –108.
Jenkins , C. D. (1971). Psychologic and
social precursors of coronary dis-ease.
New England Journal of
Medicine
,284, 244 –255, 307 –317.
Jenkins , C. D. (1976). Recent evidence
supporting psychologic and socialrisk factors for coronary disease.
New England Journal of Medicine
,
294,
987–994, 1033 –1038.
Jenkins , C. D., Zyzanski, S. J., &
Rosenman, R. H. (1976). Risk ofnew myocardial infarction in
middle-age men with manifest cor-
onary heart disease.
Circulation
,
53,
342–347.
Jenkins , S. R. (1987). Need for achieve-
ment and women ’s careers over 14
years: Evidence for occupationalstructure effects.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
53,
922–932.
Jensen , A. R. (1969). How much can we
boost IQ and scholastic achieve-ment?
Harvard Educational
Review
,39,1–123.
Jensen-Campbell ,
L. A., & Graziano,
W. G. (2001). Agreeableness as a
moderator of interpersonal conflict.
Journal of Personality
,69, 323 –362.
Jensen-Campbell , L. A., Graziano,
W. G., & West, S. G. (1995).Dominance, prosocial orientation,
and female preferences: Do nice
guys really finish last?
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
68,
427–440.
Joca, S. R. L., Zanelati, T., &
Guimaraes, F. S. (2006). Post-stressfacilitation of serotonergic, but notnoradrenergic, neurotransmission
in the dorsal hippocampus prevents
learned helplessness development inrats.
Brain Research
,1087 ,6 7 –74.
Johansson, B., Grant, J. D., Plomin, R.,
Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Berg, S.,et al. (2001). Health locus of con-
trol in late life: A study of genetic
and environmental influences in
twins aged 80 years and older.
Health Psychology
,20,3 3 –40.
John, O. P., Nauman, L. P., & Soto,
C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the
integrative Big Five trait taxonomy:History, measurement, and con-ceptual issues. In O. P. John,
R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin
(Eds.),
Handbook of personality
(3rd ed., pp. 114 –158). New York:
Guilford.
Johnson , J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes,
E. M., Kasen, S., & Brook, J. S.(2002). Television viewing and ag-gressive behavior during adoles-
cence and adulthood.
Science
,295,
2468 –2471.
Joiner ,
T. E., Catanzaro, S. J., Rudd,
M. D., & Rajab, M. H. (1999). Thecase for a hierarchical, oblique, and
bidimensional structure of loneli-ness.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,18,4 7 –75.
Jones , A., & Crandall, R. (1986).
Validation of a short index of self-actualization.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,12,
63–73.
Jones ,
E. (1953– 1957).
The life and
work
of Sigmund Freud
(Vols. 1– 3).
New
York: Basic Books.
Jones , R. E., Leen-Felder, E. W.,
Olatunji, B. O., Reardon, L. E., &Hawks, E. (2009). Psychometricproperties of the Affect Intensity
and Reactivity Measure adapted for
youth (AIR-Y).
Psychological
Assessment
,21, 162 –175.
Jones , W. H., Freemon, J. E., &
Goswick, R. A. (1981). The persis-tence of loneliness: Self and otherdeterminants.
Journal of
Personality
,49,2 7 –48.
Jones , W. H., Hobbs, S. A., &
Hockenbury, D. (1982). Lonelinessand social skill deficits.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
42,
682–689.
Jones , W. H., Sansone, C., & Helm, B.
(1983). Loneliness and interpersonaljudgments.
Personality and Social
Ps
ychology Bulletin
,9,4 3 7 –441.
Jorgensen , R. S., Johnson, B. T.,
Kolodziej, M. E., & Schreer, G. E.
(1996). Elevated blood pressure
and personality: A meta-analytic
review.
Psychological Bulletin
,120,
293–320.
Josephs ,
R. A., Markus, H. R., &
Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). GenderREFERENCES 477 |
and self-esteem. Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
63,
391–402.
Jourard , S. M. (1971).
The transparent
self
(2nd ed.). New York: Van
Nostrand.
Judge ,
T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001).
Relationship of core self-
evaluations traits —self-esteem,
generalized self-efficacy, locus of
control, and emotional stability —
with job satisfaction and job per-
formance: A meta-analysis.
Journal
of
Applied Psychology
,86,8 0 –92.
Judge , T. A., Erez, A., & Bono, J. E.
(1998). The power of being positive:
The relationship between positiveself-concept and job performance.
Human Performance
,11,1 6 7 –187.
Judge , T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen,
C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The
Big Five personality traits, general
mental ability, and career success
across the life span.
Personnel
Psychology
,52, 621 –652.
Jung, C. G. (1902 –1961/1961).
The
collected
works of Carl Jung
(Vols.
1–17).
Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Jung, C. G. (1961).
Memories, dreams,
reflections
. New York: Pantheon.
Jung,
C. G. (1964). Approaching the
unconscious. In C. G. Jung (Ed.),
Man and his symbols
(pp. 3– 94).
New
York: Dell.
Kagan , J. (1989). Temperamental con-
tributions to social behavior.
American Psychologist
,44,
668–674.
Kagan ,
J. (2003). Biology, context, and
developmental inquiry.
Annual
Review
of Psychology
,54,1–23.
Kagan ,
J., & Moss, H. A. (1962).
Birth
to
maturity
. New York: Wiley.
Kagan ,
J., & Snidman, N. (1991a).
Infant predictors of inhibited and
uninhibited profiles.
Psychological
Science
,2,4 0 –44.
Kagan , J., & Snidman, N. (1991b).
Temperamental factors in humandevelopment.
American
Psychologist
,46, 856 –862.
Kagan , J., & Snidman, N. (2004).
The
long
shadow of temperament
.
Cambridge,
MA: Harvard
University Press.
Kahn , J. H., & Hessling, R. M. (2001).
Measuring the tendency to concealversus disclose psychological dis-
tress.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,20,4 1 –65.
Kalechstein , A. D., & Nowicki, S.
(1997). A meta-analyticexamination of the relationship
between control expectancies and
academic achievement: An 11-yr.
follow-up to Findley and Cooper.
Genetic, Social and General
Psychology
Monographs
,123,
27–56.
Kallio ,
S., & Revonsuo, A. (2003).
Hypnotic phenomena and altered
states of consciousness: A multilevel
framework of description and
explanation.
Contemporary
Hypnosis
,20, 111 –164.
Kallio , S., & Revonsuo, A. (2005).
Altering the state of the altered statedebate: Reply to commentaries.
Contemporary Hypnosis
,20,
46–55.
Kamen-Siegel ,
L., Rodin, J., Seligman,
M. E. P., & Dwyer, J. (1991).Explanatory style and cell-mediatedimmunity in elderly men and
women.
Health Psychology
,10,
229–235.
Kang ,
S-M., Shaver, P. R., Sue, S., Min,
K-H., & Jing, H. (2003). Culture-specific patterns in the prediction of
life satisfaction: Roles of emotion,relationship quality, and self-esteem.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bu
lletin
,29,1 5 9 6 –1608.
Kaplan , A., & Maehr, M. L. (2007). The
contributions and prospects of goal
orientation theory.
Educational
Psychology
Review
,19, 141 –184.
Kaplan , G. D., & Cowles, A. (1978).
Health locus of control and healthvalue in the prediction of smoking
cessation.
Health Education
Monographs
,6, 129 –137.
Kasen , S. Chen, H., Sneed, J., Crawford,
T., & Cohen, P. (2006). Social roleand birth cohort influences on
gender-linked personality traits in
women: A 20-year longitudinalanalysis.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,91, 944 –958.
Kashdan , T. B., & Roberts, J. E. (2006).
Affective outcomes in superficial
and intimate interactions: Roles of
social anxiety and curiosity.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,40,
140–167.
Katz,
I. M., & Campbell, J. D. (1994).
Ambivalence over emotional ex-
pression and well-being:
Nomothetic and idiographic tests ofthe stress-buffering hypothesis.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,67, 513 –524.
Kawachi , I., Sparrow, D., Spiro, A.,
Vokonas, P., & Weiss, S. T. (1996).
A prospective study of anger andcoronary heart disease: The
Normative Aging Study.
Circulation
,94, 2090 –2095.
Keelan , J. P. R., Dion, K. L., & Dion,
K. K. (1994). Attachment style and
heterosexual relationships among
young adults: A short-term panel
study.
Journal of Social and
Personal
Relationships
,11,
201–214.
Keller ,
J., & Bless, H. (2008). Flow and
regulatory compatibility: An exper-imental approach to the flow modelof intrinsic motivation.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,34,
196–209.
Kelly ,
A. E. (1998). Clients ’secret
keeping in outpatient therapy.
Journal of Counseling Psychology
,
45,5
0–57.
Kelly , A. E., & Archer, J. A. (1995). Self-
concealment and attitudes toward
counseling in university students.
Journal of Counseling Psychology
,
42,4
0–46.
Kelly , A. E., Klusas, J. A., von Weiss,
R. T., & Kenny, C. (2001). What is
it about revealing secrets that is
beneficial?
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,27, 651 –665.
Kelly , A. E., & McKillop, K. J. (1996).
Consequences of revealing personalsecrets.
Psychological Bulletin
,120,
450–465.
Kelly ,
G. A. (1955).
The psychology of
personal
constructs
. New York:
Norton.
Kelly ,
G. A. (1969).
Clinical psychology
an
d personality: The selected papers
of George Kelly
. New York: Wiley.
Kendzierski ,
D. (1988). Self-schemata
and exercise.
Basic and Applied
Social
Psychology
,9,4 5 –61.
Kendzierski , D. (1990). Exercise self-
schemata: Cognitive and behavioral
correlates.
Health Psychology
,9,
69–82.
Kenrick ,
D. T., Keefe, R. C., Gabrielidis,
C., & Cornelius, J. S. (1996).
Adolescents ’age preferences for
dating partners: Support for anevolutionary model of life-historystrategies.
Child Development
,67,
1499 –1511.
Kent,
G. (1997). Dental phobias. In
G. C. Davey (Ed.),
Phobias: A
handbook
of theory, research and
treatment
(pp. 107 –127).
Chichester,
England: Wiley.
Keogh , B. K. (1986). Temperament and
schooling: Meaning of “Goodness
of Fit ”? In J. V. Lerner &
R. M. Lerner (Eds.),
Temperament478 REFERENCES |
and social interaction during in-
fancy and childhood
(pp. 89 –108).
San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Keogh , B. K. (1989). Applying temper-
ament research to schools. In
G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, &
M. K. Rothbart (Eds.),
Temperament in childhood
(pp.
437–450).
New York: Wiley.
Keogh , B. K. (2003).
Temperament in
the
classroom: Understanding indi-
vidual differences
. Baltimore, MD:
Paul
H. Brookes.
Kern , M. L., & Friedman, H. S. (2008).
Do conscientious individuals live
longer? A quantitative review.
Health Psychology
,27, 505 –512.
Kernis , M. H., Brockner, J., & Frankel,
B. S. (1989). Self-esteem and reac-
tions to failure: The mediating roleof overgeneralization.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
57,
707–714.
Kihlstrom , J. F. (1985). Hypnosis.
Annual Review of Psychology
,36,
385–418.
Kihlstrom ,
J. F. (1998). Dissociations
and dissociation theory in hypnosis:Comment on Kirsch and Lynn(1998).
Psychological Bulletin
,123,
186–191.
Kihlstrom ,
J. F. (2005). Is hypnosis an
altered state of consciousness or
what ?
Contemporary Hypnosis
,22,
34–38.
King,
L. A. (1998). Ambivalence over
emotional expression and readingemotions in situations and faces.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,74, 753 –762.
King, L. A. (2001). The health benefits
of writing about life goals.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,27, 798 –807.
King, L. A., & Emmons, R. A. (1990).
Conflict over emotional expression:
Psychological and physical corre-
lates.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,58, 864 –877.
King, L. A., & Miner, K. N. (2000).
Writing about the perceived bene-
fits of traumatic events:Implications for physical health.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,26, 220 –230.
Kirkpatrick , L. A., & Davis, K. E.
(1994). Attachment style, gender,
and relationship stability: A longi-
tudinal analysis.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
66,
502–512.
Kirsch , I. (1996). Hypnotic enhance-
ment of cognitive-behavioral weightloss treatments —another meta-
reanalysis.
Journal of Consulting
and
Clinical Psychology
,64,
517–519.
Kirsch ,
I. (2000). The response set the-
ory of hypnosis.
American Journal
of
Clinical Hypnosis
,42, 274 –292.
Kirsch , I. (2005). Empirical resolutions
of the altered state debate.
Contemporary Hypnosis
,22,
18–23.
Kirsch ,
I., & Council, J. R. (1992).
Situational and personality corre-
lates of hypnotic responsiveness. In
E. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.),
Contemporary hypnosis research
(pp. 267 –291). New York:
Guilford.
Kirsch , I., & Lynn, J. L. (1998).
Dissociation theories of hypnosis.
Psychological Bulletin
,123,
100–115.
Kirsch ,
I., & Lynn, S. J. (1995). The al-
tered state of hypnosis: Changes in
the theoretical landscape.
American
Psychologist
,50, 846 –858.
Kirschenbaum , H. (1979).
On becoming
Carl
Rogers
. New York: Delacorte.
Nisbett ,
R. E. (2007, December 9). All
brains are the same color.
New
York
Times
.
Nisbett ,
R. E. (2009).
Intelligence and
how
to get it: Why schools and
culture count
. New York: Norton.
Kitayama ,
S., & Markus, H. R. (Eds.).
(1994).
Emotion and culture:
Empirical
studies of mutual influ-
ence
. Washington, DC: American
Psychological
Association.
Kivimaki , M., Vahtera, J., Elovainio,
M., Helenius, H., Singh-Manoux,A., & Pentti, J. (2005). Optimism
and pessimism as predictors of
change in health after death or on-set of severe illness in family.
Health
Psychology
,24, 413 –421.
Klein , D. C., & Seligman, M. E. P.
(1976). Reversal of performancedeficits and perceptual deficits inlearned helplessness and depres-
sion.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,85,1 1 –26.
Kleinke , C. L., & Kahn, M. L. (1980).
Perceptions of self-disclosers:Effects of sex and physical attrac-
tiveness.
Journal of Personality
,48,
190–205.
Klinesmith ,
J., Kasser, T., &
McAndrew, F. T. (2006). Guns,testosterone, and aggression: An
experimental test of a mediationalhypothesis.
Psychological Science
,
17,
568–572.Klinger, B. I. (1970). Effect of peer
model responsiveness and length of
induction procedure on hypnotic
responsiveness.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,75,1 5 –18.
Klohnen , E. C., & Bera, S. (1998).
Behavioral and experiential pat-
terns of avoidantly and securely at-tached women across adulthood: A31-year longitudinal perspective.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,74, 211 –223.
Klonowicz , T. (2001). Discontented
people: Reactivity and locus of
control as determinants of subjec-
tive well-being.
European Journal
of
Personality
,15,2 9 –47.
Knee , C. R., Canevello, A., Bush, A. L.,
& Cook, A. (2008). Relationship-
contingent self-esteem and the upsand downs of romantic relation-ships.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,95, 608 –627.
Knowles , E. S., & Nathan, K. T. (1997).
Acquiescent responding in self-
reports: Cognitive style or social
concern?
Journal of Research in
Personality
,31, 293 –301.
Kobasa , S. C. (1979). Stressful life
events, personality, and health: Aninquiry into hardiness.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
37,1–11.
Konecni ,
V. J., & Doob, A. N. (1972).
Catharsis through displacement of
aggression.
Journal of Personality
an
d Social Psychology
,23,3 7 9 –387.
Kop, W. J., Berman, D. S., Gransar, H.,
Wong, N. D., Miranda-Peats, R.,
White, M. D., et al. (2005). Socialnetwork and coronary artery calci-fication in asymptomatic indivi-
duals.
Psychosomatic Medicine
,67,
343–352.
Koppitz ,
E. M. (1968).
Psychological
evaluation
of children ’s human
figure drawings
. New York:
Grune
& Stratton.
Korabik, K. (1982). Sex-role orientation
and impressions: A comparison ofdiffering genders and sex roles.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,8,2 5 –30.
Koriat , A., Melkman, R., Averill, J. R.,
& Lazarus, R. S. (1972). The self-control of emotional reactions to astressful film.
Journal of
Personality
,40, 601 –619.
Korn , J. H., Davis, R., & Davis, S. F.
(1991). Historians ’and chairper-
sons’judgments of eminence among
psychologists.
American
Psychologist
,46, 789 –792.REFERENCES 479 |
Kram , M. L., Kramer, G. L., Steciuk,
M., Ronan, P. J., & Petty, F.
(2000). Effects of learned helpless-
ness on brain GABA receptors.
Neuroscience Research
,38,
193–196.
Krantz ,
D. S., & McCeney, M. K.
(2002). Effects of psychological andsocial factors on organic disease: Acritical assessment of research on
coronary heart disease.
Annual
Review
of Psychology
,53,
341–369.
Kring ,
A. M., & Gordon, A. H. (1998).
Sex differences in emotion:
Expression, experience, and physi-
ology.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,74, 686 –703.
Kring , A. M., Smith, D. A., & Neale,
J. M. (1994). Individual differencesin dispositional expressiveness:Development and validation of the
Emotional Expressivity Scale.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,66, 934 –949.
Krokoff , L. J. (1990). Job distress is no
laughing matter in marriage, or isit?
Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships
,8,5–25.
Krueger ,
R. F., & Johnson, W. (2008).
Behavioral genetics and personality.In L. A. Pervin, O. P. John, &R. W. Robins (Eds.),
Handbook of
personality:
Theory and research
(3rd ed., pp. 287 –310). New York:
Guilford.
Krueger , R. F., South, S., Johnson, W.,
& Iacono, W. (2008). The herita-
bility of personality is not always
50%: Gene-environment interac-tions and correlations betweenpersonality and parenting.
Journal
of
Personality
,76, 1485 –1521.
Kuebli , J., Butler, S., & Fivush, R.
(1995). Mother-child talk about
past emotions: Relations of mater-
nal language and child gender over
time.
Cognition and Emotion
,9,
265–283.
Kuhlman ,
T. L. (1985). A study of sa-
lience and motivational theories of
humor.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,49, 281 –286.
Kuiper , N. A., & Derry, P. A. (1981).
The self as a cognitive prototype:
An application to person perceptionand depression. In N. Cantor &J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.),
Personality,
cognition,
and social interaction
(pp. 215 –231). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Kuiper , N. A., MacDonald, M. R., &
Derry, P. A. (1983). Parameters of adepressive self-schema. In J. Suls &
A. G. Greenwald (Eds.),
Psychological perspectives on the
self
(Vol. 2, pp. 191 –217).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kuiper , N. A., & Martin, R. A. (1998).
Laughter and stress in daily life:
Relation to positive and negative
affect.
Motivation and Emotion
,
22,
133–143.
Kuiper , N. A., McKenzie, S. D., &
Belanger, K. A. (1995). Cognitiveappraisal and individual differencesin sense of humor: Motivational
and affective implications.
Personality and Individual
Differences
,19, 359 –372.
Kulick, J. A., & Harackiewicz, J. (1979).
Opposite-sex interpersonal attrac-tion as a function of the sex roles ofthe perceiver and the perceived.
Sex
Roles
,5, 443 –452.
Kuppens , P., Realo, A., & Diener, E.
(2008). The role of positive andnegative emotions in life satisfac-tion judgment across nations.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,95,6 6 –75.
Kurdek, L. A., & Schmitt, J. P. (1986).
Interaction of sex role self-concept
with relationship quality and
relationship beliefs in married,
heterosexual cohabiting, gay andlesbian couples.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
51,
365–370.
Kwon , P. (2000). Hope and dysphoria:
The moderating role of defense
mechanisms.
Journal of
Personality
,68, 199 –223.
LaFraniere , S. (2009, June 13). An all-
nighter? For this test, some Chinesecram all year.
New York Times
,
p.
A4.
LaGasse , L., Gruber, C., & Lipsitt, L. P.
(1989). The infantile expression ofactivity in relation to later assess-
ments. In J. S. Reznick (Ed.),
Perspectives on behavioral inhibi-tion
(pp. 159 –176). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Lahey , B. B. (2009). Public health sig-
nificance of Neuroticism.
American
Psychologist
,64, 241 –256.
Lai, J. C. L., & Wong, W. S. (1998).
Optimism and coping with unem-ployment among Hong KongChinese women.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,32,
454–479.
Lamke ,
L. K., & Bell, N. J. (1982).
Sex-role orientation and relation-
ship development in same-sexdyads.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,16, 343 –354.
Landau, S. F. (1988). Violent crime and
its relation to subjective social stress
indicators: The case of Israel.
Aggressive Behavior
,14, 337 –362.
Landau, S. F., & Raveh, A. (1987).
Stress factors, social support, andviolence in Israeli society: A quan-titative analysis.
Aggressive
Behavior
,13,6 7 –85.
Landy , F. J., Shankster, L. J., & Kohler,
S. S. (1994). Personnel selection and
placement.
Annual Review of
Psychology
,45, 261 –296.
Langens , T. A., & Schuler, J. (2007).
Effects of written emotional ex-pression: The role of positive
expectancies.
Health Psychology
,
26,
174–182.
Langer , E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The
effects of choice and enhanced per-sonal responsibility for the aged: A
field experiment in an institutionalsetting.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,34, 191 –198.
Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., &
Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Can peoplefeel happy and sad at the sametime?
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,81, 684 –696.
Larsen, R. J. (1987). The stability of
mood variability: A spectral ana-
lytic approach to daily mood as-
sessments.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,52,
1195 –1204.
La
rsen, R. J., Billings, D. W., & Cutler,
S. E. (1996). Individual differences
in informational style: Associations
with dispositional affect intensity.
Journal of Personality
,64,1 8 5 –207.
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987).
Affect intensity as an individualdifference characteristic: A review.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
21,1–39.
Larsen,
R. J., Diener, E., & Cropanzano,
R. S. (1987). Cognitive operationsassociated with individual differ-
ences in affect intensity.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
53,
767–774.
Larsen, R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons,
R. A. (1985). An evaluation of
subjective well-being measures.
Social Indicators Research
,17,
1–17.
Larsen,
R. J., Diener, E., & Emmons,
R. A. (1986). Affect intensity and
reactions to daily life events.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,51, 803 –814.480 REFERENCES |
Larsen, R. J., & Kasimatis, M. (1990).
Individual differences in entrain-
ment of mood to the weekly
calendar.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,58,
164–171.
Larsen,
R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989).
Extraversion, neuroticism and sus-ceptibility to positive and negativemood induction procedures.
Personality and IndividualDifferences
,10, 1221 –1228.
Larson , D. G., & Chastain, R. L. (1990).
Self-concealment: Conceptualization,
measurement, and health implica-
tions.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Ps
ychology
,9,4 3 9 –455.
Larson , R. W. (1990). The solitary side
of life: An examination of the time
people spend alone from childhoodto old age.
Developmental Review
,
10,
155–183.
Larson , R. W. (1997). The emergence of
solitude as a constructive domain ofexperience in early adolescence.
Child Development
,68,8 0 –93.
Larson , R. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M.
(1980). The significance of solitudein adolescents ’development.
Journal of Current Adolescent
Medicine
,2,3 3 –40.
Larson , R. W., Csikszentmihalyi, M., &
Graef, R. (1982). Time alone in
daily experience: Loneliness or
renewal? In L. A. Peplau &
D. Perlman (Eds.),
Loneliness: A
sourcebook
of current theory,
research and therapy
(pp. 40 –53).
New
York: Wiley.
Larson , R. W., & Johnson, C. (1985).
Bulimia: Disturbed patterns ofsolitude.
Addictive Behaviors
,10,
281–290.
Larson ,
R. W., & Lee, M. (1996). The
capacity to be alone as a stressbuffer.
Journal of Social
Psychology
,136,5–16.
Larson ,
R. W., & Richards, M. H.
(1991). Daily companionship in
late childhood and early adoles-
cence: Changing developmental
contexts.
Child Development
,62,
284–300.
Larson ,
R. W., Zuzanek, J., & Mannell,
R. (1985). Being alone versus being
with people: Disengagement in the
daily experience of older adults.
Journal of Gerontology
,40,
375–381.
Lau,
J. Y. F., & Eley, T. C. (2008).
Attributional style as a risk markerof genetic effects for adolescent
depressive symptoms.
Journal ofAbnormal
Psychology
,117,
849–859.
Lau,
R. R., Hartman, K. A., & Ware,
J. E. (1986). Health as value:
Methodological and theoreticalconsiderations.
Health Psychology
,
5,2
5–43.
Lauder , W., Mummery, K., Jones, M.,
& Caperchione, C. (2006). A com-parison of health behaviours in
lonely and non-lonely populations.
Psychology, Health, & Medicine
,
11,
233–245.
Laurence , J-R, Beaulieu-Prevost, D., &
du Chene, T. (2008). Measuring
and understanding individual dif-
ferences in hypnotizability. InM. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.),
The Oxford handbook of hypnosis:
Theory,
research and practice
(pp. 225 –253). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Laurenceau , J-P., Feldman Barrett, L., &
Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998).
Intimacy as an interpersonal pro-cess: The importance of self-
disclosure, partner disclosure, and
perceived partner responsiveness ininterpersonal exchanges.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
1238 –1251.
Lavee , Y., & Ben-Ari, A. (2004).
Emotional expressiveness and neu-roticism: Do they predict marital
quality?
Journal of Family
Psychology
,18, 620 –627.
Lax, E. (1991).
Woody Allen: A
biography
. New York: Knopf.
Lazarus ,
R. (1968). Emotions and
adaptation. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.),
Nebraska symposium on motiva-
tion
(pp. 175 –266). Lincoln:
University of Nebraska Press.
Lazarus , R. S. (1974). Cognitive and
coping processes in emotion. InB. Weiner (Ed.),
Cognitive views of
human
motivation
(pp. 21 –32).
New
York: Academic Press.
Lazarus , R. S. (2006). Emotions and
interpersonal relationships: Toward
a person-centered conceptualization
of emotions and coping.
Journal of
Personality
,74,9–46.
Lazarus ,
R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984).
Stress, appraisal and coping
. New
York:
Springer.
Leak , G. K. (1974). Effects of hostility
arousal and aggressive humor on
catharsis and humor preference.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,30, 736 –740.
Leary , M. R. (1983a). Social anxious-
ness: The construct and itsmeasurement.
Journal of
Personality
Assessment
,47,6 6 –75.
Leary , M. R. (1983b).
Understanding
social
anxiety: Social, personality,
and clinical perspectives
. Beverly
Hills,
CA: Sage.
Leary , M. R. (1986). The impact of in-
teractional impediments on socialanxiety and self-presentation.
Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology
,22, 122 –135.
Leary , M. R., & Atherton, S. C. (1986).
Self-efficacy, social anxiety, and in-
hibition in interpersonal encoun-
ters.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,4, 256 –267.
Leary , M. R., Herbst, K. C., &
McCrary, F. (2003). Finding plea-sure in solitary activities: Desire for
aloneness or disinterest in social
contact?
Personality and Individual
Differences
,35,5 9 –68.
Leary , M. R., Knight, P. D., & Johnson,
K. A. (1987). Social anxiety anddyadic conversation: A verbal re-sponse analysis.
Journal of Social
and
Clinical Psychology
,5,3 4 –50.
Leary , M. R., & Kowalski, R. M.
(1995).
Social anxiety
. New York:
Guilford.
Leary ,
M. R., & Meadows, S. (1991).
Predictors, elicitors, and concomi-tants of social blushing.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
60,
254–262.
Ledley , D. R., & Heimberg, R. G.
(2006). Cognitive vulnerability tosocial anxiety.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,25, 755 –778.
Lee, K., Ogunfowora, B., & Ashton,
M. C. (2005). Personality traits be-
yond the Big Five: Are they within
the HEXACO space?
Journal of
Personality
,73, 1437 –1463.
Lee, Y-T., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1997).
Are Americans more optimisticthan the Chinese?
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,23,
32–40.
Leedham ,
B., Meyerowitz, B. E.,
Muirhead, J., & Frist, W. H.
(1995). Positive expectations pre-
dict health after heart transplant.
Health Psychology
,14,7 4 –79.
Lefcourt , H. M. (1982).
Locus of con-
trol:
Current trends in theory and
research
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Lefcourt ,
H. M., Davidson, K.,
Prkachin, K. M., & Mills, D. E.(1997). Humor as a stress modera-tor in the prediction of blood pres-
sure obtained during five stressfulREFERENCES 481 |
tasks. Journal of Research in
Personality
,31, 523 –542.
Lefkowitz , E. S., & Zeldow, P. B.
(2006). Masculinity and femininity
predict optimal mental health: Abelated test of the androgyny
hypothesis.
Journal of Personality
Assessment
,87,9 5 –101.
Lefkowitz , M. M., Eron, L. D., Walder,
L. O., & Huesmann, L. R. (1977).
G r o w i n gu pt ob ev i o l e n t :Al o n -
gitu
dinal study of the development
of aggression
. New York:
Pergam
on.
Leichsenring , F. (2007). Psychodynamic
psychotherapy: A systematic review
of techniques, indications andempirical evidence.
Psychology and
Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research
and Practice
,80, 217 –228.
Leichsenring , F., & Rabung, S. (2008).
Effectiveness of long-term psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy: A meta-
analysis.
Journal of the American
Medical
Association
,300,
1551 –1565.
Lenney ,
E. (1991). Sex roles: The mea-
surement of masculinity, femininity,
and androgyny. In J. P. Robinson,P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman
(Eds.),
Measures of personality
and
social psychological attitudes
(pp. 573 –660). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Lent, R. W., & Lopez, F. G. (2002).
Cognitive ties that bind: A tripartite
view of efficacy beliefs in growth-promoting relationships.
Journal of
Social
and Clinical Psychology
,21,
256–286.
Leon ,
G. R., Gillum, B., Gillum, R., &
Gouze, M. (1979). Personality sta-
bility and change over a 30-year
period —middle age to old age.
Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology
,47, 517 –524.
Lepore , S. J. (1997). Expressive writing
moderates the relation between in-trusive thoughts and depressivesymptoms.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,73,
1030 –1037.
Lerner ,
P., & Lerner, H. (1990).
Rorschach measures of psychoana-
lytic theories of defense. In J.
N. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger(Eds.),
Advances in personality
assessment
(Vol. 8, pp. 121 –160).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Leventhal , E. A., Hansell, S.,
Diefenbach, M., Leventhal, H., &Glass, D. C. (1996). Negative affect
and self-report of physicalsymptoms: Two longitudinal stud-
ies of older adults.
Health
Psychology
,15, 193 –199.
Levin , I., & Stokes, J. P. (1986). An
examination of the relation of in-
dividual difference variables to
loneliness.
Journal of Personality
,
54,
717–733.
Levy, K. N., Blatt, S. J., & Shaver, P. R.
(1998). Attachment styles and pa-rental representations.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
407–419.
Lewin , K. (1938).
The conceptual re-
presentation
and measurement of
psychological forces
. Durham, NC:
Duke
University Press.
Lewin , T. (2008, May 27). 2 colleges
end entrance exam requirement.
New York Times
, p. A19.
Lewinsohn ,
P. M., Joiner, T. E., &
Rohde, P. (2001). Evaluation ofcognitive diathesis-stress models in
predicting major depressive disor-
der in adolescents.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,110,
203–215.
Lewis-Fernandez ,
R., & Kleinman, A.
(1994). Culture, personality, andpsychopathology.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,103,6 7 –71.
Lim, S-L., & Kim, J-H. (2005).
Cognitive processing of emotional
information in depression, panic,
and somatoform disorder.
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology
,114,
50–61.
Linde ,
J. A., Rothman, A. J., Baldwin,
A. S., & Jeffery, R. W. (2006). The
impact of self-efficacy on behavior
change and weight change amongoverweight participants in a weight
loss trial.
Health Psychology
,25,
282–291.
Lindsay ,
J. L., & Anderson, C. A.
(2000). From antecedent conditionsto violent actions: A general affec-
tive aggression model.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,26,
533–547.
Lips,
H. M. (2004). The gender gap in
possible selves: Divergence of aca-demic self-views among high schooland university students.
Sex Roles
,
50,
357–371.
Littig , L. W., & Yeracaris, C. A. (1965).
Achievement motivation and inter-generational occupational mobility.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,1, 386 –389.
Loehlin , J. C. (1992).
Genes and the
environment
in personality devel-
opment
. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Loehlin ,
J. C., McCrae, R. R., & Costa,
P. T. (1998). Heritabilities of com-
mon and measure-specific compo-
nents of the Big Five personalityfactors.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,32, 431 –453.
Loehlin , J. C., Willerman, L., & Horn,
J. M. (1982). Personality resem-
blances between unwed mothersand their adopted-away offspring.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,42, 1089 –1099.
Loehlin , J., Willerman, L., & Horn,
J. M. (1987). Personality resem-
blance in adoptive families: A
10-year follow-up.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
53,
961–969.
Long , B. C., & Sangster, J. I. (1993).
Dispositional optimism/pessimism
and coping strategies: Predictors ofpsychosocial adjustment of rheu-
matoid and osteoarthritis patients.
Journal of Applied Social
Psychology
,23, 1069 –1091.
Long , C. R., & Averill, J. R. (2003).
Solitude: An exploration of benefitsof being alone.
Journal for the The-
or
y of Social Behaviour
,33,2 1 –44.
Long , C. R., Seburn, M., Averill, J. R.,
& More, T. A. (2003). Solitudeexperiences: Varieties, settings, andindividual differences.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,29,
578–583.
Lopez ,
S. J. (Ed.). (2009).
The encyclo-
pedia
of positive psychology
. New
York:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Lord , C. G. (1980). Schemas and images
as memory aids: Two modes ofprocessing social information.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,38, 257 –269.
Lucas , R. E., & Baird, B. M. (2004).
Extraversion and emotional reac-
tivity.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,86, 473 –485.
Lucas , R. E., & Diener, E. (2001).
Understanding extraverts ’enjoy-
ment of social situations: The im-
portance of pleasantness.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
81,
343–356.
Lucas , R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh,
E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-
cultural evidence for the funda-
mental features of extraversion.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,79, 452 –468.
Lucas , R. E., Le, K., & Dyrenforth, P. S.
(2008). Explaining the extraver-
sion/positive affect relation:
Sociability cannot account for482 REFERENCES |
extraverts ’greater happiness.
Journal of Personality
,76,
385–414.
Luhtanen ,
R. K., & Crocker, J. (2005).
Alcohol use in college students:
Effects of level of self-esteem,
narcissism, and contingencies of
self-worth.
Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors
,19,9 9 –103.
Lyness, S. A. (1993). Predictors of dif-
ferences between Type A and B in-dividuals in heart rate and bloodpressure reactivity.
Psychological
Bulletin
,114, 266 –295.
Lynn , R., & Martin, T. (1995). National
differences for thirty-seven nationsin extraversion, neuroticism, psy-choticism and economic, demo-
graphic and other correlates.
Personality and Individual
Differences
,19, 403 –406.
Lynn , S. J., Fassler, O., & Knox, J.
(2005). Hypnosis and the alteredstate debate: Something more ornothing more?
Contemporary
Hypnosis
,22,3 9 –45.
Lynn , S. J., Kirsch, I., & Hallquist,
M. N. (2008). Social cognitive the-ories of hypnosis. In M. R. Nash &A. J. Barnier (Eds.),
The Oxford
ha
ndbook of hypnosis: Theory, re-
search and practice
(pp. 111 –13 9).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Lynn , S. J., Kirsch, I., Knox, J., Fassler,
O., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007).
Hypnosis and neuroscience:
Implications for the altered statedebate. In G. A. Jamieson (Ed.),
Hypnosis and conscious states: The
cognitive
neuroscience perspective
.
New
York: Oxford University
Press.
Lynn , S. J., & Sherman, S. J. (2000). The
clinical importance of sociocogni-
tive models of hypnosis: Responseset theory and Milton Erickson ’s
strategic interventions.
American
Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis
,42,
294–315.
Lynn ,
S. J., Weekes, J. R., Neufeld, V.,
Zivney, O., Brentar, J., & Weiss, F.
(1991). Interpersonal climate and
hypnotizability level: Effects onhypnotic performance, rapport, and
archaic involvement.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
60,
739–743.
Lytton , H. (1977). Do parents create, or
respond to, differences in twins?
Developmental Psychology
,13,
456–459.
MacDonald ,
D. A. (2000). Spirituality:
Description, measurement, andrelation to the five factor model of
personality.
Journal of Personality
,
68,
153–197.
Mackie, M. (1983). The domestication
of self: Gender comparisons of
self-imagery and self-esteem.
Social
Psychology
Quarterly
,46,
343–350.
Maddux ,
J. E., Norton, L. W., & Leary,
M. R. (1988). Cognitive compo-
nents of social anxiety: An investi-
gation of the integration ofself-presentation theory and self-efficacy theory.
Journal of Social and
Cl
inical Psychology
,6,1 8 0 –190.
Magnus , K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., &
Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion and
neuroticism as predictors of objec-
tive life events: A longitudinal
analysis.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,65, 1046 –1053.
Mahalik , J. R., Cournoyer, R. J.,
DeFranc, W., Cherry, M., &Napolitano, J. M. (1998). Men ’s
gender role conflict and use of psy-chological defenses.
Journal of
Counselin
g Psychology
,45, 247– 255.
Mahoney , M. J., & Arnkoff, D. B.
(1979). Self-management. In O.
F. Pomerleau & J. P. Brady (Eds.),
Behavioral medicine: Theory and
practice
(pp. 75 –96). Baltimore:
Williams & Wilkins.
Maier , S. F. (2001). Exposure to the
stressor environment prevents the
temporal dissipation of behavioral
depression/learned helplessness.
Biological Psychiatry
,49, 763 –773.
Maier , S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (2005).
Stressor controllability and learnedhelplessness: The roles of dorsalraphe nucleus, serotonin, and
corticotropin-releasing factor.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews
,29, 829 –841.
Major, B., Carnevale, P. J. D., & Deaux,
K. (1981). A different perspectiveon androgyny: Evaluations of mas-culine and feminine personalitycharacteristics.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
41,
988–1001.
Mallon , S. D., Kingsley, D., Affleck, G.,
& Tennen, H. (1998).
Methodological trends in Journal of
Personality: 1970 –1995.
Journal
of
Personality
,66, 671 –685.
Mansfield , E. D., & McAdams, D. P.
(1996). Generativity and themes of
agency and communion in adultautobiography.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,22,
721–731.Marcus-Newhall ,
A., Pedersen, W. C.,
Carlson, M., & Miller, N. (2000).
Displaced aggression is alive and
well: A meta-analytic review.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,78, 670 –689.
Markey , P. M., Markey, C. N., Tinsley,
B. J., & Ericksen, A. J. (2002). A
preliminary validation of preado-lescents ’self-reports using the five-
factor model of personality.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,36,
173–181.
Marks ,
G., Richardson, J. L., Graham,
J. W., & Levine, A. (1986). Role of
health locus of control beliefs and
expectations of treatment efficacyin adjustment to cancer.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
51,
443–450.
Markus , H. (1977). Self-schemata and
processing information about the
self.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,35,6 3 –78.
Markus , H. (1983). Self-knowledge: An
expanded view.
Journal of
Personality
,51, 543 –565.
Markus , H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991).
Culture and the self: Implications
for cognition, emotion, and moti-
vation.
Psychological Review
,98,
224–253.
Markus ,
H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994).
A collective fear of the collective:Implications for selves and theories
of selves.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,20, 568 –579.
Markus , H., & Kunda, Z. (1986).
Stability and malleability of theself-concept.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,51,
858–866.
Markus ,
H., & Nurius, P. (1986).
Possible selves.
American
Psychologist
,41, 954 –969.
Markus , H., & Sentis, K. (1982). The
self and social information proces-sing. In J. Suls (Ed.),
Psychological
perspectives
on the self
(Vol. 1,
pp.
41–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Markus , H., & Smith, J. (1981). The
influence of self-schemata on theperception of others. In N. Cantor& J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.),
Personality, cognition, and socialinteraction
(pp. 233 –262).
Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Marsh , H. W., Antill, J. K., &
Cunningham, J. D. (1987).
Masculinity, femininity, and an-
drogyny: Relations to self-esteemand social desirability.
Journal of
Personality
,55, 661 –683.REFERENCES 483 |
Marsh , H. W., & Byrne, B. M. (1991).
Differentiated additive androgyny
model: Relations between mascu-
linity, femininity, and multipledimensions of self-concept.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,61, 811 –828.
Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. B.,
Vickers, R. R., Kusulas, J. W., &
Hervig, L. K. (1994). The five-
factor model of personality as a
framework for personality-healthresearch.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,67, 278 –286.
Martin , C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H.
(1995). Children ’s gender-based
reasoning about toys.
Child
Development
,66, 1453 –1471.
Martin , R., & Watson, D. (1997). Style
of anger expression and its relationto daily experience.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,23,
285–294.
Martinez ,
J. C. (1994). Perceived control
and feedback in judgment andmemory.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,28, 374 –381.
Maslow , A. H. (1968).
Toward a psy-
chology
of being
(2nd ed.). New
York:
Van Nostrand.
Maslow , A. H. (1970).
Motivation and
personality
(2nd ed.). New York:
Harper
& Row.
Maslow , A. H. (1971).
The farther
reaches
of human nature
. New
York:
Viking.
Mathersul , D., Williams, L. M.,
Hopkinson, P. J., & Kemp, A. H.
(2008). Investigating models of
affect: Relationships among EEGalpha asymmetry, depression, andanxiety.
Emotion
,8, 560 –572.
Matthews , K. A., & Haynes, S. G.
(1986). Type A behavior patternand coronary risk: Update andcritical evaluation.
American
Journal
of Epidemiology
,123,
923–960.
Matto ,
H. C. (2002). Investigating the
validity of the Draw-A-Person:
Screening procedure for emotional
disturbance: A measurement vali-dation study with high-risk youth.
Personality Assessment
,14,
221–225.
Mayer ,
J. D. (2005). A tale of two
visions. Can a new view of person-ality help integrate psychology?
American Psychologist
,60,
294–307.
Mayer ,
J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988).
The experience and meta-
experience of mood.
Journal ofPersonality
and Social Psychology
,
55,
102–111.
Mayne , T. J., Norcross, J. C., & Sayette,
M. A. (1994). Admission require-
ments, acceptance rates, and finan-cial assistance in clinical psychology
programs: Diversity across the
practice-research continuum.
American Psychologist
,49,
806–811.
McAdams ,
D. P. (1993).
Stories we live
by:
Personal myths and the making
of the self
. New York: Morrow.
McAdams ,
D. P. (2004). Generativity
and the narrative ecology of familylife. In M. W. Pratt & B. H. Fiese(Eds.),
Family stories and the life
course:
Across time and genera-
tions
. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McAdams ,
D. P., Bauer, J. J., Sakaeda,
A. R., Anyidoho, N. A., Machado,
M. A., Magrino-Failla, K., et al.
(2006). Continuity and change in
the life story: A longitudinal studyof autobiographical memories inemerging adulthood.
Journal of
Personality
,74, 1371 –1400.
McAdams , D. P., Diamond, A., de St.
Aubin, E., & Mansfield, E. (1997).
Stories of commitment: The psy-
chosocial construction of generative
lives.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,72, 678 –694.
McAdams , D. P., Hart, H. M., &
Maruna, S. (1998). The anatomy ofgenerativity. In D. P. McAdams &E. de St. Aubin (Eds.),
Generativity
and
adult development: How and
why we care for the next generation
(pp. 7– 43). Washington, DC:
American Psychological
Association.
McAdams , D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2006). A
new big five. Fundamental princi-
ples for an integrative science ofpersonality.
American Psychologist
,
61,
204–217.
McAdams , D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis,
M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman, P. J.(2001). When bad things turn good
and good things turn bad:
Sequences of redemption and con-tamination in life narrative andtheir relation to psychosocial
adaptation in midlife adults and in
students.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,27,
474–485.
McCarthy ,
E. D., Langner, T. S.,
Gersten, J. C., Eisenberg, J. G., &
Orzeck, L. (1975). Violence andbehavior disorders.
Journal of
Communication
,25,7 1 –85.McCauley , C., Woods, K., Coolidge, C.,
& Kulick, W. (1983). More ag-
gressive cartoons are funnier.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,44, 817 –823.
McClelland , D. C. (1961).
The achiev-
ing
society
. Princeton, NJ: Van
Nostrand.
McClelland ,
D. C. (1980). Motive dis-
positions: The merits of operant and
respondent measures. In L. Wheeler
(Ed.),
Review of personality and so-
ci
al psychology
(Vol. 1,p p .1 0 –41).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
McClelland , D. C. (1985). How mo-
tives, skill, and values determinewhat people do.
American
Psychologist
,40, 812 –825.
McClelland , D. C., Atkinson, J. W.,
Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L.(1953).
The achievement motive
.
New
York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts.
McClelland , D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E.
(1982). Leadership motive patternand long-term success in manage-
ment.
Journal of Applied
Psychology
,67, 737 –743.
McClelland , D. C., & Pilon, D. A.
(1983). Sources of adult motives inpatterns of parent behavior in early
childhood.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,44,
564–574.
McClure ,
E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic
review of sex differences in facial
expression processing and theirdevelopment in infants, children,
and adolescents.
Psychological
Bulletin
,126, 424 –453.
McCrae , R. R. (2001). 5 years of
progress: A reply to Block.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,35,
108–113.
McCrae ,
R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr.
(1983). Social desirability scales:More substance than style.
Journal
of
Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,51, 882 –888.
McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1986).
Personality, coping, and coping
effectiveness in an adult sample.
Journal of Personality
,54,
385–405.
McCrae ,
R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1995).
Positive and negative valence withinthe five-factor model.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,29,
443–460.
McCrae ,
R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997).
Personality trait structure as ahuman universal.
American
Psychologist
,52, 509 –516.484 REFERENCES |
McCrae , R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2008).
The five-factor theory of personal-
ity. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, &
L. A. Pervin (Eds.),
Handbook of
personality
(3rd ed.). New York:
Guilford.
Mc
Crae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Martin, T. A.,
Oryol, V. E., Rukavishnikov, A. A.,Senin, I. G., et al. (2004). Consensualvalidation of personality traits across
cultures.
Journal of Research in
Per
sonality
,38,1 7 9 –201.
McCrae , R. R., Jang, K. L., Livesley,
W. J., Riemann, R., & Angleitner,A. (2001). Sources of structure:
Genetic, environmental and arti-
factual influences on the covaria-tion of personality traits.
Journal of
Personality
,69, 511 –535.
McCrae , R. R., Terracciano, A., et al.
(2005a). Universal features of per-
sonality traits from observer ’s
perspective: Data from 50 cultures.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,88, 547 –561.
McCrae , R. R., Terracciano, A., et al.
(2005b). Personality profiles ofcultures: Aggregate personalitytraits.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,89, 407 –425.
McGhee , P. E. (1979).
Humor: Its origin
and
development
. San Francisco:
W.
H. Freeman.
McGrath , M. J., & Cohen, D. B. (1978).
REM sleep facilitation of adaptive
waking behavior: A review of the
literature.
Psychological Bulletin
,
85,2
4–57.
McGue , M., & Christensen, K. (1997).
Genetic and environmental contri-butions to depression symptom-atology: Evidence from Danish
twins 75 years of age and older.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
106,
439–448.
McGuire , M. T., & Troisi, A. (1990).
Anger: An evolutionary view. In
R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.),
Emotion: Theory, research, and
experience
(Vol. 5, pp. 43 –57). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
McGuire , P. A. (1999, March).
Therapists see new sense in use ofhumor.
APA Monitor
, pp. 1, 10.
McGuire ,
W. J., & McGuire, C. V.
(1982). Significant others in self-space: Sex differences and develop-mental trends in the social self. In
J. Suls (Ed.),
Psychological per-
spectives
on the self
(Vol. 1, pp.
71–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
McNally , R. J., & Geraerts, E., (2009).
A new solution to the recoveredmemory debate.
Perspectives on
Psychological
Science
,4, 126 –134.
McPherson , M., Smith-Lovin, L., &
Brashears, M. E. (2006). Social
isolation in America: Changes incore discussion networks over two
decades.
American Sociological
Review
,71, 353 –375.
Measelle , J. R., John, O. P., Ablow,
J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan,C. P. (2005). Can children provide
coherent, stable, and valid self-
reports on the Big Five dimensions?A longitudinal study from ages 5 to
7.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,89,9 0 –106.
Mednick , M., & Thomas, V. (2008).
Women and achievement. In F.L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.),
Psychology of women: A handbook
of
issues and theories
(2nd ed., pp.
625–651).
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Meeker , W. B., & Barber, T. X. (1971).
Toward an explanation of stagehypnosis.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,77,6 1 –70.
Meece , J. L., Anderman, E. M., &
Anderman, L. H. (2006).Classroom goal structure, studentmotivation, and academic achieve-
ment.
Annual Review of
Psychology
,57, 487 –503.
Meichenbaum , D. H. (1985).
Stress in-
oculation
training
. New York:
Pergamon.
Meichenbaum ,
D. H., & Cameron, R.
(1983). Stress inoculation training:
Toward a general paradigm for
training coping skills. In
D. Meichenbaum & M. E. Jaemko(Eds.),
Stress reduction and pre-
vention
(pp. 115 –157). New York:
Plenum.
Meichenbaum , D. H., & Deffenbacher,
J. L. (1988). Stress inoculationtraining.
Counseling Psychologist
,
16,6
9–90.
Meissner , W. W. (1984).
Psychoanalysis
and
religious experience
. New
Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
Meleshko , K. G. A., & Alden, L. E.
(1993). Anxiety and self-disclosure:Toward a motivational model.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,64, 1000 –1009.
Melges , F. T., & Weisz, A. E. (1971).
The personal future and suicidal
ideation.
Journal of Nervous and
Mental
Disease
,153, 244 –250.
Mellman , T. A., David, D., Bustamante,
V., Torres, J., & Fins, A. (2001).Dreams in the acute aftermath of
trauma and their relationship toPTSD.
Journal of Traumatic Stress
,
14,
241–247.
Mershon , B., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1988).
Number of factors in the personal-
ity sphere: Does increase in factorsincrease predictability of real-life
criteria?
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,55, 675 –680.
Metropolitan Area Child Study
Research Group (2007). Changing
the way children “think ”about
aggression: Social-cognitive effects
of a preventive intervention.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,75, 160 –167.
Meyer , G. J. (1997). Assessing reliabil-
ity: Critical corrections for a critical
examination of the Rorschach
Comprehensive System.
Psycholog-
ical
Assessment
,9, 480 –489.
Meyer , G. J., & Shack, J. R. (1989). The
structural convergence of mood andpersonality: Evidence for old and
new directions.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
57,
691–706.
Michalski , R. L., & Shackelford, T. K.
(2002). An attempted replication of
the relationships between birthorder and personality.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,36,
182–188.
Mickelson ,
K. D., Kessler, R. C., &
Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attach-
ment in a nationally representative
sample.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,73, 1092 –1106.
Mikulincer , M., & Nachshon, O.
(1991). Attachment styles and pat-terns of self-disclosure.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
61,
321–331.
Miles , D. R., & Carey, G. (1997).
Genetic and environmental archi-tecture of human aggression.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,72, 207 –217.
Miller , I. W., & Norman, W. H. (1979).
Learned helplessness in humans:
A review and attribution theory
model.
Psychological Bulletin
,86,
93–118.
Miller ,
M. G., & Ostlund, N. M.
(2006). The effect of a parentingprime on sex differences in mate
selection criteria.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,32,
1459 –1468.
Miller ,
N. E. (1941). The frustration-
aggression hypothesis.
Psychologi-
cal
Review
,48, 337 –346.
Miller , N., Pedersen, W. C., Earleywine,
M., & Pollock, V. E. (2003).REFERENCES 485 |
A theoretical model of triggered
displaced aggression.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Review
,7,
75–97.
Milling ,
L. S., Reardon, J. M., &
Carosella, G. M. (2006). Mediation
and moderation of psychological
pain treatments: Response expec-tancies and hypnotic suggestibility.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,74, 253 –262.
Mineka , S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A
contemporary learning theory per-
spective on the etiology of anxiety
disorders.
American Psychologist
,
61,1
0–26.
Mischel , W. (1968).
Personality and
assessment
. New York: Wiley.
Mischel ,
W. (1973). Toward a cognitive
social learning reconceptualization
of personality.
Psychological
Review
,80, 252 –283.
Many Americans fed up with diet
advice. (2001, January 2).
New
York
Times
, p. F10.
Mischel ,
W. (1983). Alternatives in the
pursuit of the predictability andconsistency of persons: Stable datathat yield unstable interpretations.
Journal of Personality
,51,
578–604.
Mischel ,
W. (1990). Personality dispo-
sitions revisited and revised: A viewafter three decades. In L. A. Pervin
(Ed.),
Handbook of personality:
Theory
and research
(pp. 111 –134).
New
York: Guilford.
Mischel , W. (2009). From Personality
and Assessment (1968) to personality
science 2009.
Journal of Research in
P
ersonality
,43,2 8 2 –290.
Mischel , W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A
cognitive-affective system theory of
personality: Reconceptualizing si-tuations, dispositions, dynamics,and invariance in personality
structure.
Psychological Review
,
102,
246–268.
Mischel , W., & Shoda, Y. (2008).
Toward a unified theory of per-sonality: Integrating dispositions
and processing dynamics within thecognitive-affective processing sys-tem. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins,
& L. A. Pervin (Eds.),
Handbook of
personality:
Theory and research
(3rd ed., pp. 208 –241). New York:
Guilford.
Mitchell , R. E., Cronkite, R. C., &
Moos, R. H. (1983). Stress, coping,
and depression among marriedcouples.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,92, 433 –448.Mitte , K. (2005). Meta-analysis of
cognitive-behavioral treatments for
generalized anxiety disorder: A
comparison with pharmacotherapy.
Psychological Bulletin
,131,
785–795.
Moehler ,
E., Kagan, J., Brunner, R.,
Wiebel, A., Kaufmann, C., &
Resch, F. (2006). Association ofbehavioral inhibition with hair
pigmentation in a European sam-
ple.
Biological Psychology
,72,
344–346.
Moehler ,
E., Kagan, J., Oelkers-Ax, R.,
Brunner, R., Poustka, L., Haffner,
J., et al. (2008). Infant predictors of
behavioural inhibition.
British
Journal
of Developmental
Psychology
,26, 145 –150.
Moffitt , T. E., Caspi, A., Milne, B. J.,
Melchior, M., Goldberg, D., &
Poulton, R. (2007). Generalized
anxiety disorder and depression:
Childhood risk factors in a birthcohort followed to age 32.
Psychological Medicine
,37,
441–452.
Moilanen ,
D. L. (1993). Depressive
information processing amongnonclinic, nonreferred college stu-
dents.
Journal of Counseling
Psychology
,40, 340 –347.
Moller , I., & Krahe, B. (2009).
Exposure to violent video gamesand aggression in German adoles-
cents: A longitudinal analysis.
Aggressive Behavior
,35,7 5 –89.
Moneta , G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M.
(1996). The effect of perceived
challenges and skills on the qualityof subjective experience.
Journal of
Personality
,64, 275 –310.
Moretti, M. M., Segal, Z. V., McCann,
C. D., Shaw, B. F., Miller, D. T., &Vella, D. (1996). Self-referent ver-sus other-referent information pro-
cessing in dysphoric, clinically
depressed, and remitted depressedsubjects.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,22,6 8 –80.
Morfei , M. Z., Hooker, K., Fiese, B. H.,
& Cordeiro, A. M. (2001).Continuity and change in parentingpossible selves: A longitudinal
follow-up.
Basic and Applied Social
Psychology
,23, 217 –223.
Morton , T. L. (1978). Intimacy and re-
ciprocity of exchange: A compari-son of spouses and strangers.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,36,7 2 –81.
Mosher , C. E., & Danoff-Burg, S.
(2006). Health effects of expressiveletter writing.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,25, 1122 –
1139.
Mount , M. K., Barrick, M. R., &
Strauss, J. P. (1994). Validity ofobserver ratings of the Big Five
personality factors.
Journal of
Applied
Psychology
,79, 272 –280.
Moustakas, C. E. (1968).
Individuality
and
encounter
. Cambridge, MA:
Doyle.
Mundorf ,
N., Bhatia, A., Zillmann, D.,
Lester, P., & Robertson, S. (1988).
Gender differences in humor ap-
preciation.
Humor
,1, 231 –243.
Murayama , K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009).
The joint influence of personal
achievement goals and classroom
goal structures on achievement-
related outcomes.
Journal of
Educational
Psychology
,101,
432–447.
Murray ,
B. (1998, June). Study says TV
violence still seen as heroic, glam-orous.
APA Monitor
, p. 16.
Murray ,
E. J., Lamnin, A. D., & Carver,
C. S. (1989). Emotional expressionin written essays and psychother-apy.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,8, 414 –429.
Murray , H. A. (1938).
Explorations in
personality:
A clinical and experi-
mental study of fifty men of college
age
. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Murray ,
H. A. (1967). Henry A.
Murray. In E. G. Boring &
G. Lindzey (Eds.),
A history of
psychology
in autobiography
(Vol.
5,
pp. 285 –310). New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Murray , J. A., & Terry, D. J. (1999).
Parental reactions to infant death:The effects of resources and copingstrategies.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,18, 341 –369.
Myers, D. G. (1992).
The pursuit of
happiness:
Who is happy —and
why
. New York: Morrow.
Nadon ,
R., Hoyt, I. P., Register, P. A.,
& Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991).Absorption and hypnotizability:Context effects reexamined.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,60, 144 –153.
Nasby , W., & Read, N. W. (1997). The
life voyage of a solo circumnaviga-
tor: Integrating theoretical and
methodological perspectives.
Journal of Personality
,65,
785–1068.
Nash ,
M. (1987). What, if anything, is
regressed about hypnotic age486 REFERENCES |
regression? A review of the empiri-
cal literature.
Psychological
Bulletin
,102,4 2 –52.
Neale , M. C., Rushton, P., & Fulker,
D. W. (1986). Heritability of item
responses on the Eysenck
Personality Questionnaire.
Personality and Individual
Differences
,7, 771 –779.
Neale , M. C., & Stevenson, J. (1989).
Rater bias in the EASITemperament Scales: A twin study.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,56, 446 –455.
Neimeyer , R. A. (2001). Repertory Grid
Technique. In W. E. Craighead &C. B. Nemeroff (Eds.),
The Corsini
encyclopedia
of psychology and
behavioral science
(3rd ed., Vol. 4,
pp.
1394 –1395). New York: Wiley.
Neisser , U., Boodoo, G., Bourchard, T.
J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci,
S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C.,
Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., &Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence:Knowns and unknowns.
American
Psychologist
,51,7 7 –101.
Nelson , N. W., Sweet, J. J., & Demakis,
G. J. (2006). Meta-analysis of the
MMPI-2 Fake Bad Scale: Utility in
forensic practice.
Clinical
Neuropsychologist
,20,3 9 –58.
Nes, L. S., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2006).
Dispositional optimism and coping:A meta-analytic review.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Review
,10,
235–251.
Nestoriuc ,
Y., Rief, W., & Martin, A.
(2008). Meta-analysis of biofeed-
back for tension-type headache:
Efficacy, specificity, and treatmentmoderators.
Journal of Clinical and
Consulting
Psychology
,76,
379–396.
Nevid ,
J. S., & Spencer, S. A. (1978).
Multivariate and normative data
pertaining to the RAS with the
college population.
Behavior
Therapy
,9, 675.
Nevo , O., & Nevo, B. (1983). What do
you do when asked to answerhumorously?
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,44,
188–194.
Ng,
T. W. H., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby,
L. T. (2006). Locus of control atwork: A meta-analysis.
Journal of
Organizational
Behavior
,27,
1057 –1087.
Niaura ,
R., Todaro, J. F., Stroud, L.,
Spiro, A., Ward, K. D., & Weiss, S.(2002). Hostility, the metabolic
syndrome, and incident of coronaryheart disease.
Health Psychology
,
21,
588–593.
Nicholson , I. A. M. (1997). To
“Correlate Psychology and Social
Ethics ”: Gordon Allport and the
first course in American personalitypsychology.
Journal of Personality
,
65,
733–742.
Nicholson , R. A., Mouton, G. J., Bagby,
R. M., Buis, T., Peterson, S. A., &Buigas, R. A. (1997). Utility of
MMPI-2 indicators of responsedistortion: Receiver operatingcharacteristic analysis.
Psychologi-
cal
Assessment
,9, 471 –479.
Nikles , C. D., Brecht, D. L., Klinger, E.,
& Bursell, A. L. (1998). The effects
of current-concern- and
nonconcern-related waking sugges-
tions on nocturnal dream content.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,75, 242 –255.
Niles , S. (1998). Achievement goals and
means: A cultural comparison.
Journal of Cross-CulturalPsychology
,29, 656 –667.
Noguchi, K., Gohm, C. L., & Dalsky,
D. J. (2006). Cognitive tendenciesof focusing on positive and negativeinformation.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,40, 891 –910.
Nolen-Hoeksema , S. (2000). The role of
rumination in depressive disorders
and mixed anxiety/depressive
symptoms.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,109, 504 –511.
Nolen-Hoeksema , S. (2001). Gender
differences in depression.
Current
Directions
in Psychological Science
,
10,
173–176.
Noll, R. (1997).
The Aryan Christ: The
secret
life of Carl Jung
. New York:
Random
House.
Noller , P. (1984).
Nonverbal communi-
cation
and marital interaction
.
Oxford:
Pergamon.
Nordstrom , C. R., & Segrist, D. J.
(2009). Predicting the likelihood of
going to graduate school: The im-portance of locus of control.
College
St
udent Journal
,43,2 0 0 –206.
Norem , J. K. (2001).
The positive power
of
negative thinking: Using defen-
sive pessimism to harness anxiety
and perform at your peak
.
Cambridge,
MA: Basic Books.
Norem , J. K. (2008). Defensive pessi-
mism as a positive self-critical tool. InE. C. Chang (Ed.),
Self-criticism and
se
lf-enhancement: Theory, research,
and clinical implications
(pp. 89 –
10
4). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.Norem , J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986a).
Anticipatory and post hoc cushion-
ing strategies: Optimism and de-
fensive pessimism in “risky ”
situations.
Cognitive Therapy and
Research
,10, 347 –362.
Norem , J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986b).
Defensive pessimism: Harnessing
anxiety as motivation.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
51,
1208 –1217.
Norem , J. K., & Illingworth, K. S. S.
(1993). Strategy-dependent effectsof reflecting on self and tasks: Some
implications of optimism and defen-
sive pessimism.
Journal of Personal-
it
y and Social Psychology
,65,
82
2–835.
Norem , J. K., & Illingworth, K. S. S.
(2004). Mood and performanceamong defensive pessimists and stra-tegic optimists.
Journal of Research
in
Personality
,38,3 5 1 –366.
Norman , P., & Bennett, P. (1996).
Health locus of control. In
M. Conner & P. Norman (Eds.),
Predicting health behaviour:
Research
and practice within social
cognition models
(pp. 62 –94).
Buckingham,
England: Open
University Press.
Northey , W. F. (2002). Characteristics
and clinical practices of marriageand family therapists: A national
survey.
Journal of Marital and
Family
Therapy
,28, 487 –494.
Oakley , D. A. (2008). Hypnosis, trance
and suggestion: Evidence fromneuroimaging. In M. R. Nash &
A. J. Barnier (Eds.),
The Oxford
handbook
of hypnosis: Theory, re-
search and practice
(pp. 365 –392).
New
York: Oxford University
Press.
O’Brien, M., Peyton, V., Mistry, R.,
Hruda, L., Jacobs, A., Caldera, Y.,
et al. (2000). Gender-role cognition
in three-year-old boys and girls.
Sex
Roles
,42, 1007 –1025.
Ochse , R., & Plug, C. (1986). Cross-
cultural investigation of the validityof Erikson ’s theory of personality
development.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
50,
1240 –1252.
O’Connor , B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (2001).
Rigid and extreme: A geometricrepresentation of personality disor-
ders in five-factor model space.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,81, 1119 –1130.
O’Connor , S. C., & Rosenblood, L. K.
(1996). Affiliation motivation inREFERENCES 487 |
everyday experience: A theoretical
comparison.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,70,
513–522.
O’Hea,
E. L., Grothe, K. B., Bodenlos,
J. S., Boudreaux, E. D., White,
M. A., & Brantley, P. J. (2005).
Predicting medical regimen adher-ence: The interactions of healthlocus of control beliefs.
Journal
of
Health Psychology
,10,
705–717.
O’Heron ,
C. A., & Orlofsky, J. L.
(1990). Stereotypic and nonstereo-
typic sex role trait and behavior
orientations, gender identity, andpsychological adjustment.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,58, 134 –143.
Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Goals,
culture, and subjective well-being.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,27, 1674 –1682.
Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D-W.,
Kim-Prieto, C., & Choi, I. (2007).
The dynamics of daily events
and well-being across cultures:
When less is more.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
93,
685–698.
Okazaki , S. (1997). Sources of ethnic
differences between AsianAmerican and White Americancollege students on measures of
depression and social anxiety.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
106,5
2–60.
Orgler ,H .( 1 9 6 3 ) .
Alfred Adler: The man
an
d his work
. New York: Liveright.
Orlofsky ,
J. L., & O ’Heron, C. A.
(1987). Stereotypic and nonstereo-
typic sex role trait and behavior
orientations: Implications for per-
sonal adjustment.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
52,
1034 –1042.
Otto, M. W., Teachman, B. A., Cohen,
L. S., Soares, C. N., Vitonis, A. F.,& Harlow, B. L. (2007).Dysfunctional attitudes and epi-
sodes of major depression:
Predictive validity and temporalstability in never-depressed, de-
pressed, and recovered women.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
116,
475–483.
Ouellette , J. A., Hessling, R., Gibbons,
F. X., Reis-Bergan, M., & Gerrard,
M. (2005). Using images to increase
exercise behavior: Prototypes ver-sus possible selves.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,31,
610–620.Ovcharchyn ,
C. A., Johnson, H. H., &
Petzel, T. P. (1981). Type A be-
havior, academic aspirations, and
academic success.
Journal of
Personality
,49, 248 –256.
Overmier , J. B. (2002). On learned
helplessness.
Integrative
Physiological
& Behavioral
Science
,37,4–8.
Overmier ,
J. B., & Seligman, M. E. P.
(1967). Effects of inescapable shock
upon subsequent escape andavoidance learning.
Journal of
Comparative
and Physiological
Psychology
,63,2 8 –33.
Overskeid, G. (2007). Looking for
Skinner and finding Freud.
American
Ps
ychologist
,62,5 9 0 –595.
Oyserman , D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K.
(2006). Possible selves and aca-demic outcomes: How and whenpossible selves impel action.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,91, 188 –204.
Oyserman , D., & Markus, H. R. (1990).
Possible selves and delinquency.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,59, 112 –125.
Oyserman , D., & Saltz, E. (1993).
Competence, delinquency, and
attempts to attain possible selves.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,65, 360 –374.
Paik, H., & Comstock, G. (1994). The
effects of television violence on an-tisocial behavior: A meta-analysis.
Communication Research
,21,
516–546.
Palker-Corell ,
A., & Marcus, D. K.
(2004). Partner abuse, learnedhelplessness, and trauma symp-toms.
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology
,23, 445 –462.
Papsdorf , M., & Alden, L. (1998).
Mediators of social rejection in
social anxiety: Similarity, self-
disclosure, and overt signs of anxi-
ety.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,32, 351 –369.
Park, C. L., & Adler, N. E. (2003).
Coping style as a predictor of healthand well-being across the first yearof medical school.
Health
Psychology
,22, 627 –631.
Park, L. E. (2007). Appearance-based
rejection sensitivity: Implicationsfor mental and physical health,affect, and motivation.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,33,
490–504.
Park,
L. E., Crocker, J., & Kiefer, A. K.
(2007). Contingencies of self-
worth, academic failure, and goalpursuit.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,33,
1503 –1517.
Parker ,
D. R., & Rogers, R. W. (1981).
Observation and performance of
aggression: Effects of multiple
models and frustration.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,7,
302–308.
Parker ,
K. C. H., Hanson, R. K., &
Hunsley, J. (1988). MMPI,
Rorschach, and WAIS: A meta-
analytic comparison of reliability,stability, and validity.
Psychological
Bul
letin
,103, 367– 373.
Parker , W. D. (1998). Birth order effects
in the academically talented.
Gifted
Child
Quarterly
,42,2 9 –38.
Parton, D. A., & Geshuri, Y. (1971).
Learning of aggression as a functionof presence of a human model, re-sponse intensity, and target of the
response.
Journal of Experimental
Child
Psychology
,20, 304 –318.
Pasupathi , M., McLean, K. C., &
Weeks, T. (2009). To tell or not totell: Disclosure and the narrative
self.
Journal of Personality
,77,
89–123.
Patterson ,
D. R., & Jensen, M. P.
(2003). Hypnosis and clinical pain.
Psychological Bulletin
,129,
495–521.
Paulhus ,
D. (1983). Sphere-specific
measures of perceived control.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,44, 1253 –1265.
Paulhus , D. L. (1984). Two-component
models of socially desirable re-sponding.
Journal of Personality and
So
cial Psychology
,46,5 9 8 –609.
Paulhus , D. L. (1991). Measurement
and control of response bias. InJ. P. Robinson, P. S. Shaver, &L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.),
Measures
of
personality and social psycholog-
ical attitudes
(Vol. 1,p p .1 7 –59).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Paulhus , D. L., Duncan, J. H., & Yik,
M. S. M. (2002). Patterns of shyness
in East-Asian and European-heritage
students.
Journal of Research in
Pe
rsonality
,36,4 4 2 –462.
Paulhus , D. L., & Martin, C. L. (1987).
The structure of personality capa-bilities.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,52, 354 –365.
Paulhus , D. L., & Morgan, K. L. (1997).
Perception of intelligence in lead-erless groups: The dynamic effectsof shyness and acquaintance.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,72, 581 –591.488 REFERENCES |
Paunonen , S. V. (1998). Hierarchical
organization of personality and
prediction of behavior.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
538–556.
Paunonen , S. V., & Ashton, M. C.
(2001). Big Five factors and facets
and the prediction of behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,81, 524 –539.
Paunonen , S. V., & Jackson, D. N.
(2000). What is beyond the BigFive? Plenty!
Journal of Personality
,
68,
821–835.
Payne , S. C., Youngcourt, S. S., &
Beaubein, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of the goalorientation nomological net.
Journal of Applied Psychology
,92,
128–150.
Peacock ,
E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1996).
Anticipatory stress: The relation of
locus of control, optimism, and
control appraisals to coping.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
30,
204–222.
Pedersen , D. M. (1999). Model for types
of privacy by privacy functions.
Journal of EnvironmentalPsychology
,19, 397 –405.
Pedersen , N. L., Plomin, R., McClearn,
G. E., & Friberg, L. (1988).Neuroticism, extraversion, and re-lated traits in adult twins reared
apart and reared together.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,55, 950 –957.
Pedersen , P. B. (2008). Ethics, compe-
tence, and professional issues in
cross-cultural counseling. InP. B. Pedersen, J. G. Draguns,W. J. Lonner, & J. E. Trimble
(Eds.).,
Counseling across cultures
(6th ed., pp. 5 –20). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pedersen , W. C., Bushman, B. J.,
Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N.
(2008). Kicking the (barking) dog
effect: The moderating role of targetattributes on triggered displaced ag-
gression.
Personality and Social
Ps
ychology Bulletin
,34,1 3 8 2 –1395.
Pedhazur , E. J., & Tetenbaum, T. J.
(1979). Bem Sex Role Inventory: Atheoretical and methodological cri-
tique.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,37, 996 –1016.
Pennebaker , J. W. (1989). Confession,
inhibition, and disease. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in
experimental
social psychology
(Vol. 22, pp. 211 –244). New York:
Academic Press.Pennebaker , J. W. (2000). The effects of
traumatic disclosure on physical
and mental health: The values of
writing and talking about upsettingevents. In J. M. Violanti, D. Paton,
& C. Dunning (Eds.),
Posttraumatic stress intervention:Challenges,
issues, and perspectives
(pp. 97 –114). Springfield, IL:
Charles C Thomas.
Pennebaker , J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986).
Confronting a traumatic event:
Toward an understanding of inhi-bition and disease.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,95,
274–281.
Pennebaker ,
J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp,
L. K. (1990). Accelerating the cop-
ing process.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,58,
528–537.
Pennebaker ,
J. W., & O ’Heeron, R. C.
(1984). Confiding in others and
illness rates among spouses of sui-
cide and accidental-death victims.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
,
93,
473–476.
Peplau, L. A., Russell, D., & Heim, M.
(1979). The experience of loneli-ness. In I. Frieze, D. Bar-Tel, &
J. Carroll (Eds.),
New approaches
to
social problems
(pp. 53 –78). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Perez-Edgar , K., Roberson-Nay, R.,
Hardin, M. G., Poeth, K., Guyer,
A. E., Nelson, E. E., et al. (2007).
Attention alters neural responses toevocative faces in behaviorally in-
hibited adolescents.
NeuroImage
,
35,
1538 –1546.
Perrig-Chiello , P., Perrig, W. G., &
Staehelin, H. B. (1999). Healthcontrol beliefs in old age:
Relationship with subjective andobjective health, and health behav-iour.
Psychology, Health and
Medicine
,4,8 3 –94.
Peterson , C., & Bossio, L. M. (2001).
Optimism and physical well-being.
In E. C. Chang (Ed.),
Optimism and
pessimism:
Implications for theory,
research, and practice
(pp. 127 –
145).
Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Peterson , C., Maier, S. F., & Seligman,
M. E. P. (1993).
Learned helpless-
ness:
A theory for the age of
personal control
. New York:
Oxford
University Press.
Peterson , C., Seligman, M. E. P., &
Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Pessimistic
explanatory style is a risk factor for
physical illness: A thirty-five-yearlongitudinal study.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
55,2
3–27.
Peterson , C., Seligman, M. E. P., Yurko,
K. H., Martin, L. R., & Friedman,
H. S. (1998). Catastrophizing and
untimely death.
Psychological
Science
,9, 127 –130.
Peterson , C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer,
C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky,G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982).
The Attributional Style
Questionnaire.
Cognitive Therapy
and
Research
,6, 287 –300.
Peterson , C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An
expanded Attributional StyleQuestionnaire.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,97,8 7 –89.
Petrie , K. J., Booth, R. J., & Pennebaker,
J. W. (1998). The immunologicaleffects of thought suppression.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,75, 1264 –1272.
Petrocelli , J. V., & Smith, E. R. (2005).
Who I am, who we are, and why:
Links between emotions and causal
attributions for self- and group
discrepancies.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,31,
1628 –1642.
Phares ,
E. J. (1976).
Locus of control in
personality
. Morristown, NJ:
General
Learning Press.
Phillips , A. G., & Silva, P. J. (2005).
Self-awareness and the emotional
consequences of self-discrepancies.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,31, 703 –713.
Phillips , S. D., & Bruch, M. A. (1988).
Shyness and dysfunction in careerdevelopment.
Journal of
Counseling
Psychology
,35,
159–165.
Piccione ,
C., Hilgard, E. R., &
Zimbardo, P. G. (1989). On thedegree of stability of measured
hypnotizability over a 25-year pe-
riod.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,56, 289 –295.
Piedmont , R. L. (1999). Does spirituality
represent the sixth factor of per-sonality? Spiritual transcendenceand the five-factor model.
Journal
of
Personality
,67, 985 –1013.
Piedmont , R. L., McCrae, R. R.,
Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A.(2000). On the invalidity of validityscales: Evidence from self-reports
and observer ratings in volunteer
samples.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,78, 582 –593.
Pilkonis , P. A. (1977a). Shyness, public
and private, and its relationship toREFERENCES 489 |
other measures of social behavior.
Journal of Personality
,45,
585–595.
Pilkonis ,
P. A. (1977b). The behavioral
consequences of shyness.
Journal of
Personality
,45, 596 –611.
Pinderhughes , E. E., & Zigler, E. (1985).
Cognitive and motivational deter-
minants of children ’s humor
responses.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,19, 185 –196.
Pinquart , M., & Sorensen, S. (2001).
Influences on loneliness in older
adults: A meta-analysis.
Basic and
Applied
Social Psychology
,23,
245–266.
Piotrowski ,
C., & Keller, J. W. (1989).
Psychological testing in outpatient
mental health facilities: A national
study.
Professional Psychology:
Research
and Practice
,20,
423–425.
Piotrowski ,
C., & Zalewski, C. (1993).
Training in psychodiagnostic test-ing in APA-approved PsyD andPhD clinical psychology programs.
Journal of Personality Assessment
,
61,
394–405.
Pistole , M. C. (1989). Attachment in
adult romantic relationships: Style
of conflict resolution and relation-
ship satisfaction.
Journal of Social
and
Personal Relationships
,6,
505–510.
Pizzagalli ,
D. A., Sherwood, R. J.,
Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J.(2005). Frontal brain asymmetryand reward responsiveness.
Psychological Science
,16, 805 –813.
Plomin , R., & Caspi, A. (1999).
Behavioral genetics and personality.
In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.),
Handbook of personality: Theory
and
research
(2nd ed., pp. 251 –
276).
New York: Guilford.
Plomin , R., Chipuer, H. M., & Loehlin,
J. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics
and personality. In L. A. Pervin
(Ed.),
Handbook of personality:
Theory
and research
(pp. 225 –243).
New
York: Guilford.
Plomin , R., & Crabbe, J. (2000). DNA.
Psychological Bulletin
,126,
806–828.
Plomin ,
R., Corley, R., Caspi, A.,
Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. (1998).Adoption results for self-reportedpersonality: Evidence for nonaddi-
tive genetic effects?
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
75,
211–218.
Plomin , R., & DeFries, J. C. (1998,
May). Genetics of cognitive abilitiesand disabilities.
Scientific
American
,6 2–69.
Polman ,
H., de Castro, B. O., & van
Aken, M. A. G. (2008).
Experimental study of the differen-tial effects of playing versus
watching violent video games on
children ’s aggressive behavior.
Aggressive Behavior
,34, 256 –264.
Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G.,
& Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue:Environmental gender stereotypesin the first two years of life.
Sex
Roles
,22, 359 –367.
Poropat , A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis
of the Five-Factor Model of per-sonality and academic perfor-mance.
Psychological Bulletin
,135,
322–338.
Powch,
I. G., & Houston, B. K. (1996).
Hostility, anger-in, and cardiovas-cular reactivity in White women.
Health Psychology
,15, 200 –208.
Powers , S. I., Pietromonaco, P. R.,
Gunlicks, M., & Sayer, A. (2006).
Dating couples ’attachment styles
and patterns of cortisol reactivity
and recovery in response to arelationship conflict.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
90,
613–628.
Pozo , C., Carver, C. S., Wellens, A. R.,
& Scheier, M. F. (1991). Socialanxiety and social perception:
Construing others ’reactions to the
self.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,17, 355 –362.
Prager , K. J. (1986). Intimacy status: Its
relationship to locus of control, self-
disclosure, and anxiety in adults.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,12,9 1 –109.
Pratt, M. W., Norris, J. E.,
Hebblethwaite, S., & Arnold, M. L.(2008). Intergenerational transmis-sion of values: Family generativity
and adolescents ’narratives of parent
and grandparent value teaching.
Journal of Personality
,76,1 7 1 –198.
Pressman, S. D., Cohen, S., Miller, G. E.,
Barkin, A., Rabin, B. S., & Treanor,
J. J. (2005). Loneliness, social net-work size, and immune response toinfluenza vaccination in college
freshmen.
Health Psychology
,24,
297–306.
Ptacek ,
J. T., Pierce, G. R., &
Thompson, E. L. (2006). Findingevidence of dispositional coping.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
40,
1137 –1151.
Ptacek , J. T., Smith, R. E., & Dodge,
K. L. (1994). Gender differences incoping with stress: When stressor
and appraisals do not differ.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,20, 421 –430.
Puca, R. M., & Schmalt, H-D. (2001).
The influence of the achievement
motive on spontaneous thoughts in
pre- and postdecisional action
phases.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,27, 302 –308.
Quinlan, S. L., Jaccard, J., & Blanton,
H. (2006). A decision theoretic andprototype conceptualization ofpossible selves: Implications for the
prediction of risk behavior.
Journal
of
Personality
,74, 599 –630.
Raag, T., & Rackliff, C. L. (1998).
Preschoolers ’awareness of social
expectations of gender:
Relationships to toy choices.
Sex
Roles
,38, 685 –700.
Raikkonen , K., Matthews, K. A., Flory,
J. D., & Owens, J. F. (1999). Effects
of hostility on ambulatory bloodpressure and mood during dailyliving in healthy adults.
Health
Psychology
,18,4 4 –53.
Raikkonen , K., & Matthews, K. A.
(2008). Do dispositional pessimism
and optimism predict ambulatory
blood pressure during schooldays
and nights in adolescents?
Journal
of
Personality
,76, 605 –629.
Rasmussen , H. N., Wrosch, C., Scheier,
M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2006). Self-regulation processes and health:The importance of optimism andgoal adjustment.
Journal of
Personality
,74, 1721 –1747.
Rathus , S. A. (1973). A 30-item schedule
for assessing assertive behavior.
Behavior Therapy
,4, 398 –406.
Rawsthorne, L. J., & Elliot, A. J. (1999).
Achievement goals and intrinsicmotivation: A meta-analytic review.
Personality and Social Psychology
Review
,3, 326 –344.
Raymond B. Cattell. (1997).
American
Psychologist
,52, 797 –799.
Raz, A., Kirsch, I., Pollard, J., & Nitkin-
Kaner, Y. (2006). Suggestion re-
duces the Stroop effect.
Psychological Science
,17,9 1 –95.
Regalado , A. (2006, June 16). Scientist ’s
study of brain genes sparks abacklash.
Wall Street Journal
,
pp.
A1, A12.
Reich , J. W., & Zautra, A. J. (1997).
Locus of control influences
diathesis-stress effects in rheuma-
toid arthritis patients.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,31,
423–438.490 REFERENCES |
Reilly , R. R., & Chao, G. T. (1982).
Validity and fairness of some alter-
nativeemployeeselectionprocedures.
Personnel Psychology
,35,1–62 .
Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996).
Attachment and intimacy:
Component processes. In
E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski
(Eds.),
Social psychology:
Ha
ndbook of basic principles
(pp. 523 –56 3). New York: Guilford.
Reise , S. P., & Waller, N. G. (1993).
Traitedness and the assessment of
response pattern scalability.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,65, 143 –151.
Renaud , J. M., & McConnell, A. R.
(2007). Wanting to be better but
thinking you can ’t: Implicit theories
of personality moderate the impact
of self-discrepancies on self-esteem.
Self and Identity
,6,4 1 –50.
Rescorla , R. A. (1988). Pavlovian con-
ditioning: It ’s not what you think it
is.
American Psychologist
,43,
151–160.
Revelle ,
W., & Oehlberg, K. (2008).
Integrating experimental andobservational personality research –
the contributions of Hans Eysenck.
Journal of Personality
,76,
1387 –1414.
Reynolds ,
S. K., & Clark, L. A. (2001).
Predicting dimensions of personal-
ity disorder from domains and
facets of the five-factor model.
Journal of Personality
,69,
199–222.
Reznick ,
J. S., Kagan, J., Snidman, N.,
Gersten, M., Baak, K., &
Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibitedand uninhibited children: A follow-
up study.
Child Development
,57,
660–680.
Rhee ,
S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002).
Genetic and environmental influ-ences on antisocial behavior: A
meta-analysis of twin and adoptionstudies.
Psychological Bulletin
,128,
490–529.
Rhodewalt ,
F., & Comer, R. (1982).
Coronary-prone behavior andreactance: The attractiveness of aneliminated choice.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,8,
152–158.
Rhodewalt ,
F., & Davison, J. (1983).
Reactance and the coronary-prone
behavior pattern: The role of self-
attribution in responses to reducedbehavioral freedom.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
44,
220–228.Rholes , W. S., Simpson, J. A., Campbell,
L., & Grich, J. (2001). Adult at-
tachment and transition to parent-
hood.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,81, 421 –435.
Riemann , R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau,
J. (1997). Genetic and environ-
mental influences on personality: Astudy of twins reared together usingthe self- and peer-report NEO-FFI
Scales.
Journal of Personality
,65,
449–475.
Richards ,
J. C., Hof, A., & Alvarenga,
M. (2000). Serum lipids and theirrelationships with hostility and
angry affect and behaviors in men.
Health Psychology
,19, 393 –398.
Rimer , S. (2008, September 22). College
panel calls for less focus on SATs.
New York Times
, p. A14.
Rimm-Kaufman ,
S. E., & Kagan, J.
(2005). Infant predictors of kinder-
garten behavior: The contribution
of inhibited and uninhibited
temperament types.
Behavioral
Disorders
,30, 331 –347.
Riordan, C. A., & Tedeschi, J. T.
(1983). Attraction in aversiveenvironments: Some evidence forclassical conditioning and negative
reinforcement.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
44,
683–692.
Riso, L. P., du Toit, P. L., Blandino,
J. A., Penna, S., Dacey, S., Duin,
J. S., et al. (2003). Cognitive aspects
of chronic depression.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,112,
72–80.
Ritts,
V., & Patterson, M. L. (1996).
Effects of social anxiety and actionidentification on impressions and
thoughts in interaction.
Journal of
Social
and Clinical Psychology
,15,
191–205.
Robbins ,
P. R., Tanck, R. H., & Houshi,
F. (1985). Anxiety and dream sym-
bolism.
Journal of Personality
,53,
17–22.
Roberts ,
B., & Hogan, R. (2001).
Personality psychology in theworkplace
. Washington, DC:
American
Psychological
Association.
Roberts , B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner,
R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R.
(2007). The power of personality:The comparative validity of per-
sonality traits, socioeconomic
status, and cognitive ability forpredicting important life outcomes.
Perspectives on Psychological
Science
,2, 313 –345.Roberts , B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008).
Personality trait change in adult-
hood.
Current Directions in
Psychological
Science
,17,3 1 –35.
Roberts , B. W., Walton, K. E., &
Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of
mean-level change in personality
traits across the life course: A meta-
analysis of longitudinal studies.
Psychological Bulletin
,132,1–25.
Robins ,
R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt,
T. E. (2002). It ’s not just who
you’re with, it ’s who you are:
Personality and relationship ex-
periences across multiple relation-
ships.
Journal of Personality
,70,
925–964.
Robinson ,
B. S., Davis, K. L., & Meara,
N. M. (2003). Motivational attri-
butes of occupational possible
selves for low-income rural women.
Journal of Counseling Psychology
,
50,
156–164.
Robinson , M. S., & Alloy, L. B. (2003).
Negative cognitive styles and stress-reactive rumination interact to
predict depression: A prospective
study.
Cognitive Therapy and
Research
,27, 275 –292.
Robles , T. F., Brooks, K. P., &
Pressman, S. D. (2009). Trait posi-tive affect buffers the effects ofacute stress on skin barrier recov-ery.
Health Psychology
,28,
373–378.
Roche ,
S. M., & McConkey, K. M.
(1990). Absorption: Nature, as-sessment, and correlates.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
59,9
1–101.
Rodgers, J. L., Cleveland, H. H., van
den Oord, E., & Rowe, D. C.
(2000). Resolving the debate over
birth order, family size, and intelli-gence.
American Psychologist
,55,
599–612.
Rodin ,
J., & Langer, E. J. (1977). Long-
term effects of a control-relevantintervention with the institutional-ized aged.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,35,
897–902.
Roepke ,
S., & Renneberg, B. (2009).
“Effectiveness of long-term psy-
chodynamic psychotherapy: Ameta-analysis ”: Comment.
Journal
of
the American Medical
Association
,301, 931 –932.
Rogers , C. R. (1947). The case of Mary
Jane Tildon. In W. U. Snyder (Ed.),
Casebook of nondirective counsel-ing
(pp. 128 –203). Cambridge,
MA: Houghton Mifflin.REFERENCES 491 |
Rogers , C. R. (1951). Client-centered
therapy:
Its current practice, impli-
cations, and theory
. Boston:
Houghton
Mifflin.
Rogers , C. R. (1954). The case of
Mrs. Oak: A research analysis. In
C. R. Rogers & R. F. Dymond
(Eds.),
Psychotherapy and person-
ality
change
(pp. 259 –348).
Chicago:
University of Chicago
Press.
Rogers , C. R. (1961).
On becoming a
person:
A therapist ’s view of psy-
chotherapy
. Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
Rogers ,
C. R. (1967). Carl R. Rogers. In
E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.),
A
history
of psychology in autobiog-
raphy
(Vol. 5, pp. 341 –384). New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Rogers , C. R. (1969).
Freedom to learn:
A
view of what education might
become
. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Rogers ,
C. R. (1970).
Carl Rogers on
encounter
groups
. New York:
Harper
& Row.
Rogers , C. R. (1977).
Carl Rogers on
personal
power
. New York:
Delacorte.
Rogers ,
C. R. (1982, August). Nuclear
war: A personal response.
APA
Monitor
, pp. 6 –7.
Rogers ,
M. E., Hansen, N. B., Levy,
B. R., Tate, D. C., & Sikkema, K. J.
(2005). Optimism and coping with
loss in bereaved HIV-infected men
and women.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,24, 341 –360.
Rogers , T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker,
W. S. (1977). Self-reference and theencoding of personal information.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,35, 677 –688.
Rokach , A. (1998). The relation of cul-
tural background to the causes of
loneliness.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,17,7 5 –88.
Roney , C. J. R., & O ’Connor, M. C.
(2008). The interplay betweenachievement goals and specific tar-
get goals in determining perfor-
mance.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,42, 482 –489.
Rook, K. S., & Peplau, L. A. (1982).
Perspectives on helping the lonely.In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman(Eds.),
Loneliness
(pp. 351 –378).
New
York: Wiley.
Roos , P. E., & Cohen, L. H. (1987). Sex
roles and social support as mod-erators of life stress adjustment.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,52, 576 –585.Rose , R. J., Koskenvuo, M., Kaprio, J.,
Sarna, S., & Langinvainio, H.
(1988). Shared genes, shared ex-
periences, and similarity of person-ality: Data from 14,288 adultFinnish co-twins.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
54,
161–171.
Rosenberg , A., & Kagan, J. (1989).
Physical and physiological corre-
lates of behavioral inhibition.
Developmental Psychobiology
,22,
753–770.
Rosenman ,
R. H., Brand, R. J., Jenkins,
C. D., Friedman, M., Straus, R., &
Wurm, M. (1975). Coronary heart
disease in the western collaborativegroup study: Final follow-up expe-
rience of 8-1/2 years.
Journal of the
American
Medical Association
,
233,
872–877.
Rosenthal , R. (1979). The “file drawer
problem ”and tolerance for null
results.
Psychological Bulletin
,86,
638–641.
Rosenthal ,
R. (1990). How are we doing
in soft psychology?
American
Psychologist
,45, 775 –777.
Ross, M., & Holmberg, D. (1992). Are
wives ’memories for events in rela-
tionships more vivid than their
husbands ’memories?
Journal of
Social
and Personal Relationships
,
9,
585–604.
Rosse , J. G., Stecher, M. D., Miller, J. L.,
& Levin, R. A. (1998). The impact
of response distortion on preem-ployment personality testing and
hiring decisions.
Journal of Applied
Psychology
,83, 634 –644.
Rotenberg , K. J. (1994). Loneliness and
interpersonal trust.
Journal of
Social
and Clinical Psychology
,13,
152–173.
Rotenberg ,
K. J. (1997). Loneliness and
the perception of the exchange ofdisclosures.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,16, 259 –276.
Roth , S. (1980). A revised model of
learned helplessness in humans.
Journal of Personality
,48,
103–133.
Rothbart ,
M. K. (2007). Temperament,
development, and personality.
Current Directions in Psychological
Science
,16, 207 –212.
Rothbart , M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994).
Temperament and the development
of personality.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,103,5 5 –66.
Rothbart , M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans,
D. E. (2000). Temperament andpersonality: Origins and outcomes.Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,78, 122 –135.
Rotter , J. B. (1954).
Social learning and
clinical
psychology
. Englewood
Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Rotter , J. B. (1966). Generalized expec-
tancies for internal versus external
control of reinforcement.
Psychological Monographs
,80(1).
Rotter ,
J. B. (1982).
The development
and
applications of social learning
theory: Selected papers
. New York:
Praeger.
Rotter ,
J. B., Chance, J. E., & Phares,
E. J. (Eds.). (1972).
Applications of
a
social learning theory of person-
ality
. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Rowe ,
D. C. (1987). Resolving the
person-situation debate: Invitation
to an interdisciplinary dialogue.
American Psychologist
,42,
218–227.
Rubenstein ,
C. M., & Shaver, P. (1980).
Loneliness in two northern cities. InJ. Hartog, J. R. Andy, & Y.
A. Cohen (Eds.),
The anatomy of
loneliness
(pp. 319 –337). New
York: International Universities
Press.
Rubin , J. A., Provenzano, F. J., & Luria,
Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder:
Parents ’views of sex of newborns.
American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry
,44, 512 –519.
Rubins , J. L. (1978).
Karen Horney:
Gentle
rebel of psychoanalysis
.
New
York: Dial.
Rubinstein , G., & Strul, S. (2007). The
Five Factor Model (FFM) among
four groups of male and femaleprofessionals.
Journal of Research
in
Personality
,41, 931 –937.
Rudasill , K. M., & Konold, T. R.
(2008). Contribution of children ’s
temperament to teacher ’s judg-
ments of social competence fromkindergarten through second grade.
Early Education and Development
,
19,
643–666.
Rudasill , K. M., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E.,
Justice, L. M., & Pence, K. (2006).Temperament and language skillsas predictors of teacher-child
relationship quality in preschool.
Early Education and Development
,
17,
271–291.
Russell , D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona,
C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA
Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and
discriminant validity.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
39,
472–480.492 REFERENCES |
Russell , J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999).
On the bipolarity of positive and
negative affect.
Psychological
Bulletin
,125,3–30.
Rusting ,
C. L. (1998). Personality,
mood, and cognitive processing of
emotional information: Three con-
ceptual frameworks.
Psychological
Bulletin
,124, 165 –196.
Rusting , C. L. (1999). Interactive effects
of personality and mood on
emotion-congruent memory andjudgment.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,77,
1073 –1086.
Rusting ,
C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1998).
Personality and cognitive proces-sing of affective information.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,24, 200 –213.
Rutledge , T., Linke, S. E., Olson, M. B.,
Francis, J., Johnson, B. D., Bittner,
V., et al. (2008). Social networks
and incident stroke among women
with suspected myocardial ische-mia.
Psychosomatic Medicine
,70,
282–287.
Ruvolo ,
A. P., & Markus, H. R. (1992).
Possible selves and performance:The power of self-relevant imagery.
Social Cognition
,10,9 5 –124.
Sackett , P. R., Kuncel, N. R., Arneson,
J. J., Cooper, S. R., & Waters, S. D.
(2009). Does socioeconomic status
explain the relationship between
admissions tests and post-secondary academic performance?
Psychological Bulletin
,135,1–22.
Sadalla ,
E. K., Kenrick, D. T., &
Vershure, B. (1987). Dominanceand heterosexual attraction.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,52, 730 –738.
Sadler , W. A., & Johnson, T. B. (1980).
From loneliness to anomie. In
J. Hartog, J. R. Audy, & Y.
A. Cohen (Eds.),
The anatomy of
loneliness
(pp. 34 –64). New York:
International Universities Press.
Salgado , J. F. (1997). The five factor
model of personality and job
performance in the European
community.
Journal of Applied
Psychology
,82,3 0 –43.
Salili, F. (1994). Age, sex, and cultural
differences in the meaning anddimensions of achievement.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,20, 635 –648.
Sandstrom , M. J., & Cramer, P. (2003).
Girls ’use of defense mechanisms
following peer rejection.
Journal of
Personality
,71, 603 –627.Sanford , S., & Eder, D. (1984).
Adolescent humor during peer
interaction.
Social Psychology
Quarterly
,47, 235 –243.
Sanna , L. J., Chang, E. C., Carter, S. E.,
& Small, E. M. (2006). The future
is now: Prospective temporal self-
appraisals among defensive pessi-
mists and optimists.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,32,
727–739.
Sansone ,
C., & Harackiewicz, J. M.
(1998). “Reality ”is complicated.
American Psychologist
,53,
673–674.
Saragovi ,
C., Koestner, R., Di Dio, L., &
Aube, J. (1997). Agency, commu-nion, and well-being: Extending
Helgeson ’s (1994) model.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,73, 593 –609.
Sarbin , T. R. (1950). Contributions to
role-taking theory: I. Hypnotic
behavior.
Psychological Review
,
57,
225–270.
Sarbin , T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1972).
Hypnosis: A social psychological
analysis
of influence communica-
tion
. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Sarbin ,
T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1979).
Hypnosis and psychopathology:
Replacing old myths with freshmetaphors.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,88, 506 –526.
Sargent, R. T., Crocker, J., & Luhtanen,
R. K. (2006). Contingencies of self-
worth and depressive symptoms in
college students.
Journal of Social
and
Clinical Psychology
,25,
628–646.
Sarkar ,
U., Ali, S., & Whooley, M. A.
(2009). Self-efficacy as a marker of
cardiac function and predictor of
heart failure hospitalization andmortality in patients with stable
coronary heart disease: Findings
from the Heart and Soul study.
Health Psychology
,28, 166 –173.
Saucier , G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1998).
What is beyond the Big Five?
Journal of Personality
,66,
495–524.
Savickas ,
M. L. (1997). Constructivist
career counseling: Models and
methods. In G. J. Neimeyer &
R. A. Neimeyer (Eds.),
Advances in
personal
construct psychology
(Vol. 4, pp. 149 –182). Greenwich,
CT: JAI Press.
Sayers, S. L., Baucom, D. H., & Tierney,
A. M. (1993). Sex roles, interper-
sonal control, and depression: Whocan get their way?
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,27,
377–395.
Scarr ,
S. (1969). Social introversion-
extraversion as a heritable re-
sponse.
Child Development
,40,
823–832.
Scarr ,
S., & Carter-Saltzman, L. (1979).
Twin method: Defense of a criticalassumption.
Behavior Genetics
,9,
527–542.
Scarr ,
S., Webber, P. L., Weinberg,
R. A., & Wittig, M. A. (1981).Personality resemblances among
adolescents and their parents in
biologically related and adoptivefamilies.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,40, 885 –898.
Scarr-Salapatek , S. (1971). Race, social
class, and IQ.
Science
,174,
1286 –1295.
Scheier ,
M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985).
Optimism, coping, and health:
Assessment and implications of
generalized outcome expectancies.
Health Psychology
,4, 219 –247.
Scheier , M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges,
M. W. (2001). Optimism, pessi-mism, and psychological well-being. In E. C. Chang (Ed.),
Optimism and pessimism:Implications
for theory, research,
and practice
(pp. 189 –216).
Washington,
DC: American
Psychological Association.
Scheier , M. F., Matthews, K. A., Owens,
J. F., Magovern, G. J., Lefebvre,R. C., Abbott, R. A., & Carver,
C. S. (1989). Dispositional opti-
mism and recovery from coronaryartery bypass surgery: The benefi-
cial effects on physical and psycho-
logical well-being.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
57,
1024 –1040.
Scheier , M. F., Weintraub, J. K., &
Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with
stress: Divergent strategies of opti-
mists and pessimists.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
51,
1257 –1264.
Schimmack , U., & Diener, E. (1997).
Affect intensity: Separating inten-sity and frequency in repeatedly
measured affect.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
73,
1313 –1329.
Schlenker , B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982).
Social anxiety and self-presentation:
A conceptualization and model.
Psychological Bulletin
,92,6 4 1 –669.
Schlenker , B. R., Weigold, M. F., &
Hallam, J. R. (1990). Self-servingREFERENCES 493 |
attributions in social context:
Effects of self-esteem and social
pressure.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,58, 855 –863.
Schmalt , H-D. (1999). Assessing the
achievement motive using the grid
technique.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,33, 109 –130.
Schmidt , J. A., Shernoff, D. J., &
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007).Individual and situational factors
related to the experience of flow in
adolescence: A multilevel approach.In A. D. Ong & M. H. M. van
Dulmen (Eds.),
Oxford handbook
of
methods in positive psychology
(pp. 542 –558). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Schmidt , N., Gooding, R. Z., Noe,
R. A., & Kirsch, M. (1984). Meta-
analyses of validity studies pub-lished between 1964 and 1982 and
the investigation of study charac-
teristics.
Personnel Psychology
,37,
407–422.
Schmidt ,
N., & Sermat, V. (1983).
Measuring loneliness in different
relationships.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
44,
1038 –1047.
Schmukle , S. C., Egloff, B., & Burns,
L. R. (2002). The relationship be-
tween positive and negative affectin the Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,36, 463 –475.
Schulz , R., & Heckhausen, J. (1999).
Aging, culture and control: Settinga new research agenda.
Journal of
Gerontology:
Psychological
Sciences
,54B, 139 –145.
Schuyler , B. A., & Coe, W. C. (1981). A
physiological investigation of voli-
tional and nonvolitional experienceduring posthypnotic amnesia.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,40, 1160 –1169.
Schwartz , C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan,
J. (1996). Early childhood temper-
ament as a determinant of exter-
nalizing behavior in adolescence.
Development and
Psychopathology
,8, 527 –537.
Schwartz , C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan,
J. (1999). Adolescent social anxietyas an outcome of inhibited temper-ament in childhood.
Journal of the
American
Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry
,38,
1008 –1015.
Schwartz ,
C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin,
L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, S. L.
(2003). Inhibited and uninhibitedinfants “grown up ”: Adult amyg-
dalar response to novelty.
Science
,
300,
1952 –1953.
Schwartz , R. D., & Higgins, R. L.
(1979). Differential outcome from
automated assertion training as a
function of locus of control.
Journal
of
Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,47, 686 –694.
Sears , R. R. (1941). Non-aggressive re-
actions to frustration.
Psychological
Review
,48, 343 –346.
Segal , D. L., Coolidge, F. L., & Mizuno,
H. (2007). Defense mechanism dif-ferences between younger and older
adults: A cross-sectional investiga-
tion.
Aging & Mental Health
,11,
415–422.
Segal ,
D. L., & Murray, E. J. (1993).
Emotional processing in cognitivetherapy and vocal expression offeeling.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,13, 189 –206.
Segal , Z. V., Gemar, M., & Williams, S.
(1999). Differential cognitive re-
sponse to a mood challenge fol-
lowing successful cognitive therapy
or pharmacotherapy for unipolardepression.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,108,3–10.
Segerstrom ,
S. C. (2007). Optimism and
resources: Effects on each other andon health over 10 years.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,41,
772–786.
Segrin ,
C. (1999). Social skills, stressful
life events, and the development ofpsychosocial problems.
Journal
of
Social and Clinical Psychology
,
18,1
4–34.
Segrin , C., & Flora, J. (2000). Poor so-
cial skills are a vulnerability factor
in the development of psychosocial
problems.
Human Communication
Research
,26, 489 –514.
Segura , S. L., & Gonzalez-Roma, V.
(2003). How do respondentsconstrue ambiguous responseformats of affect items?
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
85,
956–968.
Seidlitz , L., & Diener, E. (1998). Sex
differences in the recall of affectiveexperiences.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,74,
262–271.
Seligman ,
M. E. P. (1975).
Helplessness:
On
depression, development and
death
. San Francisco: W. H.Freeman.
Seligman ,
M. E. P. (1976).
Learned
helplessness
and depression in ani-
mals and men
. Morristown, NJ:
General
Learning Press.Seligman , M. E. P., & Hager, J. L.
(Eds.). (1972).
Biological bound-
aries
of learning
. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Seligman , M. E. P., & Maier, S. F.
(1967). Failure to escape traumatic
shock.
Journal of Experimental
Psychology
,74,1–9.
Seligman ,
M. E. P., & Schulman, P.
(1986). Explanatory style as a pre-dictor of productivity and quitting
among life insurance sales agents.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,50, 832 –838.
Seligman , M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park,
N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positivepsychology progress: Empiricalvalidation of interventions.
American Psychologist
,60,
410–421.
Senko ,
C., & Harackiewicz, J. M.
(2005). Achievement goals, task
performance, and interest: Why
perceived goal difficulty matters.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,31, 1739 –1753.
Shadish , W. R., & Baldwin, S. A.
(2005). Effects of behavioral mari-tal therapy: A meta-analysis ofrandomized controlled trials.
Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology
,73,6–18.
Shah ,
J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001).
Regulatory concerns and appraisal
efficiency: The general impact of
promotion and prevention.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,80, 693 –705.
Shaw , J. S. (1982). Psychological an-
drogyny and stressful life events.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,43, 145 –153.
Shedler , J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M.
(1993). The illusion of mentalhealth.
American Psychologist
,48,
1117 –1131.
Sheldon ,
K. M. (1994). Emotionality
differences between artists andscientists.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,28, 481 –491.
Sheldon , W. H. (1942).
The varieties of
temperament:
A psychology of
constitutional differences
.
New
York: Harper & Row.
Shepperd , J. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1990).
Shyness and self-presentation. InW. R. Crozier (Ed.),
Shyness and
embarrassment:
Perspectives from
social psychology
(pp. 286 –314).
Cambridge:
Cambridge University
Press.
Shepperd , J. A., Maroto, J. J., & Pbert,
L. A. (1996). Dispositional494 REFERENCES |
optimism as a predictor of health
changes among cardiac patients.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
30,
517–534.
Shiffman , S., Balabanis, M. H., Paty,
J. A., Engberg, J., Gwaltney, C. J.,
Liu, K. S., et al. (2000). Dynamic
effects of self-efficacy on smokinglapse and relapse.
Health
Psychology
,19, 315 –323.
Shiner , R. L. (1998). How shall we
speak of children ’s personalities in
middle childhood? A preliminarytaxonomy.
Psychological Bulletin
,
124,
308–332.
Shoda , Y., Tiernan, S. L., & Mischel, W.
(2002). Personality as a dynamicalsystem: Emergence of stability and
distinctiveness from intra- and in-
terpersonal interactions.
Personality and Social PsychologyReview
,6, 316 –325.
Showers , C. (1992). The motivational
and emotional consequences ofconsidering positive and negativepossibilities for an upcoming event.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,63, 474 –484.
Shurcliff , A. (1968). Judged humor,
arousal, and the relief theory.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,4, 360 –363.
Siegel , B. (2007, June 12). Stressful times
for Chinese students.
Time
.
Siegler ,
I. C. (1994). Hostility and risk:
Demographic and lifestyle vari-
ables. In A. W. Siegman &T. W. Smith (Eds.),
Anger, hostility,
and
the heart
(pp. 199 –214).
Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Siegman , A. W. (1994). From Type A to
hostility to anger: Reflections on the
history of coronary-prone behavior.
In A. W. Siegman & T. W. Smith(Eds.),
Anger, hostility, and the
heart
(pp. 1– 21). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Siem, F. M. (1998). Metatraits and
self-schemata: Same or different?
Journal of Personality
,66,
783–803.
Simms ,
L. J. (2007). The Big Seven
model of personality and its rele-vance to personality pathology.
Journal of Personality
,75,6 5 –94.
Simoni , J. M., & Ng, M. T. (2002).
Abuse, health locus of control,
and perceived health among
HIV-positive women.
Health
Psychology
,21,8 9 –93.
Simpson , J. A. (1990). Influence of
attachment styles on romantic rela-tionships.
Journal of Personalityand
Social Psychology
,59,
971–980.
Simpson ,
J. A., Collins, A., Tran, S., &
Haydon, K. C. (2007). Attachment
and the experience and expressionof emotion in romantic relation-
ships: A developmental perspective.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,92, 355 –367.
Simpson , J. A., Ickes, W., & Grich, J.
(1999). When accuracy hurts:
Reactions of anxious-ambivalentdating partners to a relationshipthreatening situation.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
76,
754–769.
Simpson , J. A., Rholes, W. S., &
Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support
seeking and support giving within
couples in an anxiety-provokingsituation: The role of attachmentstyles.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,62, 434 –446.
Simpson , J. A., Rholes, W. S., Orina,
M. M., & Grich, J. (2002).
Working models of attachment,
support giving, and support seeking
in a stressful situation.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,28,
598–608.
Simpson ,
T. L., & Arroyo, J. A. (1998).
Coping patterns associated withalcohol-related negative conse-quences among college women.
Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology
,17, 150 –166.
Singer , J. A. (2004). Narrative identity
and meaning making across the adult
lifespan: An introduction.
Journal of
Pe
rsonality
,72,4 3 7 –459.
Singer , J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1981).
Television, imagination, and ag-gression:
A study of preschoolers
.
Hillside,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Sipley , C. G., Fischer, R., & Lui, J. H.
(2005). Reliability and validity ofthe Revised Experiences in Close
Relationships (ECR-R) self-reportmeasure of adult romantic attach-ment.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,31,
1524 –1536.
Skinner ,
B. F. (1967). B. F. Skinner. In
E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey (Eds.),
A
history
of psychology in autobiog-
raphy
(Vol. 5, pp. 387 –413). New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner , B. F. (1971).
Beyond freedom
and
dignity
. New York: Bantam.
Skinner ,
B. F. (1974).
About behavior-
ism
. New York: Vintage Books.
Skinner ,
B. F. (1983).
A matter of con-
sequences
. New York: Knopf.Skinner ,
E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., &
Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for
the structure of coping: A review
and critique of category systems forclassifying ways of coping.
Psychological Bulletin
,129,
216–269.
Slater ,
J., & Depue, R. A. (1981). The
contribution of environmental
events and social support to serious
suicide attempts in primary depres-
sive disorder.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,90, 275 –285.
Slavkin , M., & Stright, A. D. (2000).
Gender role differences in collegestudents from one- and two-parentfamilies.
Sex Roles
,42,2 3 –37.
Slife, B., & Rychlak, J. F. (1982). Role
of affective assessment in modelingaggressive behavior.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
43,
861–868.
Sloan, W. W., & Solano, C. H. (1984).
The conversational styles of lonelymales with strangers and room-mates.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,10, 293 –301.
Sloman, L. (2008). A new comprehen-
sive evolutionary model of depres-
sion and anxiety.
Journal of
Affective
Disorders
,106, 219 –228.
Slotter , E. B., & Finkel, E. J. (2009). The
strange case of sustained dedicationto an unfulfilling relationship:
Predicting commitment and
breakup from attachment anxietyand need fulfillment within rela-tionships.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,35,8 5 –100.
Smillie , L. D., Pickering, A. D., &
Jackson, C. J. (2006). The new
reinforcement sensitivity theory:
Implications for personality mea-
surement.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Review
,10, 320 –335.
Smith, C. A., Wallston, K. A., & Dwyer,
K. A. (1995). On babies and bathwater: Disease impact and negativeaffectivity in the self-reports ofpersons with rheumatoid arthritis.
Health Psychology
,14,6 4 –73.
Smith, C. E., Fernengel, K., Holcroft, C.,
Gerald, K., & Marien, M. (1994).
Meta-analysis of the associations
between social support and health
outcomes.
Annals of Behavioral
Medicine
,16, 352 –362.
Smith, D. (1982). Trends in counseling
and psychotherapy.
American
Psychologist
,37, 802 –809.
Smith, R. E. (1989). Effects of coping
skills training on generalized self-
efficacy and locus of control.REFERENCES 495 |
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,56, 228 –233.
Smith , S. L., & Donnerstein, E. (1998).
Harmful effects of exposure to me-
dia violence: Learning of aggres-sion, emotional desensitization, and
fear. In R. G. Geen &
E. Donnerstein (Eds.),
Human
aggression:
Theories, research, and
implications for social policy
(pp. 167 –202). San Diego:
Academic Press.
Smith , T. W. (2006). Personality as risk
and resilience in physical health.
Current Directions in PsychologicalScience
,15, 227 –231.
Smith , T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., &
Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility,
anger, aggressiveness, and coronary
heart disease: An interpersonal
perspective on personality, emo-tion, and health.
Journal of
Personality
,72, 1217 –1270.
Smyth , J., True, N., & Souto, J. (2001).
Effects of writing about traumatic
experiences: The necessity for nar-
rative structuring.
Journal of Social
and
Clinical Psychology
,20,
161–172.
Snyder ,
C. R. (1988). From defenses to
self-protection: An evolutionary
perspective.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,6, 155 –158.
Solano , C. H., Batten, P. G., & Parish,
E. A. (1982). Loneliness and pat-
terns of self-disclosure.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
43,
524–531.
Solano , C. H., & Koester, N. H. (1989).
Loneliness and communicationproblems: Subjective anxiety orobjective skills?
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,15,
126–133.
Soldz ,
S., & Vaillant, G. E. (1999). The
Big Five personality traits and the
life course: A 45-year longitudinal
study.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,33, 208 –232.
Solomon , Z., Mikulincer, M., &
Avitzur, E. (1988). Coping, locus ofcontrol, social support, andcombat-related posttraumatic stressdisorder: A prospective study.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,55, 279 –285.
Solomon , Z., Weisenberg, M.,
Schwarzwald, J., & Mikulincer, M.
(1988). Combat stress reaction and
posttraumatic stress disorder as de-
terminants of perceived self-efficacyin battle.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,6, 356 –370.Spangler , W. D., & House, R. J. (1991).
Presidential effectiveness and the
leadership motive profile.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,60, 439 –455.
Spanos , N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive
approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn
& J. W. Rhue (Eds.),
Theories of
hypnosis:
Current models and
perspectives
(pp. 324 –361). New
York: Guilford.
Spanos , N. P., & Hewitt, E. C. (1980).
The hidden observer in hypnoticanalgesia: Discovery or experimen-
tal creation?
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,39,
1201 –1214.
Spanos ,
N. P., & Katsanis, J. (1989).
Effects of instructional set on attri-
butions of nonvolition during hyp-
notic and nonhypnotic analgesia.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,56, 182 –188.
Spanos , N. P., Liddy, S. J., Baxter, C. E.,
& Burgess, C. A. (1994). Long-term
and short-term stability of behav-
ioral and subjective indexes of
hypnotizability.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,28,
301–313.
Spanos ,
N. P., Radtke, H. L., &
Dubreuil, D. L. (1982). Episodicand semantic memory in posthyp-notic amnesia: A reevaluation.
Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology
,43, 565 –573.
Spector , P. E. (1982). Behavior in
organizations as a function of
employee ’s locus of control.
Psychological Bulletin
,91,4 8 2 –497.
Spector , P. E., Cooper, C. L., Sanchez,
J. L., O’ Driscoll, M., Sparks, K.,
Bernin, P., et al. (2001). Do na-
tional levels of individualism and
internal locus of control relate towell-being: An ecological level
international study.
Journal of
Organizational
Behavior
,22,
815–832.
Spence ,
J. T. (1993). Gender-related
traits and gender ideology:
Evidence for a multifactorial the-
ory.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,64, 624 –635.
Spence , J. T., & Helmreich, R. L.
(1983). Achievement-related mo-tives and behaviors. In J. T. Spence(Ed.),
Achievement and achieve-
ment
motives: Psychological and
sociological approaches
(pp. 7– 74).
San
Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Spence , J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp,
J. (1974). The Personal AttributesQuestionnaire: A measure of
sex-role stereotypes and
masculinity-femininity.
JSAS
Catalog
of Selected Documents in
Psychology
,4, 127 (Ms. No. 617).
Spencer , S. M., & Norem, J. K. (1996).
Reflection and distraction:
Defensive pessimism, strategicoptimism, and performance.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,22, 354 –365.
Spett, M. C. (1983). All psychologists
are not members of Divisions 12
and 17.
American Psychologist
,38,
498.
Spiegel ,
D. (2005). Multileveling the
playing field: Altering our state of
consciousness to understand hyp-
nosis.
Contemporary Hypnosis
,22,
31–33.
Sprecher,
S., & Hendrick, S. S. (2004).
Self-disclosure in intimate relation-ships: Associations with individual
and relationship characteristicsover time.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,23, 857 –877.
Sprecher, S., Sullivan, Q., & Hatfield, E.
(1994). Mate selection preferences:Gender differences examined in anational sample.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
66,
1074 –1080.
Srivastava , S., Angelo, K. M., &
Vallereux, S. R. (2009).
Extraversion and positive affect: A
day reconstruction study of person-environment transactions.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,42,
1613 –1618.
Staats ,
A. W. (1996).
Behaviorism and
personality:
Psychological behav-
iorism
. New York: Springer.
Stajkovic ,
A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998).
Self-efficacy and work-relatedperformance: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin
,124,
240–261.
Stake ,
J. E. (2000). When situations call
for instrumentality andexpressive-
ness: Resource appraisal, coping
strategy choice, and adjustment.
Sex Roles
,42, 865 –885.
Stake , J. E., Huff, L., & Zand, D.
(1995). Trait self-esteem, positiveand negative events, and event-
specific shifts in self-evaluation and
affect.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,29, 223 –241.
Stake , J. E., & Nickens, S. D. (2005).
Adolescent girls ’and boys ’science
peer relationships and perceptionsof the possible self as scientist.
Sex
Roles
,52,1–11.496 REFERENCES |
Stanton , A. L., Kirk, S. B., Cameron,
C. L., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2000).
Coping through emotional ap-
proach: Scale construction andvalidation.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,78,
1150 –1169.
Steel,
P., & Ones, D. S. (2002).
Personality and happiness: Anational-level analysis.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
83,
767–781.
Steger , M. F., Kawabata, Y., Shimai, S.,
& Otake, K. (2008). The meaning-
ful life in Japan and the United
States: Levels and correlates ofmeaning in life.
Journal of Research
in
Personality
,42, 660 –678.
Stein, G. L., Kimiecik, J. C., Daniels, J.,
& Jackson, S. A. (1995).Psychological antecedents of flowin recreational sports.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,21,
125–135.
Stelmack ,
R. M. (1990). Biological bases
of extraversion: Psychophysiologi-
cal evidence.
Journal of Personality
,
58,
293–311.
Stelmack , R. M., & Pivik, R. T. (1996).
Extraversion and the effects ofexercise on spinal motoneuronal
excitability.
Personality and
Individual
Differences
,21,6 9 –76.
Stephenson , W. (1953).
The study of
behavior:
Q-technique and its
methodology
. Chicago: University
of
Chicago Press.
Steptoe , A., O’ Donnell, K., Marmot, M.,
& Wardle, J. (2008). Positive affect
and psychosocial processes related
to health.
British Journal of
Psychology
,99, 211 –227.
Steptoe , A., & Wardle, J. (2001). Locus
of control and health behaviourrevisited: A multivariate analysis ofyoung adults from 18 countries.
British Journal of Psychology
,92,
659–672.
Sternberg ,
R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., &
Kidd, K. K. (2005). Intelligence,
race, and genetics.
American
Psychologist
,60,4 6 –59.
Stewart , A. J. (1982).
Motivation and
society
. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Stoolmiller ,
M. (1999). Implications of
the restricted range of family en-
vironments for estimates of herita-bility and nonshared environment
in behavior-genetic adoption stud-
ies.
Psychological Bulletin
,125,
392–409.
Storr ,
A. (1988).
Solitude: A return to
the
self
. New York: Free Press.Story ,
A. L. (1998). Self-esteem and
memory for favorable and unfa-
vorable personality feedback.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,24,5 1 –64.
Strahan , E. J., Lafrance, A., Wilson,
A. E., Ethier, N., Spencer, S. J., &
Zanna, M. (2008). Victoria ’s dirty
secret: How sociocultural normsinfluence adolescent girls and
women.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,34, 288 –301.
Strauman , T. J. (1996). Stability within
the self: A longitudinal study of thestructural implications of self-
discrepancy theory.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
71,
1142 –1153.
Strelau , J. (1987). Emotion as a key
concept in temperament research.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
21,
510–528.
Strentz , T., & Auerbach, S. M. (1988).
Adjustment to the stress of simu-
lated captivity: Effects of emotion-focused versus problem-focused
preparation on hostages differing in
locus of control.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
55,
652–660.
Strickland , B. R. (1978). Internal-
external expectancies and health-related behaviors.
Journal of
Consulting
and Clinical
Psychology
,46, 1192 –1211.
Strickland , B. R. (1989). Internal-
external control expectancies: From
contingency to creativity.
American
Psychologist
,44,1–12.
Stright ,
A. D., Gallagher, K. C., &
Kelley, K. (2008). Infant tempera-ment moderates relations between
maternal parenting in early child-
hood and children ’s adjustment in
first grade.
Child Development
,79,
186–200.
Strough ,
J., Leszczynski, J. P., Neely,
T. L., Flinn, J. A., & Margrett, J.(2007). From adolescence to lateradulthood: Femininity, masculinity,
and androgyny in six age groups.
Sex Roles
,57, 385 –396.
Strube , M. J. (1982). Time urgency and
Type A behavior: A methodologicalnote.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,8, 563 –565.
Strube , M. J., Berry, J. M., &
Moergen, S. (1985).Relinquishment of control and the
Type A behavior pattern: The role
of performance evaluation.
Journal
of
Personality and Social
Psychology
,49, 831 –842.Sturmer , T., & Hasselbach, P. (2006).
Personality, lifestyle, and risk of
cardiovascular disease and cancer:
Follow-up of population basedcohort.
British Medical Journal
,
332,
1359.
Suedfeld , P. (1980).
Restricted environ-
mental
stimulation: Research and
clinical applications
. New York:
Wiley.
Suedfeld ,
P. (1982). Aloneness as a
healing experience. In L. A. Peplau
& D. Perlman (Eds.),
Loneliness:
A
sourcebook of current theory,
research and therapy
(pp. 54 –67).
New
York: Wiley.
Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., &
Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting
basis of life satisfaction judgments
across cultures: Emotions versusnorms.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,74, 482 –493.
Suinn , R. M. (2001). The terrible twos —
anger and anxiety: Hazardous toyour health.
American
Psychologist
,56,2 7 –36.
Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The
relative efficacy of avoidant andnonavoidant coping strategies: Ameta-analysis.
Health Psychology
,
4,
249–288.
Surtees , P. G., Wainwright, N. W. J.,
Luben, R., Khaw, K-T., & Day,N. E. (2006). Mastery, sense of
coherence, and mortality: Evidence
of independent associations fromthe EPIC-Norfolk prospectivecohort study.
Health Psychology
,
25,
102–110.
Suvak , M. K., Vogt, D. S., Savarese,
V. W., King, L. A., & King, D. W.(2002). Relationship of war-zone
coping strategies to long-term general
life adjustment among Vietnam ve-terans: Combat exposure as a mod-eratorvariable.
PersonalityandSocial
Ps
ychology Bulletin
,28,9 7 4 –985.
Swann , W. B., & Seyle, C. (2005).
Personality psychology ’s comeback
and its emerging symbiosis with
social psychology.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,31,
155–165.
Swickert ,
R. J., & Gilliland, K. (1998).
Relationship between the brainstem
auditory evoked response and
extraversion, impulsivity, andsociability.
Journal of Research in
Personality
,32, 314 –330.
Tafarodi , R. W., Marshall, T. C., &
Katsura, H. (2004). Standing out in
Canada and Japan.
Journal of
Personality
,72, 785 –814.REFERENCES 497 |
Tafarodi , R. W., & Vu, C. (1997).
Two-dimensional self-esteem and
reactions to success and failure.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,23, 626 –635.
Tamir , M. (2009). Differential prefer-
ences for happiness: Extraversionand trait-consistent emotion regu-lation.
Journal of Personality
,77,
447–470.
Tamres ,
L. K., Janicki, D., & Helgeson,
V. S. (2002). Sex differences incoping behavior: A meta-analyticreview and an examination of rela-
tive coping.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Review
,6,2–30.
Tarabulsky ,
G. M., Provost, M. A.,
Larose, S., Moss, E., Lemelin, J-P,Moran, G., et al. (2008).
Similarities and differences in
mothers ’and observers’ ratings of
infant security on the Attachment
Q-Sort.
Infant Behavior &
Development
,31,1 0 –22.
Tarlow , E. M., & Haaga, D. A. F.
(1996). Negative self-concept:Specificity to depressive symptoms
and relation to positive and nega-
tive affectivity.
Journal of Research
in
Personality
,30, 120 –127.
Taubes , G. (1998, May –June). Telling
time by the second hand.
Technology Review
,101, 76.
Taylor , D. A., & Belgrave, F. Z. (1986).
The effects of perceived intimacyand valence on self-disclosure reci-procity.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,12, 247 –255.
Taylor , M. C., & Hall, J. A. (1982).
Psychological androgyny: Theories,methods and conclusions.
Psycho-
logical
Bulletin
,92, 347 –366.
Taylor , S. E. (1989).
Positive illusions:
Creative
self-deception and the
healthy mind
. New York: Basic
Books.
Tellegen ,
A., & Atkinson, G. (1974).
Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ( “absorption ”),
a trait related to hypnotic
susceptibility.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,83, 268 –277.
Terracciano , A., Costa, P. T., &
McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personalityplasticity after age 30.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Bulletin
,32,
999–1009.
Terracciano ,
A., McCrae, R. R.,
Hagemann, D., & Costa, P. T.
(2003). Individual difference vari-
ables, affective differentiation, andthe structure of affect.
Journal of
Personality
,71, 669 –703.Terry , D. J., & Hynes, G. J. (1998).
Adjustment to a low-control situa-
tion: Reexamining the role of
coping responses.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
74,
1078 –1092.
Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein,
M. (1991). Personality measures as
predictors of job performance: Ameta-analytic review.
Personnel
Psychology
,44, 703 –739.
Thibodeau , R., Jorgensen, R. S., & Kim,
S. (2006). Depression, anxiety, and
resting frontal EEG asymmetry: A
meta-analytic review.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,115,
715–729.
Thomas,
A., & Chess, S. (1977).
Temperament and development
.
New
York: Brunner/Mazel.
Thorndike , E. L. (1911).
Animal intelli-
gence:
Experimental studies
. New
York:
Macmillan.
Thrash , T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002).
Implicit and self-attributed achieve-
ment motives: Concordance and
predictive validity.
Journal of
Personality
,70, 729 –755.
Thrash , T. M., Elliot, A. J., &
Schultheiss, O. C. (2007).Methodological and dispositional
predictors of congruence between
implicit and explicit need forAchievement.
Personality and Social
Ps
ychology Bulletin
,33,9 6 1 –974.
Tidwell , M-C. O., Reis, H. T., &
Shaver, P. R. (1996). Attachment,
attractiveness, and social interac-
tion: A diary study.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
71,
729–745.
Timmers , M., Fischer, A. H., &
Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Gender
differences in motives for regulating
emotions.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,24, 974 –985.
Todorov , A., & Bargh, J. A. (2002).
Automatic sources of aggression.
Aggression and Violent Behavior
,
7,5
3–68.
Torges , C. M., Stewart, A. J., &
Duncan, L. E. (2009). Appreciatinglife’s complexities: Assessing narra-
tive ego integrity in late midlife.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
43,6
6–74.
Tracy , J. L., Robins, R. W., & Sherman,
J. W. (2009). The practice of psy-
chological science: Searching for
Cronbach ’s two streams in social-
personality psychology.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
96,
1206 –1225.Triandis , H. C. (1989). The self and
social behavior in differing cultural
contexts.
Psychological Review
,96,
506–520.
Triandis ,
H. C. (2001). Individualism-
collectivism and personality.
Journal of Personality
,69,
907–924.
Tripathi ,
R., & Cervone, D. (2008).
Cultural variations in achievement
motivation despite equivalent mo-
tivational strength: Motivationalconcerns among Indian andAmerican corporate professionals.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
42,
456–464.
Trivers , R. L. (1972). Parental invest-
ment and sexual selection. In
B. Campbell (Ed.),
Sexual selection
and
the descent of man: 1871 –1971
(pp. 136 –179). Chicago: Aldine.
Trull , T. J., Widiger, T. A., & Burr, R.
(2001). A structured interview for
the assessment of the five-factormodel of personality: Facet-levelrelations to the Axis II personality
disorder.
Journal of Personality
,69,
175–198.
Tucker ,
J. S., & Anders, S. L. (1999).
Attachment style, interpersonalperception accuracy, and relation-
ship satisfaction in dating couples.
Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin
,15, 403 –412.
Tuerlinckx , F., De Boeck, P., & Lens,
W. (2002). Measuring needs withthe Thematic Apperception Test:A psychometric study.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
82,
448–461.
Twenge , J. M. (2000). The age of
anxiety? Birth cohort change in
anxiety and neuroticism, 1952 –
1993.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,79, 1007 –1021.
Uchino , B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., &
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The
relationship between social supportand physiological processes: Areview with emphasis on underlying
mechanisms and implications for
health.
Psychological Bulletin
,119,
488–531.
Ungerer ,
J. A., Waters, B., & Barnett, B.
(1997). Defense style and adjust-
ment in interpersonal relationships.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
31,
375–384.
Urbina , S. P., & Grey, A. (1975).
Cultural and sex differences in the
sex distribution of dream charac-ters.
Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology
,6, 358 –364.498 REFERENCES |
Vaidya, J. G., Gray, E. K., Haig, J., &
Watson, D. (2002). On the tempo-
ral stability of personality: Evidence
for differential stability and the roleof life experiences.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
83,
1469 –1484.
Vaillant , G. E. (1977).
Adaptation to
life
. Boston: Little, Brown.
Vaillant ,
G. E. (1992).
Ego mechanisms
of
defense: A guide for clinicians
and researchers
. Washington, DC:
American
Psychiatric Press.
Valentiner , D. P., Foa, E. B., Riggs,
D. S., & Gershuny, B. S. (1996).
Coping strategies and posttrau-
matic stress disorder in femalevictims of sexual and nonsexual
assault.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,105, 455 –458.
Valli, K., Revonsuo, A., Palkas, O., &
Punamaki, R-L. (2006). The effectof trauma on dream content— A
field study of Palestinian children.
Dreaming
,16,6 3 –87.
van Ijendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M.
J. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg,
M. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M.(2004). Assessing attachmentsecurity with the Attachment
Q-Sort: Meta-analytic evidence
for the validity of the observerAQS.
Child Development
,75,
1188 –1213.
Van
Yperen , N. W. (2006). A novel
approach to assessing achievementgoals in the context of the 2 X 2framework: Identifying distinct
profiles and individuals with dif-
ferent dominant achievement goals.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,32, 1432 –1445.
Vasquez , E. A., Denson, T. F., Pedersen,
W. C., Stenstrom, D. M., & Miller,
N. (2005). The moderating effect oftrigger intensity on triggered dis-
placed aggression.
Journal of
Experimental
Social Psychology
,
41,6
1–67.
Vaughn , L. A., & Weary, G. (2002).
Roles of the availability of expla-
nations, feelings of ease, and
dysphoria in judgments about thefuture.
Journal of Social and
Clinical
Psychology
,21, 686 –704.
Veroff , J., Depner, C., Kulka, R., &
Douvan, E. (1980). Comparison of
American motives: 1957 versus
1976.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,39,
1249 –1262.
Verona ,
E., & Sullivan, E. A. (2008).
Emotional catharsis and aggressionrevisited: Heart rate reduction
following aggressive responding.
Emotion
,8, 331 –340.
Viglione , D. J. (1999). A review of recent
research addressing the utility of
the Rorschach.
Psychological
Assessment
,11, 251 –265.
Viglione , D. J., & Hilsenroth, M. J.
(2001). The Rorschach: Facts,fictions, and future.
Psychological
Assessment
,13, 452 –471.
Vignoles , V. L., Manzi, C., Regalia, C.,
Jemmolo, S., & Scabini, E. (2008).Identity motives underlying desired
and feared possible future selves.
Journal of Personality
,76,
1165 –1200.
Vitaliano ,
P. P., DeWolfe, D. J., Maiuro,
R. D., Russo, J., & Katon, W.
(1990). Appraised changeability
of a stressor as a modifier ofthe relationship between coping
and depression: A test of the
hypothesis of fit.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
59,
582–592.
Vitkus, J., & Horowitz, L. M. (1987).
Poor social performance of lonely
people: Lacking a skill or adoptinga role?
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,52,
1266 –1273.
Vittengl ,
J. R., Clark, L. A., Dunn,
T. W., & Jarrett, R. B. (2007).
Reducing relapse and recurrence in
unipolar depression: A comparativemeta-analysis of cognitive-behavioral therapy ’s effects.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology
,75, 475 –488.
Vo, K., & Ostrov, B. F. (2006, February
19). Women seek reliable health
advice in vain: Studies keep con-
tradicting the conventional wisdom.
San Jose Mercury News
, pp. 1A,
15A.
Todorov ,
A., & Bargh, J. A. (2002).
Automatic sources of aggression.
Aggression and Violent Behavior
,
7,5
3–68.
Voelz , C. J. (1985). Effects of gender
role disparity on couples ’decision-
making processes.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
49,
1532 –1540.
Vogel , D. A., Lake, M. A., Evans, S., &
Karraker, K. H. (1991). Children ’s
and adults ’sex-stereotyped percep-
tions of infants.
Sex Roles
,24,
605–616.
Vogel ,
G. W. (1975). Review of REM
sleep deprivation.
Archives of
General
Psychiatry
,32, 749 –761.Wacker , J., Chavanon, M., Leue, A., &
Stemmler, G. (2008). Is running
away right? The behavioral
activation-behavioral inhibitionmodel of anterior asymmetry.
Emotion
,8, 232 –249.
Wallston , K. A. (1992). Hocus-pocus,
the focus isn ’t strictly on locus:
Rotter ’s social learning theory
modified for health.
Cognitive
Therapy
and Research
,16,
183–199.
Wallston ,
K. A. (Ed.). (2005). Research
with the Multidimensional Health
Locus of Control (MHLC) Scales
[Special issue].
Journal of Health
Psychology
,10(5).
Wallston ,
K. A., Maides, S., &
Wallston, B. S. (1976). Health-related information seeking as afunction of health-related locus ofcontrol and health value.
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,10,
215–222.
Wallston ,
K. A., & Smith, M. S. (1994).
Issues of control and health: The
action is in the interaction. In
G. N. Penney, P. Bennett, &M. Herbert (Eds.),
Health psychol-
ogy:
A lifespan perspective
(pp. 153 –168). London, England:
Harwood.
Ward , C. H., & Eisler, R. M. (1987).
Type A behavior, achievement
striving, and a dysfunctional self-
evaluation system.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
53,
318–326.
Watkins , C. E., Campbell, V. L.,
Nieberding, R., & Hallmark, R.(1995). Contemporary practice ofpsychological assessment by clinical
psychologists.
Professional
Psychology:
Research and Practice
,
26,5
4–60.
Watson , D. (1988). Intraindividual
and interindividual analyses of
positive and negative affect:
Their relation to healthcomplaints, perceived stress, and
daily activities.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
54,
1020 –1030.
Watson , D., & Clark, L. A. (1991).
Self- versus peer-ratings of specific
emotional traits: Evidence of con-
vergent and discriminant validity.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,60, 927 –940.
Watson , D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G.
(1988). Positive and negative
affectivity and their relation to
anxiety and depressive disorders.REFERENCES 499 |
Journal of Abnormal Psychology ,
97,
346–353.
Watson , D., Clark, L. A., McIntyre,
C. W., & Hamaker, S. (1992).
Affect, personality, and socialactivity.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,63,
1011 –1025.
Watson ,
D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A.
(1988). Development and valida-
tion of brief measures of positive
and negative affect: The PANASScales.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,54, 1063 –1070.
Watson , D., Hubbard, B., & Wiese, D.
(2000). Self-other agreement inpersonality and affectivity: The roleof acquaintanceship, trait visibility,
and assumed similarity.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
78,
546–558.
Watson , D., & Naragon, K. (2009).
Positive affectivity: The disposition
to experience positive emotional
states. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder(Eds.),
Oxford handbook of positive
ps
ychology
(2nd ed., pp. 207 –21 5).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Watson , D., & Pennebaker, J. W.
(1989). Health complaints, stress,
and distress: Exploring the central
role of negative affectivity.
Psychological Review
,96,
234–254.
Watson ,
D., & Tellegen, A. (1985).
Toward a consensual structure ofmood.
Psychological Bulletin
,98,
219–235.
Watson ,
J. B. (1936). John Broadus
Watson. In C. Murchison (Ed.),
A
history
of psychology in autobiog-
raphy
(Vol. 3, pp. 271 –281).
Worcester, MA: Clark UniversityPress.
Watson , J. B. (1924/1970).
Behaviorism
. New York: Norton.
Watson ,
J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920).
Conditioned emotional reactions.
Journal of ExperimentalPsychology
,3,1–14.
Webb ,
S. D. (1978). Privacy and psy-
chosomatic stress: An empiricalanalysis.
Social Behavior and
Personality
,6, 227 –234.
Weeks , D. G., Michela, J. L., Peplau,
L. A., & Bragg, M. E. (1980).Relation between loneliness anddepression: A structural equation
analysis.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,39, 1238 –1244.
Wegner , D. M., Wenzlaff, R. M., &
Kozak, M. (2004). Dream rebound:The return of suppressed thoughtsin dreams.
Psychological Science
,
15,
232–236.
Wei, M., Russell, D. W., & Zakalik,
R. A. (2005). Adult attachment,
social self-efficacy, self-disclosure,loneliness, and subsequent depres-
sion for freshman college students:
A longitudinal study.
Journal of
Counseling
Psychology
,52,
602–614.
Weiner ,
B. (1985). An attributional
theory of achievement motivationand emotion.
Psychological
Bulletin
,92, 548 –573.
Weiner , B. (1990). Attribution in
personality psychology. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.),
Handbook of
personality:
Theory and research
(pp. 465 –485). New York:
Guilford.
Weiner , B. (2006).
Social motivation,
justice,
and the moral emotions: An
attributional approach
. Mahwah,
NJ:
Erlbaum.
Weiner , I. B. (1995). Methodological
considerations in Rorschach re-
search.
Psychological Assessment
,
7,
330–337.
Weiner , I. B. (1996). Some observations
on the validity of the Rorschachinkblot method.
Psychological
Assessment
,8, 206 –213.
Weiner , I. B. (2001). Advancing the sci-
ence of psychological assessment:The Rorschach inkblot method as
exemplar.
Psychological
Assessment
,13, 423 –432.
Weinstein , S. E., & Quigley, K. S.
(2006). Locus of control predicts
appraisals and cardiovascularreactivity to a novel active copingtask.
Journal of Personality
,74,
911–931.
Wenzel ,
A., & Emerson, T. (2009).
Mate selection in socially anxiousand nonanxious individuals.
Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology
,28, 341 –363.
Westen , D. (1996). A model and a
method for uncovering the nomo-
thetic from the idiographic: An al-
ternative to the five-factor model?
Journal of Research in Personality
,
30,
400–413.
Westen , D. (1998). The scientific legacy
of Sigmund Freud: Toward a
psychodynamically informed psy-chological science.
Psychological
Bulletin
,124, 333 –371.
Wheeler , R. E., Davidson, R. J., &
Tomarken, A. J. (1993). Frontal
brain asymmetry and emotional
reactivity: A biological substrate ofaffective style.
Psychophysiology
,
30,8
2–89.
Whiting , R. (1989).
You gotta have Wa
.
New
York: Vintage.
Whitley , B. E. (1983). Sex-role orienta-
tion and self-esteem: A criticalmeta-analytic review.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
44,
765–778.
Whitley , D. (2002, March 31). Former
major leaguer confronts painful
past.
San Jose Mercury News
,
p.
16D.
Whyte , L. L. (1978).
The unconscious
before
Freud
. New York: St.
Martin ’s.
Wichman ,
A. L., Rodgers, J. L., &
MacCallum, R. C. (2006). A mul-
tilevel approach to the relationship
between birth order and intelli-gence.
Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin
,32, 117 –127.
Wicker , F. W., Barron, W. L., & Willis,
A. C. (1980). Disparagementhumor: Dispositions and resolu-tions.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,39, 701 –709.
Widiger , T. A. (2005). Five factor model
of personality disorder: Integrating
science and practice.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,39,
67–83.
Wilkowski ,
B. M., & Robinson, M. D.
(2008). The cognitive basis of trait
anger and reactive aggression: An
integrative analysis.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Review
,12,
3–21.
Williams ,
J. E., Nieto, F. J., Sanford,
C. P., Couper, D. J., & Tyroler,H. A. (2002). The association be-tween trait anger and incident
stroke risk: The Atherosclerosis
Risk in Communities (ARIC) study.
Stroke
,33,1 3 –20.
Williams , J. E., Nieto, F. J., Sanford,
C. P., & Tyroler, H. A. (2001).Effects of an angry temperament oncoronary heart disease risk: TheAtherosclerosis Risk in
Communities study.
American
Journal
of Epidemiology
,154,
230–235.
Williams ,
J. E., Paton, C. C., Seigler,
I. C., Eigenbrodt, M. L., Nieto,
F. J., & Tyroler, H. A. (2000).
Anger proneness predicts coronaryheart disease risk: Prospective
analysis from the Atherosclerosis
Risk in Communities (ARIC) study.
Circulation
,101, 2034 –2039.
Williams , J. G., & Solano, C. H. (1983).
The social reality of feeling lonely:500 REFERENCES |
Friendship and reciprocation.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,9, 237 –242.
Williams , S. L. (1995). Self-efficacy and
anxiety and phobic disorders. In
J. E. Maddux (Ed.),
Self-efficacy,
adaptation,
and adjustment:
Theory, research, and application
(pp. 69 –108). New York: Plenum.
Williams , P. G., Colder, C. R., Lane,
J. D., McCaskill, C. C., Feinglos,
M. N., & Surwit, R. S. (2002).Examination of the neuroticism-symptom reporting relationship in
individuals with Type 2 diabetes.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin
,28, 1015 –1025.
Williams-Piehota , P., Schneider, T. R.,
Pizarro, J., Mowad, L., & Salovey,P. (2004). Matching health mes-sages to health locus of controlbeliefs for promoting mammogra-
phy utilization.
Psychology and
Health
,19, 407 –423.
Wilson , T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1982).
Improving the academic perfor-mance of college freshmen:
Attribution therapy revisited.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
,42, 367 –376.
Wilson , T. D., & Linville, P. W. (1985).
Improving the performance ofcollege freshmen with attributionaltechniques.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,49,
287–293.
Windle ,
M., & Windle, R. C. (1996).
Coping strategies, drinking
motives, and stressful life events
among middle adolescents:Associations with emotional andbehavioral problems and with
academic functioning.
Journal of
Abnormal
Psychology
,105,
551–560.
Winter ,
D. A. (2003). Repertory grid
technique as a psychotherapy re-
search measure.
Psychotherapy
Research
,13,2 5 –42.
Wittenberg , M. T., & Reis, H. T.
(1986). Loneliness, social skills, and
social perception.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,12,
121–130.
Woike ,
B. A. (2008). A functional
framework for the influence of im-plicit and explicit motives on auto-biographical memory.
Personality
and
Social Psychology Review
,12,
99–117.
Wolfe ,
R. N., & Kasmer, J. A. (1988).
Type versus trait: Extraversion,
impulsivity, sociability, andpreferences for cooperative and
competitive activities.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
54,
864–871.
Won-Doornink , M. J. (1985). Self-
disclosure and reciprocity in con-
versation: A cross-national study.
Social Psychology Quarterly
,48,
97–107.
Wong ,
M. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M.
(1991). Motivation and academic
achievement: The effects of person-
ality traits and the quality ofexperience.
Journal of Personality
,
59,
539–574.
Wood , J. M., Garb, H. N., Lilienfeld,
S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2002).Clinical assessment.
Annual Review
of
Psychology
,53, 519 –543.
Wood , J. M., Nezworski, M. T., &
Stejskal, W. J. (1996). Thinking
critically about the Comprehensive
System for the Rorschach: A reply
to Exner.
Psychological Science
,7,
14–17.
Wood ,
J. M., Nezworski, M. T., &
Stejskal, W. J. (1997). The reliabil-
ity of the Comprehensive System
for the Rorschach: A comment onMeyer (1997).
Psychological
Assessment
,9, 490 –494.
Wood , J. V., Heimpel, S. A., Manwell,
L. A., & Whittington, E. J. (2009).
This mood is familiar and I don ’t
deserve to feel better anyway:
Mechanisms underlying self-esteemdifferences in motivation to repairsad moods.
Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology
,96,
363–380.
Wood ,
J. V., Heimpel, S. A., Newby-
Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (2005).
Snatching defeat from the jaws of
victory: Self-esteem differences inthe experience and anticipation ofsuccess.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,89,
764–780.
Wood ,
W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002).
A cross-cultural analysis of the
behavior of women and men:
Implications for the origins of sexdifferences.
Psychological Bulletin
,
128,
699–727.
Wood , W., Wong, F. Y., & Chachere,
J. G. (1991). Effects of media vio-lence on viewers ’aggression in
unconstrained social interaction.
Psychological Bulletin
,109,
371–383.
Woodhill,
B. M., & Samuels, C. A.
(2004). Desirable and undesirable
androgyny: A prescription for thetwenty-first century.
Journal of
Gender
Studies
,13,1 5 –28.
Woodward , S. A., McManis, M. H.,
Kagan, J., Deldin, P., Snidman, N.,
Lewis, M., et al. (2001). Infanttemperament and the brainstem
auditory evoked response in later
childhood.
Developmental
Psychology
,37, 533 –538.
Woody , E. Z., & Sadler, P. (2008).
Dissociation theories of hypnosis.In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier(Eds.),
The Oxford handbook of
hypnosis:
Theory, research and
practice
(pp. 81 –110). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Wrosch , C., & Miller, G. E. (2009).
Depressive symptoms can be useful:
Self-regulatory and emotional ben-
efits of dysphoric mood in adoles-cence.
Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology
,96,
1181 –1190.
Wrosch ,
C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen,
J. (2002). Health stresses and de-pressive symptomatology in the
elderly: The importance of health
engagement control strategies.
Health Psychology
,21,
340–348.
Wu,
K. D., & Clark, L. A. (2003).
Relations between personality traitsand self-reports of daily behavior.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
37,
231–256.
Wyatt , E. (2005, February 24). A
Mormon daughter ’s book stirs a
storm.
New York Times
.
Yarnold,
P. R., Mueser, K. T., &
Grimm, L. G. (1985). Interpersonaldominance of Type As in groupdiscussions.
Journal of Abnormal
Psychology
,94, 233 –236.
Young , J. E. (1982). Loneliness, depres-
sion and cognitive therapy: Theory
and application. In L. A. Peplau &
D. Perlman (Eds.),
Loneliness
(pp. 379 –405). New York: Wiley.
Zadra , A., Desjardins, S., & Marcotte,
E. (2006). Evolutionary function ofdreams: A test of the threat simu-
lation theory in recurrent dreams.
Consciousness and Cognition
,15,
450–463.
Zadra ,
A. L., O ’Brien, S. A., & Donderi,
D. C. (1998). Dream content,
dream recurrence and well-being:A replication with a younger
sample.
Imagination, Cognition
and
Personality
,17,
293–311.
Zajonc ,
R. B. (2001). The family
dynamics of intellectualREFERENCES 501 |
development. American
Psychologist
,56, 490 –496.
Zajonc , R. B., & Sulloway, F. J. (2007).
The confluence model: Birth order
as a within-family or between-family dynamic?
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,33,
1187 –1194.
Zammichieli ,
M. E., Gilroy, F. D., &
Sherman, M. F. (1988). Relations
between sex-role orientation and
marital satisfaction.
Personality and
Social
Psychology Bulletin
,14,
747–754.
Ze
idner , M. (2007). Anxiety and coping
with community disasters: The Israeliexperience.
Journal of Research in
Per
sonality
,41,2 1 3 –220.
Zeidner , M., & Hammer, A. L. (1992).
Coping with missile attack:Resources, strategies, and out-comes.
Journal of Personality
,60,
709–746.
Zeigler-Hill ,
V., Chadha, S., &
Osterman, L. (2008). Psychologicaldefense and self-esteem instability:
Is defense style associated with
unstable self-esteem?
Journal
of
Research in Personality
,42,
348–364.
Zernike ,
K. (2000, August 25).
Academic race gap grows again.
San Jose Mercury News
, p. 21A.
Zezima ,
K., & Carey, B. (2009,
September 11). Ex-priest challenges
abuse conviction on repressed
memories.
New York Times
,
p.
A13.
Zhou, X., Saucier, G., Gao, D., & Liu, J.
(2009). The factor structure ofChinese personality traits.
Journal
of
Personality
,77, 363 –400.
Zillmann , D. (1979).
Hostility and
aggression
. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zillmann ,
D., Bryant, J., & Cantor, J. R.
(1974). Brutality of assault inpolitical cartoons affecting humor
appreciation.
Journal of
Research
in Personality
,7,
334–345.Zimbardo,
P. G. (1986). The Stanford
Shyness Project. In W. H. Jones,
J. M. Cheek, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.),
Shyness: Perspectives on research
and
treatment
(pp. 17 –25).
New
York: Plenum.
Zuckerman , M., & Gagne, M. (2003).
The COPE revised: Proposing a
5-factor model of coping strategies.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
37,
169–204.
Zuckerman , M., Kuhlman, D. M.,
Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M.(1993). A comparison of three
structural models for personality:
The Big Three, the Big Five, and thealternative five.
Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology
,
65,
757–768.
Zur, O., Williams, M. H., Lehavot, K.,
& Knapp, S. (2009).Psychotherapist self-disclosure and
transparency in the Internet age.
Professional Psychology: Research
and
Practice
,40,2 2 –30.502 REFERENCES |
NAME INDEX
Abela, J. R. Z., 450
Ablow, J. C., 163Aboud, F., 78
Abramson, L. Y., 424, 446, 449 –450
Accortt, E. E., 246
Adams, N. E., 363Adler, N. E., 129
Affleck, G., 15
Ahadi, S. A., 230 –232
Ainsworth, M. D. S., 140Albino, A., 130, 145
Alden, L. E., 203
Aldwin, C. M., 130Alford, B. A., 445Ali, S., 364
Allard, L. M., 145 –146
Allen, A., 168
Allen, J., 145Allen, J. J. B., 246
Allen, M., 312
Allen, M., 318Alloy, L. B., 446, 450Allport, G. W., 149, 153 –154
Almagor, M., 163Alper, C. M., 320Altman, I., 311, 335Altman, J., 128
Alvarenga, M., 196
Amir, N., 201Amirkhan, J. H., 263
Anderman, E. M., 193
Anderman, L. H., 193
Anders, S. L., 144Andersen, B. L., 419Anderson, C. A., 134, 200, 319,
391, 394 –395, 436 –438, 451Anderson, K. B., 134
Angelo, K. M., 263Angie, A., 262
Angleitner, A., 150, 176, 255
Ansbacher, H. L., 98
Ansbacher, R. R., 98Antill, J. K., 381, 383
Antonen, M., 216
Antoni, M. H., 215Antrobus, J. S., 74Araya, R., 450
Archer, J. A., 316
Archer, J., 248Archibald, F. S., 320Arkin, A. M., 74
Arkin, R. M., 203, 400
Armeli, S., 159Arneson, J. J., 177Arnkoff, D. B., 367
Arnold, M. L., 118
Aron, A., 145Aron, E. N., 145Arroyo, J. A., 130
Arthur, W., 162
Asendorpf, J. B., 161Aserinsky, E., 74Ashton, M. C., 163 –164
Aspinwall, L. G., 214, 216Atherton, S. C., 203Atkins, D. C., 368
Atkinson, G., 91
Atkinson, G. R., 11
Atkinson, J. W., 184Aube, J., 384Auerbach, S. M., 130
Austenfeld, J. L., 130Austin, T., 309
Averill, J. R., 126, 335, 338Aviles, F. E., 136
Avitzur, E., 405
Bach, L., 429
Bacon, P. L., 444
Bagby, R. M., 175
Bailey, J. M., 62, 269Baird, B. M., 262Baker, E. L., 87
Baker, L. A., 259
Baker, S. R., 215Balabanis, M. H., 364Baldwin, A. S., 364
Baldwin, M. W., 202
Baldwin, S. A., 368Bandura, A., 351, 353, 356 –357,
362 –364, 378, 387
Bannister, D., 428Barbaranelli. C., 364Barber, A. M., 240Barber, T. X., 88, 90 –92
Barbo, E., 266Bardos, A. N., 60Bargh, J. A., 438Bar-Haim, Y., 233
Barkin, A., 320
Barlett, C. P., 394Barlow, D. H., 238
Barnes, M., 266, 269
Barnes, R. D., 382
Barnett, B., 80Baron, R. A., 83Barrett, K. C., 142
Barrett, M. S., 309
503 |
Barrett, P., 227
Barrick, M. R., 171 –172
Barron, K. E., 193
Barron, W. L., 83Barry, J. E., 319Bartholomew, K., 142, 320
Bartholow, B. D., 394
Bartlett, M. Y., 272Barton, J. R., 429
Bartussek, D., 246
Basgall, J. A., 405
Bata. I., 197Bates, J. E., 248Batten, P. G., 324
Batty, G. D., 406
Baucom, D. H., 368, 381Bauder, D., 390Bauer, J. J., 364
Baumeister, R. F., 137 –138, 169, 237
Bauminger, N., 312
Baxter, C. E., 91Beall, S. K., 313
Beard, C., 201
Beaubien, J. M., 192Beaulieu-Prevost, D., 91Bechtle, R., 116
Beck, A. T., 58, 400, 425, 445 –446, 450
Becker, D., 386
Beevers, C. G., 450
Belanger, K. A., 84
Belgrave, F. Z., 310
Bell, N. J., 383Bell, R. C., 428Bem, D. J., 168
Bem, S. L., 379, 381
Ben-Ari, A., 210, 212Benassi, V. A., 404Benet-Martinez, V., 10, 163
Bengston, P. L., 298
Benham, G., 92Ben Hamida, S., 269, 272Benjamin, A. J., 7, 195, 394
Bennett, P., 407 –408
Bera, S., 144
Bereson, K. R.,Berg, J. H., 324
Berkman, L. F., 320
Berkow, I., 317Berkowitz, L., 83, 133 –134, 438
Berman, J. S., 309
Berman, M. E., 7
Berman, M. I., 272Bernston, G. G., 320 –321
Berry, D. S., 206
Berry, J. D., 321
Berry, J. M., 195Bettencourt, B. A., 7, 195, 407
Beyer, J., 363
Bianchi, E. C., 116
Biederman, J., 235Billings, D. W., 209Bissonnette, V., 202
Bhar, S. S., 58Bhatia, A., 83
Blankenship, V., 184
Blanton, H., 213, 423
Blatt, S. J., 76, 142Blehar, M. C., 140Bleidorn, W., 150
Bleske, A. L., 236
Bless, H., 292Block, J., 9, 80, 163, 298
Block, J. H., 9
Bloise, S. M., 441
Bodenlos, J. S., 406Bodner, E., 400Boldizar, J. P., 389
Boldry, J., 145
Bolduc, D., 377Bonacci, A. M., 136Bonanno, G. A., 364
Bond, M., 79
Bono, J. E., 405Booth, R. J., 316Booth-Kewley, S., 165
Borkenau, P., 255
Borod, J. C., 314Bossio, L. M., 215Bouas, H. K., 193
Bouchard, T. J., 252
Boudreaux, E. D., 406Bourbeau, L. S., 142
Bouvrette, A., 329
Bovbjerg, D. H., 360
Bower, E., 201Bowers, S. P., 381Bowker, A., 242
Bowlby, J., 140
Bowman, P. J., 118Boyatzis, R. E., 187Bradbury, T. N., 146, 384
Bragg, M. E., 319
Branch, O., 394Brantley, P. J., 406Brashears, M. E., 317
Brass, D. J., 136
Braverman, J., 270Brebner, J., 262Brenman, M., 87, 89
Brennan, K. A., 142, 144
Bridges, N. A., 214, 309Briggs, S. R., 163 –164, 202
Brilliant, M. A., 78
Brissette, I., 206, 215
Britt, T. W., 168 –169
Brockner, J., 325 –326
Brodkin, J. D., 400
Brody, J. E., 50
Brody, L. R., 78Brook, A. T., 328
Brook, J. S., 391
Brooks, K. P., 206
Brooks-Gunn, J., 382Brower, A. M., 217Brown, J., 398
Brown, J. D., 212, 325, 327Brown, M., 62
Brown, R. J., 73
Brown, S., 390
Bruch, M. A., 201, 320Brunhart, S. M., 216Brunner, R., 233
Brunstein, J. C., 185
Bryant, C. M., 161Bryant, J., 83
Buchholz, E. S., 335
Buckley, M. E., 297
Buhler, C., 318Buigas, R. A., 175Buijsse, B., 216
Buis, T., 175
Bulkeley, K., 74Bullock, W. A., 226Bunde, J., 196
Burge, D., 146
Burger, J. M., 172, 270, 335, 337,
400, 404
Burgess,
C. A., 91
Burke, B. W., 163
Burleson, M. H., 320Burns, J. W., 364Burns, L. R., 205
Burr, R., 165
Busch, A. M., 424Bush, A. L., 330
Bushman, B. J., 136 –138, 391 –392,
393 –394, 437
Buss, A. H., 200 –201, 230
Buss, D. M., 236 –238, 265 –267,
269 –270
Bussey, K., 378
Bustamante, V., 74Butcher, J. N., 175Butera, F., 192
Butler, A. C., 425
Butler, J. M., 299Butler, S., 441Buunk, B. P., 213
Byrne, B. M., 382
Byrne, D., 128
Caccioppo, J. T., 196, 205, 319 –321
Cain, D. J., 296, 301, 307
Cairns, R. B., 7Caldwell, D. F., 172Calkins, S. D., 233
Calverley, D. S., 92
Camara, W. J., 62, 173Cameron, C. L., 128Cameron, J., 371
Cameron, L. D., 316
Cameron, R., 425Campbell, J. B., 261Campbell, J. D., 212, 326, 328
Campbell, L., 145
Campbell, S. M., 384Campbell, V. L., 62, 175Campos, J. J., 142
Canevello, A., 330504 NAME INDEX |
Canli, T., 226, 260
Cantor, J. R., 83
Cantor, N., 217
Caperchione, C., 320Cappella, E., 404Caprara, G. V., 364
Card, N. A., 439
Carey, B., 50Carey, G., 7, 206, 252
Carlozzi, B. L., 315
Carlson, M., 135, 438
Carnagey, N. L., 394, 438Carnelley, K. B., 146Carnevale, P. J. D., 382
Carosella, G. M., 86
Carr, D. E., 82Carroll, J. M., 205Carter, S. E., 217
Carter-Saltzman, L., 257
Carvajal, S. C., 216Carver, C. S., 129, 142, 185, 205,
212 –215, 316
Caspi, A., 170, 206, 230 –232, 255 –256
Catalano, R., 132Catanzaro, S. J., 320Cattell, H. E. P., 159
Cattell, R. B., 160
Cavallero, C., 71Cepeda-Benito, A., 316
Cervone, D., 188
Chachere, J. G., 391
Chadha, S., 80Chaikin, A. L., 311 –312
Chalmers, A., 213
Chance, J. E., 351
Chang, E. C., 213 –214, 217
Chao,G. T., 171Chaplin, W. F., 80
Chapman, J. E., 425
Charles, S. T., 205Chase, S. K., 330Chason, S., 312
Chastain, R. L., 316
Chatard, S., 443Chavanon, M., 246Cheek, J. M., 200 –201
Chelune, G. J., 324Chen, H., 379Cheng, C., 130, 381Cherry, M., 78
Chess, S., 239 –242
Cheung, M., 130
Chia, R., 405Chipperfield, J. G., 399
Chipuer, H. M., 255
Chitsabesan, P., 429Choi, D-W, 332
Choi, I., 332
Christensen, A., 368
Christensen, K., 9, 252Christensen, P. N., 321Church, A. T., 10, 163
Churchill, S. L., 242Clair, S. D., 216
Clancy, S. M., 444
Clark, C. L., 142
Clark, D. A., 445 –447, 449
Clark, L. A., 165, 168, 204 –206,
230, 425
Clark, M. S., 312Clark, R. A., 184Clearfield, M. W., 377
Cleveland, H. H., 101
Clore, G. L., 210
Coccaro, E. F., 7Coe, W. C., 88 –89, 91
Cohen, D. B., 74
Cohen, L. H., 159, 381
Cohen, P., 379, 391Cohen, S., 197, 206 –207, 263, 320
Cohn, N. B., 310
Colder, C. R., 206, 315
Colder, M., 315Cole, S. G., 202Coleman, M., 384
Collins, A., 144
Collins, N. L., 144 –146, 310
Comer, R., 195Compas, B. E., 129
Comstock, G., 389, 391
Conger, R. D., 161Conley, J. J., 227
Constantinople, A., 379
Conti, D., 50
Conti, R., 293Contrada, R. J., 197Conway, F., 215
Conway, M. A., 119
Cook, A., 330Cook, O., 319Cook, S. W., 128
Coolidge, C., 82
Coolidge, F. L., 80Cooper, C., 262Cooper, H. M., 241, 262, 403 –404
Cooper, M. L., 130, 145, 329Cooper, S. M., 242Cooper, S. R., 177Cooper, T., 195
Coopman, S. J., 429
Coplan, R. J., 240, 242Corbett, S., 429Cordeiro, A. M., 422
Cordova, J. V., 212
Corley, R., 256Cornell, D. G., 248Cornelius, J. S., 266
Cornette, M. M., 424
Cosby, M., 270Cossette, L., 377
Costa, P. T., 129, 159 –160, 163, 177,
196, 205, 255, 270
Costello, C. G., 449
Council, J. R., 91
Couper,
D. J., 196
Cournoyer, R. J., 78Cowan, C. P., 163Cowan, P. A., 163
Cowen, E. I., 240
Cowles, A., 408
Cox-Fuenzalida, L., 229, 262Crabbe, J., 258
Crabbe, J. C., 242
Craig, J., 384Craighead, W. E., 449Craik, K. H., 15
Cramer, K. M., 319, 337
Cramer, P., 76, 78, 80Cramer, R. E., 266, 269Crandall, R., 288
Crane, E. C., 212
Crane, F. G., 212Crawford, E., 320Crawford, T., 379
Creighton, A., 92
Crick, F., 75Crick, N. R., 436, 439Crisp, R. J., 444
Crocker, J., 328 –330
Cromwell, R. L., 448
Cronkite, R. C., 129Cropanzano, R. S., 2090
Cross, S. E., 442, 444
Crossen, C., 334Crost, N. W., 246Crowson, J. J., 448
Csikszentmihalyi, I. S., 291
Csikszentmihalyi, M., 291 –293, 334
Cunningham, J. A., 312
Cunningham, J. D., 381
Cunningham, M. R., 206
Curran, J. P., 203Cury, F., 191Cutler, S. E., 209
Cutmore, T. R. H., 360
Cutrona, C. E., 318 –319
Cyranowski, J. M., 419
Dadds, M. R., 360
Da Fonseca, D., 191Daniels, D., 259Daniels, J., 292
Danoff-Burg, S., 386
Dalsky, D. J., 264Danoff-Burg, S., 128Darnon, C., 192
David, D., 74
Davidson, J., 73Davidson, K., 76, 80, 84, 297
Davidson, K. W., 197
Davidson, R. J., 244 –246
Davila, J., 146
Davis, J. D., 310Davis, K. E., 144
Davis, K. L., 423
Davis, P. J., 442Davis, R., 344Davis, S. F., 344
Davison, J., 195NAME INDEX 505 |
Dawes, R. M., 62
Day, N. E., 399
Deary, I. J., 406
Deaux, K., 382De Boeck, P., 184de Castro, B. O., 394
Deckers, L., 82
Deffenbacher, J., 197, 425DeFranc, W., 78
DeFries, J. C., 252, 256
de Kort, Y. A. W., 337
del Valle, C. H. C., 217Demakis, G. J., 175Dembroski, T. M., 196
DeNeve, K. M., 262, 403
Denson, T. F., 136DePaulo, B. M., 201 –202
De Pelsmacker, P., 209
Depner, C., 187
Depue, R. A., 404, 449de Rivera, J., 50Derlega, V. J., 310 –312
Derr, W. R., 326Derry, P. A., 446de Sales French, R., 320De Schipper, J. C., 242
Desjardins, S., 73
de St. Aubin, E., 118DeSteno, D., 272
Detre, T., 195
Deutsch, C. L., 382
De Vellis, B. M., 398De Vellis, R. F., 398de Vries, M. W., 449
DeWolfe, D. J., 130
Diamond, A., 118Dickens, W. T., 253Di Dio, L., 384
Diefenbach, M., 206
Diehl, M., 142Diener, E., 11, 205, 209 –210, 227,
263 –264, 332 –333, 440 –441
Diener, M., 333Digman, J., 159, 163DiLalla, L. F., 252Dill, K. E., 394 –395
DiMatteo, M. R., 210Dindia, K., 312Dion, K. K., 144Dion, K. L., 144
Di Paula, A., 328
Dobson, K. S., 449Docter, R., 450Dodge, K. A., 129, 436, 438 –439
Dodgson, P. G., 327Dollard, J., 131Dollinger, S. J., 405, 444
Domhoff, B., 71, 75
Dompnier, B., 192
Donderi, D. C., 73Donnellan, M. B., 161Donnelly, D. A., 316
Donnerstein, E., 389 –391Donovan, J. J., 171
Doob, A. N., 131 –132, 135
Dooley, D., 132
Doucet, C., 229Douvan, E., 187Doyle, W. J., 320
Dozois, D. J. A., 449
Draguns, J. G., 11Dubow, E. F., 391
Dubreuil, D. L., 89
du Chene, T., 91
Dufour, C. L., 404Duke, T., 73Dull, W. R., 201
Dunbar, E., 50
Duncan, J. H., 203Duncan, K. E., 117Dunn, T. W., 425
Durante, K. M., 266
Durbin, C. E., 297Dutton, K. A., 325, 327Dwivedi, Y., 400
Dwyer, J., 215
Dwyer, K. A., 207Dyce, J. A., 165Dyrenforth, P. S., 262
Eagly, A. H., 247, 270
Earleywine, M., 136
Eastwick, P. W., 270
Eaves, L., 259
Eby, L. T., 403Eccles, J., 187Eder, D., 81
Edge, K., 128
Eggleston, T., 213Egloff, B., 205Eisenberg, J. G., 391
Eisenberger, R., 371
Eisenbud, L., 377Eisler, R. M., 195Ekman, P., 244
Eley, T. C., 450
Elliot, A. J., 185, 191, 193Elliott, A. J., 365, 432Elliott, R., 296
Ellis, A., 425 –426
Ellman, S. J., 74
Elnick, A. B., 142Else-Quest, N. M., 231
Emde, R. N., 248
Emerson, T., 201Emmons, R. A., 205, 209, 212, 264Endler, N. S., 166
Epstein, J. A., 202
Epstein, S., 167 –168
Erazo-Caicedo, M. I., 429
Erez, A., 405
Ericksen, A. J., 163
Erickson, M. H., 87Erikson, E. H., 105 –106, 111
Ernst, J. M., 319
Eron, L. D., 7, 132, 391Escobar, O., 386
Espindle, D., 419
Etaugh, C., 377
Eubanks, J., 394, 438Evans, D. E., 230Evans, F. B., 62
Evans, J., 450
Evans, R. I., 216, 355
Evans,
S., 377
Evans, W. P., 404
Evon, D., 364
Ey, S., 129
Eysenck, H. J., 164, 224 –229, 259
Eysenck, S. B. G., 224, 227 –228
Fairey, P. J., 326
Fancher, R. E., 43Fanning, J. R., 7
Farabee, D. J., 202
Farber, B. A., 308 –309, 312
Farley, F., 228Fassler, O., 89, 92
Fearn, M., 246
Feeney, J. A., 142, 145 –146
Feinglos, M. N., 206Feingold, A., 269
Feist, G. J., 161
Feixas, G., 429Feldman Barrett, L., 310, 441Fenichel, O., 79
Ferguson, S. M., 400
Fernengel, K., 196Fiese, B. H., 422Finch, J. F., 270
Fincham, F. D., 384
Findley, M. J., 404Finkel, D., 145, 258
Finkel, E. J., 270
Finkelstein, L. E., 315
Fins, A., 74Finzi-Dottan, R., 312Fioravanti, M., 299
Fisch, M., 382
Fischer, A. H., 210Fischer, A. R., 11Fischer, P., 438
Fischer, R., 142, 213
Fischetti, M., 203Fisher, M., 40Fivush, R., 441
Flanagan, M., 394
Fletcher, B., 129Fletcher, G. J. O., 269Flinn, J. A., 379
Floderus-Myrhed, B., 259
Flora, J., 322Flory, J. D., 197Flynn, J. R., 253
Foa, E. B., 129
Fodor, E. M., 185Folkman, S., 128 –129
Fonagy, P., 58
Fontaine, R. G., 436, 438506 NAME INDEX |
Ford, M. B., 146
Ford, R. Q., 76
Forman, E. M., 425
Foulkes, D., 71Fox, N. A., 233, 245Fraley, R. C., 146, 450
Frankel, A., 322
Frankel, B. S., 325Fransella, F., 411, 428
Frattaroli, J., 314 –316
Frazier, L. D., 422Frazier, P. A., 272Fredickson, B. L., 329Freemon, J. A., 320
Freidrich-Cofer, L., 391
Frensch, K. M., 118Fresco, D. M., 450Freud, A., 79
Freud, S., 47, 53 –55, 57 –58, 81, 91, 115
Friberg, L., 260
Friedman, L. C., 214Friedman, H. S., 162, 210, 212, 216
Friedman, M., 193
Friesen, M., 269Friesen, W. V., 244Frist, W. H., 212
Fritz, H. L., 320, 384 –385
Froming, W. J., 419
Fromm, E., 117
Fry, G., 92
Fujita, F., 263, 441
Fulker, D. W., 256, 259Funder, D. C., 166, 170
Gabbard, G. O., 58
Gabriel, S., 443Gabrielidis, C., 266Gacono, C. B., 62
Gaeddert, W. P., 188
Gagne, M., 128Gale, C. R., 406Gallagher, F. M., 327
Gallagher, K. C., 240
Gallo, L. C., 196Gangestad, S. W., 248Ganiban, J. M., 231
Ganong, L. H., 384
Gao, D., 163Garb, H. N., 62 –63
Garcia, J., 371
Garcia, S., 202
Gardner, W. L., 443Gaschke, Y. N., 205Gasper, K., 217
Gastorf, J. W., 194
Gatz, M., 205Gaudiano, B. A., 425
Geary, D. C., 265
Gee, C. B., 212
Geen, R. G., 137, 262, 391 –392
Geers, A. L., 213Geleijnse, K. M., 216
Gemar, M. C., 449Gendlin, E. T., 280
Gentile, D. A., 395
George, W. H., 368
Geraerts, E., 50Gerrard, M., 213, 423Gershoff, E. T., 389
Gershuny, B. S., 129
Gerald, K., 196Gerstein, R. K., 450
Gersten, J. C., 391
Gersten, M., 239
Geshuri, Y., 387Gest, S. D., 233, 235Geuens, M., 209
Gibbons, F. X., 213, 423
Gibson, H. B., 228Gidron, Y., 197Giesler, R. B., 448
Gilbert, L., 382
Gill, M. M., 87, 89Gillespie, N. A., 258, 260Gilliland, K., 226, 229
Gillum, B., 9
Gillum, R., 9Gilroy, F. D., 384Giltay, E. J., 216
Gjerde, P. F., 9
Glasberg, R., 78Glass, C. R., 203
Glass, D. C., 194 –195, 206
Glazer, K., 196Gleser, G. C., 76Glisky, M. L., 91Gohm, C. L., 210, 264
Gol, A. R., 128
Goldberg, J. F., 450Goldberg, L. R., 159, 163 –164, 170 –171
Goldsmith, H. H., 142, 231
Gonzales-Roma, V., 205
Good, T. E., 241Gooding, R. Z., 171Goodwin, R., 319
Gorassini, D. R., 92
Gordon, A. H., 210Gore, J. S., 444Gorsuch, R. L., 164
Goswick, R. A., 320 –321
Gotay, C. C., 195
Gottesman, I. I., 252Gough, H. G., 299
Gouze, M., 9
Graef, R., 334Graham, J. W., 404Graham, S. M., 312
Granberg, E., 423
Gray, E. K., 163Gray, J. A., 226
Graziano, W. G., 161 –162, 176,
231, 270
Green, B. L., 382Green, J. P., 88, 91Green, L. R., 319
Greenberg, J. M., 201Greenberg, R., 74
Greene, K., 310
Greenier, K. D.,
Greening, L., 450Greitemeyer, T., 438Greve, W., 327
Grich, J., 145 –146
Grigorenko, E. L., 253
Grimm, L. G., 195
Grob, A., 227
Gronnerod, C., 62
Gross, J. J., 210
Grotevant, H. D., 297
Grothe,
K. B., 406
Grove, J. R., 189
Gruber, C., 233Gruzelier, J. H., 91Guerra, N. G., 132, 389
Guichard, A. C., 146
Guimaraes, F. S., 400Guimond, S., 443
Gullone, E., 320
Gunderson, J. G., 58
Gunlicks, M., 145Gunthert, K. C., 159Gur, R. C., 246
Guyll, M., 197
Gwaltney, C. J., 364
Haaga, D. A. F., 206
Haan, B., 313
Haans, A., 337Habashi, M. M., 161Haeffel, C. J., 450
Haemmerlie, F. M., 203
Hagemann, D., 205, 246Hager, J. L., 371Hair, E. C., 176
Haig, J., 163
Hall, C., 405Hall, C. S., 70 –71
Hall, J. A., 380 –381
Hall, M. H., 277, 285
Hallam, J. R., 327Hallmark, R., 62, 175Hallquist, M. N., 88
Hamaker, S., 206
Hamamura, T., 331Hamer, R. J., 201Hammen, C., 146
Hammer, A. L., 213
Handler, L., 60Handley, G. W., 88Hankin, B. L., 450
Hanley-Dunn, P., 321
Hanrahan, S. J., 189Hansell, S., 206Hansen, J. S., 206
Hansen, R. D., 129
Hanson, R. K., 62Harackiewicz, J., 193, 371, 382Hardin, E. E., 424
Har-Even, D., 312NAME INDEX 507 |
Harmon-Jones, E., 246
Harris, C. R., 272
Harris, M. B., 134
Harris, R., 394Harrison, A. A., 264Harrist, A. W., 315
Harrist, S., 315
Hart, H. M., 118Harter, S. L., 429
Hartman, K. A., 408
Hartshorne, H., 166
Harvey, J. H., 312Harvey, R. J., 200Haselton, M. G., 266
Hatfield, E., 266
Haselton, M. G., 236Hasselbach, P., 406Havermans, R., 449
Hawkley, L. C., 329 –321
Hawks, E., 209
Hawley, C. W., 261,Hayden, E. P., 297
Haydon, K. C., 144
Haynes, S. G., 196Haynes, S. N., 366Hazan, C., 141 –142, 145
Headey, B., 263Heath, A. C., 259Heatherton, T. F., 325
Hebblethwaithe, S., 118
Heckhausen, J., 399
Hedlund, S., 449Heery, E. A., 201Heim, M., 319
Heimberg, R. G., 201
Heimpel, S. A., 326 –327
Heine, S. J., 213, 331 –332, 424
Heinrich, L. M., 320
Heinricks, N., 203
Helbraun, E., 335Helgeson, V. S., 126, 312, 320, 378,
381, 384 –386
Helm, B., 321Helmreich, R. L., 184, 379Helms, J. E., 175Hemenover, S. H., 316
Henderson, H. A., 233
Hendrick, S. S., 312Henriques, J. B., 246Herbst, J. H., 338
Herbst, K. C., 100
Heron, J., 450Herrnstein, R. J., 253Hervig, L. K., 165
Hessling, R. M., 314, 316, 423
Hewig, J., 246Hewitt, E. C., 89
Hibbard, S., 76
Higgins, C. A., 172
Higgins, E. T., 420, 423Higgins, R. L., 406Hilgard, E. R., 88, 91
Hill, C. E., 71, 309Hill, G. J., 203
Hilsenroth, M. J., 62
Hiroto, D. S., 397
Hobbs, S. A., 322Hockenbury, D., 322Hof, J., 196
Hoffman, L. W., 257
Hogan, M. E., 450Hogan, R., 171
Hojnoski, R. L., 62
Holcom, M. L., 202
Holcroft, C., 196Hollon, S. D., 425Holloway, R. A., 419
Holloway, S., 262
Holmberg, D., 444Holmes, A., 119Hoobler, J. M., 136
Hooker, K., 213, 422
Hoover, C. W., 201Hopkins, J. R., 105Hopkinson, P. J., 246
Horley, J., 429
Horn, J., 160Horn, J. M., 256Horney, K. D., 113 –114
Hornstein, G. A., 312Horowitz, L. M., 142, 320, 322Hough, R., 132
House, R. J., 187
Houshi, F., 73
Houston, B. K., 197Howard, M. L., 89Howell, D. C., 129
Hoyt, I. P., 91
Hoyt, M. F., 74Huang, J. Y., 312Hubbard, B., 162
Huesmann, L. R., 7, 132, 389, 391,
436, 438
Huff, L., 325
Huff, M., 197
Hui, C. H., 188
Hunsley, J., 62Hunt, J. M., 166Hurtz, G. M., 171
Huston, A. C., 391
Hutchinson, C., 213Hyde, J. S., 187, 231
Iacono, W., 258
Ialongo, N., 377Ickes, W., 145, 201 –202, 382 –383, 419
Ihilevich, D., 76
Ilardi, S. S., 449
Ilgen, M., M., 364Illingworth, K. S. S., 217
Ingram, R. E., 449
Inouye, D. K., 398
Inouye, J., 163
Jaccard, J., 423
Jackson, B., 197Jackson, C. J., 227
Jackson, D. N., 164, 171
Jackson, L. A., 377, 382
Jackson, S. A., 292Jacobs, D. R., 197Jaffe, K., 146
Jahoda, M., 63, 69
Jang, K. L., 255Janicki, D., 126
Janicki-Devers, D., 320
Janoff-Bulman, R., 316
Jarrett, R. B., 425Jefferson, T., 100Jeffery, R. W., 364
Jeffrey, A., 309
Jemmolo, S., 422Jenkins, C. D., 193, 195Jenkins, S. R., 187
Jensen, A. R., 253
Jensen, M. P., 86Jensen-Campbell, L. A., 161, 176,
231, 270
Jing, H., 331Joca,
S. R. L., 400
Johansson, B., 406
John, O. P., 159, 163, 210
Johnson, B. T., 197
Johnson, C., 334Johnson, E. A., 80Johnson, H. H., 195
Johnson, J. G., 391 –392
Johnson, K. A., 202
Johnson, L. M., 266Johnson, M. K., 441
Johnson, P. M., 422
Johnson, T. B., 318Johnson, W., 255, 258
Joiner, T. E., 320, 449
Joireman, J., 163
Jome, L. M., 11Jones, A., 288Jones, E., 40, 64
Jones, M., 320
Jones, R. E., 209Jones, S. C., 215Jones, W. H., 320 –322
Jorgensen, R. S., 197Jorgensen, S. J., 246Josephs, R. A., 444, 448Jourard, S. M., 308, 312
Judge, T. A., 171 –172, 405
Jung, C. G., 102 –104
Justice, L. M., 241
Kaflowitz, N. G., 320
Kagan, J., 232 –235
Kahn, J. H., 314, 316Kahn, M. L., 313
Kaiser, F. G., 337
Kalechstein, A. D., 404Kallio, S., 89Kamen-Siegel, L., 215
Kandler, C., 150508 NAME INDEX |
Kang, S-M., 331
Kaplan, A., 191 –193
Kaplan, G. D., 408
Kaprio, J., 259Karney, B. R., 146Karpinski, A., 330
Karraker, K. H., 377
Kasen, S., 279, 391Kashdan, T. B., 201
Kashy, D. A., 145, 321
Kasimatis, M., 263
Kasmer, J. A., 164Kasser, T., 7Kaster-Bundgaard, J., 365
Katon, W., 130
Katsanis, J., 88Katsura, H., 331Katz, I. M., 212
Kaus, C. R., 422
Kawabata, Y., 332Kawachi, I., 196Keefe, R. C., 266
Keelan, J. P. R., 144 –145
Keller, J., 292
Keller, J. W., 175Kelley, K., 240
Kelly, A. E., 316
Kelly, G. A., 411, 413, 431Kemp, A. H., 246
Kendler, K. S., 259
Kendzierski, D., 419
Kennedy, S. J., 449Kenny, C., 316Kenny, D. A., 201
Kenrick, D. T., 266, 270, 382
Kent, G., 359Keogh, B. K., 240 –242
Kern, M. L., 162
Kernis, M. H., 325
Kessler, R. C., 142, 259Ketelaar, T., 264Khaw, K-T., 399
Kidd, K. K., 253
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., 196, 320Kiefer, A. K., 330Kihlstrom, J. F., 87, 91
Kim, J-H., 447
Kim, S., 246Kimiecik, J. C., 292Kim-Prieto, C., 332
King, D. W., 129
King, L. A., 129, 212, 316Kingsley, D., 15Kirk, S. B., 128
Kirker, W. S., 421
Kirkpatrick, L. A., 144 –145
Kirsch, I., 86, 88 –89, 91
Kirsch, M., 171
Kirschenbaum, H., 280
Kitayama, S., 10 –11, 331 –332
Kivimaki, M., 213Klein, D. C., 401
Klein, D. N., 297Kleinke, C. L., 313
Kleinman, A., 11
Kleitman, N., 74
Klinesmith, J. 7Kling, K. C., 187Klinger, B. I., 92
Klohnen, E. C., 144
Klonowicz, T., 406Klusas, J. A., 316
Knapp, S., 309
Knee, C. R., 330
Knight, P. D., 202Knowles, E. S., 179Knox, J., 89, 92
Knox, S., 309
Kobasa, S. C., 407Koelling, R. A., 371Koester, N. H., 320, 324
Koestner, R., 384
Kohler, S. S., 171Kolodziej, M. E., 197Konecni, V. J., 135
Konold, T. R., 240
Kop, W. J., 320Korabik, K., 382Koriat, A., 126
Korn, J. H., 344
Koskenvuo, M., 259Kowalski, R. M., 202 –203
Kozak, M., 74
Kraft, M., 163
Krahe, B., 395Kram, M. L., 400Kramer, G. L., 400
Krantz, D. S., 196
Kring, A. M., 201, 209 –211
Krokoff, L. J., 84Kromhout, D., 216
Krueger, R. F., 255, 258
Kubzansky, L. D., 197Kuebli, J., 441Kuhlman, D. M., 163
Kuhlman, T. L., 81, 85
Kuiper, N. A., 84, 421, 446Kulick, J. A., 382Kulick, W., 81
Kulka, A., 187
Kuncel, N. R., 170, 177Kunda, Z., 418Kuppens, P. 332
Kurdek, L. A., 383
Kusulas, J. W., 165Kwon, P., 80
Labouvie-Vief, G., 142
LaFraniere, S., 177Lagace-Seguin, D. G., 240
LaGasse, L., 233
Lago, T., 319
Lahey, B. B., 159Lai, J. C. L., 214Laing, W. N., 326
Lake, M. A., 377Lake, R. P., 337
Lakin, J. L., 424
Lamb, M. E., 142
Lamke, L. K., 383Lamnin, A. D., 316Landau, S. F., 133
Landy, F. J., 171
Lane, J. D., 206Lane, R. D., 441
Langens, T. A., 316
Langer, E. J., 391, 398 –399
Langinvainio, H., 259
Langston, C. A., 217Larsen, J. T., 205
Larsen, R. J., 207, 209 –210,
263 –264
Larson, D. G., 316
Larson, R. W., 334 –337
Lassiter, G. D., 213
Lau, J. Y. F., 450
Lau, R. R., 408Lauder, W., 320Laurence, J-R., 91
Laurenceau, J-P, 310
Lavee, Y., 210, 212Lax, E., 293Lazarus, R. S., 126, 128 –129
Lazzari, R., 299Le, K., 262Leak, G. K., 83
Leary, M. R., 200 –203, 237, 338
LeBeau, L. S., 217Ledley, D. R., 201Lee, K., 163Lee, M., 337
Lee, Y-T., 213, 450
Leedham, B., 212Leen-Felder, E. W., 209Lefcourt, H. M., 84, 403
LeFevre, J., 292
Lefkowitz, E. S., 381Lefkowitz, M. M., 391Lehavor, K., 309
Lehman, D. R., 213, 332, 424
Lehman, J. M., 215
Leichsenring, F., 58LeMay, C. S., 202
Lenney, E., 379, 381
Lens, W., 184Lent, R. W., 364Leon, G. R., 9
Lepore, S. J., 315 –316, 381
Lerner, H., 76
Lerner, P., 76Lester, P., 83
Leszczynski, J. P., 379
Leue, A., 246Leventhal, E. A., 206Leventhal, H., 206
Levin, I., 321
Levin, R. A., 175Levine, A., 404Levy, B. R., 129, 315
Levy, K. N., 142NAME INDEX 509 |
Lewin, K., 411
Lewin, T., 177
Lewinsohn, P. M., 449 –450
Lewis, G., 450
Lewis, M., 118Lewis-Fernandez, R., 11
Li, M. P., 266
Liddy, S. J., 91Lilienfeld, S. O., 62 –63, 89
Lim, S-L., 447
Linde, J. A., 364
Linder, J. R., 395Lindsay, J. L., 134Linville, P. W., 190
Lips, H. M., 423
Lipsitt, L. P., 233Liss, M. B., 377Littig, L. W., 187
Little, T. D., 439
Liu, J., 163Liu, J. H., 142Livesley, W. J., 255
Lloyd, G. K., 400
Lockard, J. S., 178Loehlin, J. C., 255 –256
Long, B. C., 214
Long, C. R., 335, 338
Long, F. Y., 228Lopez, F. G., 364
Lopez, S. J., 301
Lord, C. G., 422
Lowell, E. L., 184Lozinski, R. H., 207Luben, R., 399
Lucas, R. E., 227, 262
Lueck, L., 246Luhtanen, R. K., 329 –330
Lumley, V., 365
Luria, Z., 377
Luthans, F., 364Lynch, P. J., 395Lynch, R., 197
Lyness, S. A., 195
Lynn, R., 227Lynn, S. J., 86, 88 –90, 92
Lytton, H., 256
MacCallum, R. C., 101
MacDonald, D. A., 164MacDonald, M. R., 446
MacGregor, M. W., 76, 80, 297
McAdams, D. P., 4, 117 –119
McAndrew, F. T., 7McCarthy, E. D., 391
McCaskill, C. C., 206
McCauley, Ch., 398McCauley, Cl., 82
McCeney, M. K., 196
McClearn, G. E., 260
McClelland, D. C., 184 –185, 187
McCloskey, M. S., 7McClure, E. B., 210
McConkey, K. M., 91McConnell, A. M., 424
McCrae, R. R., 100, 129, 159 –160,
163 –164, 176 –177, 205, 255, 270
McCrary, F., 338
McGhee, P. E., 85McGovern, A. R., 315
McGrath, M. J., 74
McGraw, A. P., 205McGregor, H. A., 191
McGue, M., 9, 252, 258
McGuire, C. V., 444
McGuire, M. T., 237McGuire, P. A., 84McGuire, W. J., 444
McInman, A., 189
McIntyre, C. W., 206McKellar, J., 364McKenzie, S. D., 84
McKillop, K. J., 315
McLean, K. C., 118McManus, J., 419McNalley, R. J., 50
McNaughton, N., 226
McPherson, M., 317Mackie, M., 444Maddux, J. E., 203
Madson, L., 442
Maehr, M. L., 191 –193
Magai, C., 215
Magnus, K., 263
Mahalik, J. R., 78
Mahoney, M. J., 367Maides, S., 407Maier, G. W., 185
Maier, M. A., 193
Maier, S. F., 395, 398, 400 –401
Main, K. J., 202Maiuro, R. D., 130
Major, B., 382
Malcuit, G., 377Mallon, S. D., 15Manis, M., 175
Mannell, R., 334
Manning-Ryan, B., 266Mansfield, E., 118 –119
Mansi, C., 422
Manstead, A. S. R., 210
Manwell, L. A., 326Marcotte, E., 73Marcus, D-K., 450
Marcus-Newhall, A., 135, 438
Margarett, J., 379Marien, M., 196Markey, C. N., 163
Markey, P. M., 163
Marks, G., 404Markus, H., 10 –11, 331 –332, 418 –420,
422 –423,
444
Marmot, M., 206
Maroto, J. J., 216Marsh, H. W., 381 –382
Marsh, S. C., 404
Marshall, G. N., 165Marshall, M. A., 212, 327
Marshall, T. C., 321
Martin, A., 362
Martin, C. L., 203, 377Martin, L. R., 216Martin, R., 197
Martin, R. A., 84
Martin, T., 227Martinez, J. C., 404Martinot, D., 444
Martyna, W., 381
Maruna, S., 118Marx, R., 320Masi, C.M., 321
Maslow, A. H., 279, 284, 286 –287,
290, 296, 334
Mateos, P. M., 217
Mathersul, d., 246
Matthews, K. A., 196 –197, 216
Matthews, W. J., 62
Matto, H. C., 60Maxwell, S. E., 321
May, M. A., 166
Mayer, J. D., 4, 205Mayman, M., 175Mayne, T. J., 58, 63, 296
Mead, A. D., 159
Meadows, S., 201
Meara, N. M., 423Measelle, J. R., 163
Mednick, M., 187
Meece, J. L., 193Meeker, W. B., 90Meichenbaum, D., 425
Meissner, W. W., 117
Meleshko, K. G. A., 203Melges, F. T., 404Melkman, R., 126
Mellman, T. A., 74
Menzaghi, F., 400Mershon, B., 164Meyer, B., 450
Meyer, G. J., 205
Meyerowitz, B. E., 212Michalski, R. L., 100Michela, J. L., 319
Mickelson, K. D., 142
Mikulincer, M., 145, 364, 400, 405Miles, D. R., 7
Miller, G. E., 248, 320
Miller, I. W., 449 –450
Miller, J. L., 175
Miller, L. C., 310Miller, M. G., 270
Miller, N., 135 –137, 438
Miller, N. E., 131 –132
Miller-Herringer, T., 210
Milling, L. S., 86
Mills, D. E., 84
Miltenberger, R. G., 365Min, K-H., 321Mineka, S., 269, 359
Miner, K. N., 316510 NAME INDEX |
Subsets and Splits